Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T15:36:38.532Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Subclinical levels of anxiety but not depression are associated with planning performance in a large population-based sample

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 September 2017

J. M. Unterrainer*
Affiliation:
Medical Psychology and Medical Sociology, Faculty of Medicine, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany
K. Domschke
Affiliation:
Department for Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Medical Center, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany
B. Rahm
Affiliation:
Medical Psychology and Medical Sociology, Faculty of Medicine, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany
J. Wiltink
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy, University Medical Center Mainz, Mainz, Germany
A. Schulz
Affiliation:
Preventive Cardiology and Preventive Medicine/Center for Cardiology, University Medical Center Mainz, Mainz, Germany
N. Pfeiffer
Affiliation:
Department of Ophthalmology, University Medical Center Mainz, Mainz, Germany
K. J. Lackner
Affiliation:
Institute of Clinical Chemistry and Laboratory Medicine, University Medical Center Mainz, Mainz, Germany
T. Münzel
Affiliation:
Center for Cardiology I, University Medical Center Mainz, Mainz, Germany
P. S. Wild
Affiliation:
Preventive Cardiology and Preventive Medicine/Center for Cardiology, University Medical Center Mainz, Mainz, Germany
M. Beutel
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy, University Medical Center Mainz, Mainz, Germany
*
*Address for correspondence: J. M. Unterrainer, Medical Psychology and Medical Sociology, Faculty of Medicine, University of Freiburg, Rheinstraße 12, 79104 Freiburg, Germany. (Email: [email protected])

Abstract

Background

Major depression and anxiety disorders are known to negatively influence cognitive performance. Moreover, there is evidence for greater cognitive decline in older adults with generalized anxiety disorder. Except for clinical studies, complex executive planning functions and subclinical levels of anxiety have not been examined in a population-based sample with a broad age range.

Methods

Planning performance was assessed using the Tower of London task in a population-based sample of 4240 participants aged 40–80 years from the Gutenberg Health Study (GHS) and related to self-reported anxiety and depression by means of multiple linear regression analysis.

Results

Higher anxiety ratings were associated with lower planning performance (β = −0.20; p < 0.0001) independent of age (β = 0.03; p = 0.47). When directly comparing the predictive value of depression and anxiety on cognition, only anxiety attained significance (β = −0.19; p = 0.0047), whereas depression did not (β = −0.01; p = 0.71).

Conclusions

Subclinical levels of anxiety but not of depression showed negative associations with cognitive functioning independent of age. Our results demonstrate that associations observed in clinical groups might differ from those in population-based samples, also with regard to the trajectory across the life span. Further studies are needed to uncover causal interrelations of anxiety and cognition, which have been proposed in the literature, in order to develop interventions aimed at reducing this negative affective state and to improve executive functioning.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th edn. American Psychiatric Publishing: Arlington, VA.Google Scholar
Andreescu, C, Varon, D (2015). New research on anxiety disorders in the elderly and an update on evidence-based treatments. Current Psychiatry Reports 17, 53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beaudreau, SA, MacKay-Brandt, A, Reynolds, J (2013). Application of a cognitive neuroscience perspective of cognitive control to late-life anxiety. Journal of Anxiety Disorders 27, 559566.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byers, AL, Yaffe, K, Covinsky, KE, Friedman, MB, Bruce, ML (2010). High occurrence of mood and anxiety disorders among older adults: The National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry 67, 489496.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D'Antuono, G, La Torre, FR, Marin, D, Antonucci, G, Piccardi, L, Guariglia, C (2016). Role of working memory, inhibition, and fluid intelligence in the performance of the Tower of London task. Applied Neuropsychology: Adult 20, 111.Google Scholar
Deptula, D, Singh, R, Pomara, N (1993). Aging, emotional states, and memory. American Journal of Psychiatry 150, 429434.Google ScholarPubMed
Donker, T, van Straten, A, Marks, I, Cuijpers, P (2011). Quick and easy self-rating of generalized anxiety disorder: validity of the Dutch web-based GAD-7, GAD-2 and GAD-SI. Psychiatry Research 188, 5864.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eysenck, MW, Derakshan, N, Santos, R, Calvo, MG (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: attentional control theory. Emotion 7, 336353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geiger, M, Domschke, K, Homola, G, Schulz, S, Nowak, J, Akhrif, A, Pauli, P, Romanos, M, Deckert, J, Neufang, S (2016a). ADORA2A genotype modulates interoceptive and exteroceptive processing in a fronto-insular network. European Neuropsychopharmacology 26, 12741285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geiger, M, Domschke, K, Ipser, J, Hattingh, C, Baldwin, D, Lochner, C, Stein, DJ (2016b). Altered executive control network resting-state connectivity in social anxiety disorder. World Journal of Biological Psychiatry 17, 4757.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goel, V (2002). Planning: neural and psychological. In Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science (ed. Nadel, L.), pp. 697703. Nature Publishing Group: London.Google Scholar
Hilbert, K, Lueken, U, Beesdo-Baum, K (2014). Neural structures, functioning and connectivity in Generalized Anxiety Disorder and interaction with neuroendocrine systems: a systematic review. Journal of Affective Disorder 158, 114126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jenkins, CD, Stanton, BA, Niemcryk, SJ, Rose, RM (1988). A scale for the estimation of sleep problems in clinical research. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 41, 313321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaller, CP, Debelak, R, Köstering, L, Egle, J, Rahm, B, Wild, PS, Blettner, M, Beutel, ME, Unterrainer, JM (2016). Assessing planning ability across the adult life span: population-representative and age-adjusted reliability estimates for the Tower of London (TOL-F). Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 31, 148164.Google Scholar
Kaller, CP, Unterrainer, JM, Kaiser, S, Weisbrod, M, Aschenbrenner, S (2012a). Tower of London – Freiburger Version. Schuhfried: Mödling.Google Scholar
Kaller, CP, Unterrainer, JM, Stahl, C (2012b). Assessing planning ability with the Tower of London task: psychometric properties of a structurally balanced problem set. Psychological Assessment 24, 4653.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kertz, S, Bigda-Peyton, J, Bjorgvinsson, T (2013). Validity of the Generalized Anxiety Disorder-7 scale in an acute psychiatric sample. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy 20, 456464.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kroenke, K, Spitzer, RL, Williams, JB, Monahan, PO, Löwe, B (2007). Anxiety disorders in primary care: prevalence, impairment, comorbidity, and detection. Annals of Internal Medicine 146, 317325.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lampert, T, Kroll, LE (2006). Messung des sozioökonomischen Status in sozialepidemiologischen Studien. In: Gesundheitliche Ungleichheit – Theorien, Konzepte und Methoden (ed. Richter, M. Hurrelmann, K.), pp. 297319. VS Verlag für Sozialwissenschaften: Wiesbaden.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Löwe, B, Spitzer, RL, Grafe, K, Kroenke, K, Quenter, A, Zipfel, S, Buchholz, C, Witte, S, Herzog, W (2004). Comparative validity of three screening questionnaires for DSM-IV depressive disorders and physicians’ diagnoses. Journal of Affective Disorders 78, 131140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Löwe, B, Wahl, I, Rose, M, Spitzer, C, Glaesmer, H, Wingenfeld, K, Schneider, A, Brähler, E (2010). A 4-item measure of depression and anxiety: validation and standardization of the Patient Health Questionnaire-4 (PHQ-4) in the general population. Journal of Affective Disorders 122, 8695.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mah, L, Binns, MA, Steffens, DC (2015). Alzheimer's disease neuroimaging initiative. Anxiety symptoms in amnestic mild cognitive impairment are associated with medial temporal atrophy and predict conversion to Alzheimer disease. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 23, 466476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mah, L, Szabuniewicz, C, Fiocco, AJ (2016). Can anxiety damage the brain? Current Opinion in Psychiatry 29, 5663.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neufang, S, Geiger, M, Homola, G, Mahr, M, Akhrif, A, Nowak, J, Reif, A, Romanos, M, Solymosi, L, Deckert, J, Domschke, K (2015). Modulation of prefrontal functioning in attention systems by NPSR1 gene variation. NeuroImage 14, 199206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Potvin, O, Forget, H, Grenier, S, Préville, M, Hudon, C (2011). Anxiety, depression, and 1-year incident cognitive impairment in community-dwelling older adults. Journal of the American Geriatric Society 59, 14211428.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rock, PL, Roiser, JP, Riedel, WJ, Blackwell, AD (2014). Cognitive impairment in depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine 44, 20292040.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, LA, Brown, TA (2017). Psychometric properties of the generalized anxiety disorder scale-7 (GAD-7) in outpatients with anxiety and mood disorders. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment 39, 140146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shallice, T (1982). Specific impairments of planning. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences 298, 199209.Google ScholarPubMed
Snyder, H (2013). Major depressive disorder is associated with broad impairments on neuropsychological measures of executive function: a meta-analysis and review. Psychological Bulletin 139, 81132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Unterrainer, JM, Owen, AM (2006). Planning and problem solving: from neuropsychology to functional neuroimaging. Journal of Physiology-Paris 99, 308317.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Unterrainer, JM, Rahm, B, Kaller, CP, Leonhart, R, Quiske, K, Hoppe-Seyler, K, Meier, C, Müller, C, Halsband, U (2004). Planning abilities and the Tower of London: is this task measuring a discrete cognitive function? Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 26, 846856.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wetherell, JL, Reynolds, CA, Gatz, M, Pedersen, NL (2002). Anxiety, cognitive performance, and cognitive decline in normal aging. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 57, 246255.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wild, PS, Zeller, T, Beutel, M, Blettner, M, Dugi, KA, Lackner, KJ, Pfeiffer, N, Münzel, T, Blankenberg, S (2012). [The Gutenberg Health Study]. Bundesgesundheitsblatt, Gesundheitsforschung, Gesundheitsschutz 55, 824829.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, MW, Kueider, AM, Dmitrieva, NO, Manly, JJ, Pieper, CF, Verney, SP, Gibbons, LE (2016). Anxiety symptoms bias memory assessment in older adults. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 32, 983990.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiltink, J, Beutel, ME, Till, Y, Ojeda, FM, Wild, PS, Münzel, T, Blankenberg, S, Michal, M (2011). Prevalence of distress, comorbid conditions and well being in the general population. Journal of Affective Disorder 130, 429437.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, X, Norton, J, Carrière, I, Ritchie, K, Chaudieu, I, Ancelin, ML (2015). Generalized anxiety in community-dwelling elderly: prevalence and clinical characteristics. Journal of Affective Disorder 1, 2429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar