Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-fbnjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T06:29:50.107Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Exploring ‘theory of mind’ in people with schizophrenia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 July 2009

Christopher D. Frith*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University College London and Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, Institute of Neurology, London
Rhiannon Corcoran
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University College London and Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, Institute of Neurology, London
*
1Address for correspondence: Professor Christopher D. Frith, Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, Institute of Neurology, 12 Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG.

Synopsis

Mentalizing ability was studied in 46 symptomatic schizophrenic patients and 44 non-symptomatic controls. Subjects heard six stories and simultaneously were shown simple cartoon pictures depicting the action sequencing occurring in the stories. All the stories involved false belief or deception, so that it was necessary to infer the mental states of the characters in order to understand their behaviour. After each story, subjects were asked one memory/reality question (concerning an event in the story) and one question that depended on the ability to infer the mental state of one of the characters. Patients with paranoid delusions were impaired on the questions concerning mental states. Patients with behavioural signs (i.e. negative features or incoherence) were also impaired on the mental state questions, but this difficulty was associated with memory impairments. Patients with symptoms of passivity (e.g. delusions of control) and patients in remission did not differ from normal controls. These results are consistent with the hypothesis that certain of the positive symptoms of schizophrenia reflect an impairment in the ability to infer the mental states of others.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1996

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Allen, H. A., Liddle, P. F. & Frith, C. D. (1993). Negative features, retrieval processes and verbal fluency in schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry 163, 769775.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ammons, R. B. & Ammons, C. H. (1962). The Quick Test. Psychological Test Specialists: Missoula, MT.Google Scholar
Arndt, S., Alliger, R. J. & Andreasen, N. C. (1991). The distinction of positive and negative symptoms. The failure of a two-dimensional model. British Journal of Psychiatry 158, 316322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baren-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H. & Cohen, D. J. (1993). Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from Autism. Oxford University Press: Oxford.Google Scholar
Bentall, R. P., Kaney, S. & Dewey, M. E. (1991). Paranoia and social reasoning: An attribution theory analysis. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 30, 1323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bilder, R. M., Mukherjee, S., Reider, R. O. & Bandurangi, A. A. K. (1985). Symptomatic and neuropsychology components of defect states. Schizophrenia Bulletin 11, 409419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corcoran, R., Frith, C. D. & Mercer, G. (1995). Schizophrenia, symptomatology and social inference: investigating ‘theory of mind’ in people with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 17, 513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cutting, J. & Murphy, D. (1990). Impaired ability of schizophrenics relatives to manics, or depressives, to appreciate social knowledge about their culture. British Journal of Psychiatry 157, 355358.Google Scholar
Diamond, M. D. (1956). The ability of schizophrenics to modify responses in an interpersonal situation. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 20, 441444.Google Scholar
Ebmeier, K. P., Blackwood, D. H. R., Murray, C., Souza, V., Walker, M., Dougall, N., Moffoot, P. R., O'Carroll, R. E. & Goodwin, G. M. (1993). Single-photon emission computed tomography with 99mTc-Exametazime in unmedicated schizophrenic patients. Biological Psychiatry 33, 487495.Google Scholar
Frith, C. D. (1992). The Cognitive Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; Hove.Google Scholar
Frith, C. D. (1994). Theory of mind in schizophrenia. In The Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia (ed. David, A. S. and Cutting, J. C. pp. 147162. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hove.Google Scholar
Frith, C. D. & Done, D. J. (1989). Experiences of alien control in schizophrenia reflect a disorder in the central monitoring of action. Psychological Medicine 19, 359363.Google Scholar
Frith, C. D., Leary, J., Cahill, C. & Johnstone, E. C. (1991). Performance on psychological tests. Demographic and clinical correlates of the results of these tests. British Journal of Psychiatry 159 (suppl. 13), 2629.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garety, P., Hemsley, D. & Wesseley, S. (1991). Reasoning in deluded schizophrenic and paranoid patients. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 179, 194201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Good, D. (1990). Repair and cooperation in conservation. In Computers and Conversation (ed. Luff, P., Gilbert, N. and Frohlich, D., pp. 133150. Academic Press: London.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Happé, F. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: understanding of story characters' thoughts and feelings by able autistics, mentally handicapped and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 24, 129154.Google Scholar
Happé, F. & Frith, U. (1994). Theory of mind in autism In Learning and Cognition in Autism (ed. Schopler, E. and Mesibov, G. B.), Plenum Press: New York.Google Scholar
Kaplan, R. D., Szechtman, H., Franco, S., Szechtman, B., Nahmias, C., Garnett, E. S., List, S. & Cleghorn, J. M. (1993). Three clinical syndromes of schizophrenia in untreated subjects: relation to brain glucose activity measured by positron emission tomography (PET). Schizophrenia Research 11, 4754.Google Scholar
La Russo, L. (1978). Sensitivity of paranoid patients to nonverbal cues. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 87, 463471.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretence and representation: the origins of ‘Theory of Mind’. Psychological Review 94, 412426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liddle, P. F. (1987). The symptoms of chronic schizophrenia: a re-examination of the positive–negative dichotomy. British Journal of Psychiatry 151, 145151.Google Scholar
Liddle, P. F. & Morris, D. L. (1991). Schizophrenic syndromes and frontal lobe performance. British Journal of Psychiatry 158, 340345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liddle, P. F., Friston, K. F., Frith, C. D., Hirsch, S. R., Jones, T. & Frackowiak, R. S. J. (1992). Patterns of cerebral blood flow in schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry 160, 179186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McKenna, P. J., Tamlyn, D., Lund, C. E., Mortimer, A. M., Hammond, S. & Baddeley, A. D. (1990). Amnesic syndrome in schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine 20, 967972.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mlakar, J., Jensterle, J. & Frith, C. D. (1994). Central monitoring deficiency and schizophrenic symptoms. Psychological Medicine 24, 557564.Google Scholar
Mortimer, A. M., Lund, C. E. & McKenna, P. J. (1990). The positive-negative dichotomy in schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry 157, 4149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shallice, T., Burgess, P. W. & Frith, C. D. (1991). Can the neuropsychological case study approach be applied to schizophrenia? Psychological Medicine 21, 661673.Google Scholar
Sodian, B. & Frith, U. (1992). Deception and sabotage in autistic, retarded and normal children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 33, 591605.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wing, J. K., Cooper, J. E. & Sartorius, N. (1974). Measurement and Classification of Psychiatric Symptoms. An Instruction Manual for the PSE and CATEGO Program. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge.Google Scholar