Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-s2hrs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-03T02:38:25.338Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Memory control in post-traumatic stress disorder: evidence from item method directed forgetting in civil war victims in Northern Uganda

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 October 2011

B. Zwissler
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Konstanz, Germany
A. Hauswald
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Konstanz, Germany
S. Koessler
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Konstanz, Germany
V. Ertl
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Konstanz, Germany Vivo – The Victim's Voice Non-Governmental Organization, Allensbach, Germany Department of Psychology, University of Bielefeld, Germany
A. Pfeiffer
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Konstanz, Germany Vivo – The Victim's Voice Non-Governmental Organization, Allensbach, Germany
C. Wöhrmann
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Konstanz, Germany
N. Winkler
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Konstanz, Germany Vivo – The Victim's Voice Non-Governmental Organization, Allensbach, Germany
J. Kissler*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Konstanz, Germany Department of Psychology, University of Bielefeld, Germany
*
*Address for correspondence: Prof.Dr J. Kissler, Department of Psychology, Pf 10 01 31, University of Bielefeld, 33501 Bielefeld, Germany. (Email: [email protected])

Abstract

Background

Traumatized individuals and particularly post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) patients are characterized by memory disturbances that suggest altered memory control. The present study investigated the issue using an item method, directed forgetting (DF) paradigm in 51 civil war victims in Uganda. All participants had been exposed to severe traumatic stress and 26 additionally suffered from PTSD.

Method

In an item cued, DF paradigm photographs were presented, each followed by an instruction to either remember or forget it. A recognition test for all initially presented photographs and thematically similar distracters followed. DF patterns were compared between the non-PTSD and the PTSD groups. Post-experimental ratings of picture valence and arousal were collected and correlated with DF.

Results

Results revealed DF, that is, reduced recognition for ‘to-be-forgotten’ items in the non-PTSD but not in the PTSD group. Moreover, in the non-PTSD, but not in the PTSD group, false alarms were reduced for ‘to-be-remembered’ items. Finally, DF was reduced in those participants who rated the pictures as more arousing, the PTSD group giving, on average, higher arousal ratings.

Conclusions

Data indicate that DF is reduced in PTSD and that the reduction is related to stimulus arousal. Furthermore, individuals with PTSD are characterized by a more global encoding style than individuals without PTSD, reflected in a higher false alarm rate. In sum, traumatized individuals with (but not without) PTSD are impaired in their ability to selectively control episodic memory encoding. This impairment may contribute to clinical features of the disorder such as intrusions and flashbacks.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adenauer, H, Pinösch, S, Catani, C, Gola, H, Keil, J, Kissler, J, Neuner, F (2010). Early processing of threat cues in posttraumatic stress disorder-evidence for a cortical vigilance-avoidance reaction. Biological Psychiatry 68, 451458.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, MC, Bjork, EL, Bjork, RA (1994). Remembering can cause forgetting: retrieval dynamics in long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition 20, 10631087.Google ScholarPubMed
Anderson, MC, Green, C (2001). Suppressing unwanted memories by executive control. Nature 410, 131134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aupperle, RL, Melrose, AJ, Stein, MB, Paulus, MP (2011). Executive function and PTSD: disengaging from trauma. Neuropharmacology. Published online: 22 February 2011. doi:10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.02.008.Google ScholarPubMed
Basden, BH, Basden, DR (1996). Directed forgetting: further comparisons of the item and list methods. Memory 4, 633653.Google Scholar
Basden, BH, Basden, DR (1998). Directed forgetting: a contrast of methods and interpretations. In Intentional Forgetting: Interdisciplinary Approaches (ed. Golding, J. M. and MacLeod, C. M.). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Mahwah, NJ.Google Scholar
Bremner, JD (2006). Traumatic stress: effects on the brain. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience 8, 445461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bremner, JD, Krause, K, Shobe, K, Kihlstrom, JF (2000). False memories in women with self-reported childhood sexual abuse: an empirical study. Psychological Science 11, 333337.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bremner, JD, Marmar, CR (1998). Trauma, Memory, and Dissociation. American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Clancy, S, Schacter, D, McNally, R, Pitman, RK (2000). False recognition in women reporting recovered memories of sexual abuse. Psychological Science 11, 2631.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corson, Y, Verrier, N (2007). Emotions and false memories: valence or arousal? Psychological Science 18, 208211.Google Scholar
Cottencin, O, Vaiva, G, Huron, C, Devos, P, Ducrocq, F, Jouvent, R, Goudemand, M, Thomas, P (2006). Directed forgetting in PTSD: a comparative study versus normal controls. Journal of Psychiatric Research 40, 7080.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dalgleish, T, Hauer, B, Kuyken, W (2008). The mental regulation of autobiographical re-collection in the aftermath of trauma. Current Directions in Psychological Science 17, 259263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DePrince, AP, Freyd, JJ (1999). Dissociation, attention and memory. Psychological Science 10, 449452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DePrince, AP, Freyd, JJ (2001). Memory and dissociative tendencies: the roles of attentional context and word meaning in a directed forgetting task. Journal of Trauma & Dissociation 2, 6782.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DePrince, AP, Freyd, JJ (2004). Forgetting trauma stimuli. Psychological Science 15, 488492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeQuervain, DJF, Kolassa, IT, Ertl, V, Onyut, PL, Neuner, F, Elbert, T, Papassotiropoulos, A (2007). A deletion variant of the α2b-adrenoreceptor is related to emotional memory in Europeans and Africans. Nature Neuroscience 10, 11371139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ehlers, A, Hackmann, A, Steil, R, Clohessy, S, Wenninger, K, Winter, H (2002). The nature of intrusive memories after trauma: the warning signal hypothesis. Behaviour Research and Therapy 40, 995–1002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ertl, V, Pfeiffer, A, Saile, R, Schauer, E, Elbert, T, Neuner, F (2010). Validation of a mental health assessment in an African conflict population. Psychological Assessment 22, 318324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fawcett, JM, Taylor, TL (2008). Forgetting is effortful: evidence from reaction time probes in an item-method directed forgetting task. Memory & Cognition 36, 11681181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foa, EB, Cashman, L, Jaycox, L, Perry, K (1997). The validation of a self-report measure of posttraumatic stress disorder: the Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale. Psychological Assessment 9, 445451.Google Scholar
Geraerts, E, McNally, RJ (2008). Forgetting unwanted memories: directed forgetting and thought suppression methods. Acta Psychologica (Amsterdam) 127, 614622.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilbertson, MW, Shenton, ME, Ciszewski, A, Kasai, K, Lasko, NB, Orr, SP, Pitman, RK (2002). Smaller hippocampal volume predicts pathologic vulnerability to psychological trauma. Nature Neuroscience 5, 12421247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilboa, A, Shalev, AY, Laor, L, Lester, H, Louzoun, Y, Chisin, R, Bonne, O (2004). Functional connectivity of the prefrontal cortex and the amygdala in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry 55, 263272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hauswald, A, Kissler, J (2008). Directed forgetting of complex pictures in an item method paradigm. Memory 16, 797809.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hauswald, A, Schulz, H, Iordanov, T, Kissler, J (2011). ERP dynamics underlying successful directed forgetting of neutral but not negative pictures. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 6, 450459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, JP, LaBar, KS, McCarthy, G, Selgrade, E, Nasser, J, Dolcos, F, VISN 6 Mid-Atlantic MIRECC Workgroup, Morey, RA (2011). Reduced hippocampal and amygdala activity predicts memory distortions for trauma reminders in combat-related PTSD. Journal of Psychiatric Research 45, 660669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karunakara, UK, Neuner, F, Schauer, M, Singh, K, Hill, K, Elbert, T, Burnham, G (2004). Traumatic events and symptoms of post-traumatic stress disorder amongst Sudanese nationals, refugees and Ugandans in the West-Nile. African Health Sciences 4(2), 8393.Google Scholar
McNally, RJ (2006). Cognitive abnormalities in post-traumatic stress disorder. Trends in Cognitive Science 10, 271277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNally, RJ, Metzger, LJ, Lasko, NB, Clancy, SA, Pitman, RK (1998). Directed forgetting of trauma cues in adult survivors of childhood sexual abuse with and without posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 107, 596601.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNally, RJ, Ristuccia, CS, Perlman, CA (2005). Forgetting of trauma cues in adults reporting continuous or recovered memories of childhood sexual abuse. Psychological Science 16, 336340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moulds, ML, Bryant, RA (2002). Directed forgetting in acute stress disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 111, 175179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neuner, F, Schauer, M, Klaschik, C, Karunakara, U, Elbert, T (2004). A comparison of narrative exposure therapy, supportive counseling, and psychoeducation for treating posttraumatic stress disorder in an african refugee settlement. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 72, 579587.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nowicka, A, Marchewka, A, Jednoróg, K, Tacikowski, P, Brechmann, A (2010). Forgetting of emotional information is hard: an fMRI study of directed forgetting. Cerebral Cortex. Published online: 27 June 2010. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhq117.Google ScholarPubMed
Onyut, LP, Neuner, F, Schauer, E, Ertl, V, Odenwald, M, Schauer, M, Elbert, T (2004). The Nakivale Camp Mental Health Project: building local competency for psychological assistance to traumatised refugees. Intervention: International Journal of Mental Health, Psychosocial Work & Counselling in Areas of Armed Conflict 2, 90–107.Google Scholar
Quinlan, CK, Taylor, TL, Fawcett, JM (2010). Directed forgetting: comparing pictures and words. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 64, 4146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paz-Caballero, MD, Menor, J, Jimenez, JM (2004). Predictive validity of event-related potentials (ERPs) in relation to the directed forgetting effects. Clinical Neurophysiology 115, 369377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rauch, SAM, Defever, E, Favorite, T, Duroe, A, Garrity, C, Martis, B, Liberzon, I (2009). Prolonged exposure for PTSD in a veterans health administration PTSD clinic. Journal of Traumatic Stress 22, 6064.Google Scholar
Roediger, HL III (1973). Inhibition in recall from cueing with recall targets. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal behavior 12, 644657.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schnyder, A, Treyer, V, Buck, A (2000). Selection of currently relevant memories by the human posterior medial orbitofrontal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience 20, 58805884.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shepard, RN (1967). Recognition memory for words, sentences and pictures. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 6, 156163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snodgrass, JG, Corwin, J (1988). Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: applications to dementia and amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 117, 3450.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, F, Cahill, L (2002). Propranolol for reemergent posttraumatic stress disorder following an event of retraumatization: a case study. Journal of Trauma and Stress 15, 433437.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiner, B, Reed, H (1969). Effects of the instructional sets to remember and to forget on short-term retention: studies of rehearsal control and retrieval inhibition (repression). Journal of Experimental Psychology 79, 226232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wylie, GR, Foxe, JJ, Taylor, TL (2008). Forgetting as an active process: an FMRI investigation of item-method-directed forgetting. Cerebral Cortex 18, 670682.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zoellner, LA, Foa, EB, Brigidi, BD, Przeworski, A (2000). Are trauma victims susceptible to ‘false memories’? Journal of Abnormal Psychology 109, 517524.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zoellner, LA, Sacks, MB, Foa, EB (2003). Directed forgetting following mood induction in chronic posttraumatic stress disorder patients. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 112, 508514.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zwissler, B, Koessler, S, Engler, H, Schedlowski, M, Kissler, J (2011). Acute psychosocial stress does not disrupt item-method directed forgetting, emotional stimulus content does. Neurobiology of Learning & Memory 95, 346354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed