Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dlnhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T12:58:31.442Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Neural correlates of the abolished self-referential memory effect in schizophrenia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 May 2013

Y. Zhao
Affiliation:
Center for Psychiatric Research, Beijing Huilongguan Hospital, China
D. Zhang
Affiliation:
Institute of Affective and Social Neuroscience, Shenzhen University, China State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning, Beijing Normal University, China
S. Tan*
Affiliation:
Center for Psychiatric Research, Beijing Huilongguan Hospital, China
C. Song
Affiliation:
Center for Psychiatric Research, Beijing Huilongguan Hospital, China
J. Cui
Affiliation:
Center for Psychiatric Research, Beijing Huilongguan Hospital, China
F. Fan
Affiliation:
Center for Psychiatric Research, Beijing Huilongguan Hospital, China
X. Zhu
Affiliation:
Center for Psychiatric Research, Beijing Huilongguan Hospital, China
Y. Zou
Affiliation:
Center for Psychiatric Research, Beijing Huilongguan Hospital, China
Y. Luo
Affiliation:
Institute of Affective and Social Neuroscience, Shenzhen University, China
*
*Address for correspondence: Professor S. Tan, Beijing Huilongguan Hospital, Beijing 100096China. (Email: [email protected])

Abstract

Background

The self-referential memory (SRM) effect refers to the phenomenon that stimuli processed with reference to the self are better remembered than those referenced to others. Studies have shown that schizophrenia patients do not have this memorial advantage for self-referenced information. The current study investigated the electrophysiological mechanism of the abolished SRM effect in schizophrenia.

Method

Twenty schizophrenia patients and 22 controls were recruited to complete an SRM task. We used a high-time resolution event-related potential (ERP) technique to analyze the electrophysiological differences between patients and controls during self- and other-reflection processing.

Results

Behavior data indicated that healthy controls had a typical SRM bias that was absent in the schizophrenia patients. ERP comparison between groups showed that the schizophrenia patients presented smaller voltages in both self- and other-reflection conditions in the 160–260 ms (P2 component) and 800–1200 ms (positive slow wave) time windows over the pre/frontal cortex. Furthermore, the N2 amplitudes (270–380 ms) differed between self- and other-reflection conditions in patients but not in normal controls. More importantly, we found that the P3 amplitudes in the parietal cortex correlated significantly with the SRM bias score in the patients (r = –0.688).

Conclusions

These results provide comprehensive and direct electrophysiological evidence for self- and other-reflective dysfunction in schizophrenia patients and contribute to our understanding of the underlying neural substrates of the abolished SRM effect in schizophrenia.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

These authors contributed equally to this work.

References

APA (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edn. American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Blackwood, NJ, Bentall, RP, Ffytche, DH, Simmons, A, Murray, RM, Howard, RJ (2004). Persecutory delusions and the determination of self-relevance: an fMRI investigation. Psychological Medicine 34, 591596.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, A, Weng, X, Yuan, J, Lei, X, Qiu, J, Yao, D, Li, H (2008). The temporal features of self-referential processing evoked by Chinese handwriting. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 20, 816827.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, J, Yuan, J, Feng, T, Chen, A, Gu, B, Li, H (2011). Temporal features of the degree effect in self-relevance: neural correlates. Biological Psychology 87, 290295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, J, Zhang, Y, Zhong, J, Hu, L, Li, H (2013). The primacy of the individual versus the collective self: evidence from an event-related potential study. Neuroscience Letters 535, 3034.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delorme, A, Makeig, S (2004). EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. Journal of Neuroscience Methods 134, 921.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
First, MB, Gibbon, M, Spitzer, RL, Williams, JBW (2002). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Non-Patient Edition (SCID-I/NP). Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute: New York.Google Scholar
First, MB, Spitzer, RL, Gibbon, M, Williams, JBW, Benjamin, L (1996). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders (SCID-II). Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute: New York.Google Scholar
Harvey, PO, Lee, J, Horan, WP, Ochsner, K, Green, MF (2011). Do patients with schizophrenia benefit from a self-referential memory bias? Schizophrenia Research 127, 171177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holt, DJ, Cassidy, BS, Andrews-Hanna, JR, Lee, SM, Coombs, G, Goff, DC, Gabrieli, JD, Moran, JM (2011). An anterior-to-posterior shift in midline cortical activity in schizophrenia during self-reflection. Biological Psychiatry 69, 415423.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hu, X, Wu, H, Fu, G (2011). Temporal course of executive control when lying about self- and other-referential information: an ERP study. Brain Research 1369, 149157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huang, Y, Luo, Y (2006). Temporal course of emotional negativity bias: an ERP study. Neuroscience Letters 398, 9196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karayanidis, F, Michie, PT (1996). Frontal processing negativity in a visual selective attention task. Electromyography and Clinical Neurophysiology 99, 3856.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kay, SR, Fiszbein, A, Opler, LA (1987). The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 13, 261276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelley, WM, Macrae, CN, Wyland, CL, Caglar, S, Inati, S, Heatherton, TF (2002). Finding the self? An event-related fMRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 14, 785794.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keyes, H, Brady, N, Reilly, RB, Foxe, JJ (2010). My face or yours? Event-related potential correlates of self-face processing. Brain and Cognition 72, 244254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lombardo, MV, Chakrabarti, B, Bullmore, ET, Sadek, SA, Pasco, G, Wheelwright, SJ, Suckling, J; MRC AIMS Consortium, Baron-Cohen, S (2010). Atypical neural self-representation in autism. Brain 133, 611624.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lou, H, Luber, B, Stanford, A, Lisanby, S (2010). Self-specific processing in the default network: a single-pulse TMS study. Experimental Brain Research 207, 2738.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luck, SJ (2005). An Introduction to the Event-Related Potential Technique. MIT Press: Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Lysaker, PH, Dimaggio, G, Carcione, A, Procacci, M, Buck, KD, Davis, LW, Nicolo, G (2010). Metacognition and schizophrenia: the capacity for self-reflectivity as a predictor for prospective assessments of work performance over six months. Schizophrenia Research 122, 124130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Macrae, CN, Moran, JM, Heatherton, TF, Banfield, JF, Kelley, WM (2004). Medial prefrontal activity predicts memory for self. Cerebral Cortex 14, 647654.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Modinos, G, Renken, R, Ormel, J, Aleman, A (2011). Self-reflection and the psychosis-prone brain: an fMRI study. Neuropsychology 25, 295305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mu, Y, Han, S (2010). Neural oscillations involved in self-referential processing. NeuroImage 53, 757768.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, ER, Brent, BK, Benton, M, Pruitt, P, Diwadkar, V, Rajarethinam, RP, Keshavan, MS (2010). Differential processing of metacognitive evaluation and the neural circuitry of the self and others in schizophrenia: a pilot study. Schizophrenia Research 116, 252258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Philippi, CL, Duff, MC, Denburg, NL, Tranel, D, Rudrauf, D (2012). Medial PFC damage abolishes the self-reference effect. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 24, 475481.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Picton, TW, Bentin, S, Berg, P, Donchin, E, Hillyard, SA, Johnson, R Jr., Miller, GA, Ritter, W, Ruchkin, DS, Rugg, MD, Taylor, MJ (2000). Guidelines for using human event-related potentials to study cognition: recording standards and publication criteria. Psychophysiology 37, 127152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, TB, Kuiper, NA, Kirker, WS (1977). Self-reference and the encoding of personal information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 35, 677688.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shad, MU, Brent, BK, Keshavan, MS (2011). Neurobiology of self-awareness deficits in schizophrenia: a hypothetical model. Asian Journal of Psychiatry 4, 248254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shad, MU, Keshavan, MS, Steinberg, JL, Mihalakos, P, Thomas, BP, Motes, MA, Soares, JC, Tamminga, CA (2012). Neurobiology of self-awareness in schizophrenia: an fMRI study. Schizophrenia Research 138, 113119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silva, JR, Torres, WM, Ortiz, MS (2008). Abnormal electrophysiological activation in schizophrenics during a personal traits attribution task. Biological Research 41, 143150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Staresina, BP, Gray, JC, Davachi, L (2009). Event congruency enhances episodic memory encoding through semantic elaboration and relational binding. Cerebral Cortex 19, 11981207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Su, YH, Chen, AT, Yin, HZ, Qiu, J, Lv, JY, Wei, DT, Tian, F, Tu, S, Wang, T (2010). Spatiotemporal cortical activation underlying self-referencial processing evoked by self-hand. Biological Psychology 85, 219225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Symons, CS, Johnson, BT (1997). The self-reference effect in memory: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin 121, 371394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tacikowski, P, Nowicka, A (2010). Allocation of attention to self-name and self-face: an ERP study. Biological Psychology 84, 318324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van der Meer, L, Costafreda, S, Aleman, A, David, AS (2010). Self-reflection and the brain: a theoretical review and meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies with implications for schizophrenia. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 34, 935–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, LA, Dritschel, B, Obonsawin, MC, Jentzsch, I (2007). Seeing yourself in a positive light: brain correlates of the self-positivity bias. Brain Research 1152, 106110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woods, SW (2003). Chlorpromazine equivalent doses for the newer atypical antipsychotics. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 64, 663667.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, GS, Wang, DF (1999). The Chinese personality dimensions. In Explorations of Chinese Personality (ed. Wang, D. F. and Cui, H.), pp. 371414. Social Sciences Academic Press: Beijing.Google Scholar
Zhang, D, Wang, L, Luo, Y, Luo, Y (2012). Individual differences in detecting rapidly presented fearful faces. PLoS ONE 7, e49517.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed