Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jkksz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T17:42:48.500Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The public's view of the costs and benefits of alternative energy systems

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 December 2011

T. R. Lee
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Surrey, Guildford, Surrey GU2 5XH, U.K.
Get access

Synopsis

The collective expression of attitudes towards energy systems governs demand and hence cost, which combines with other factors to shape energy policy. For example, in the national debate over nuclear power, where decision makers either share the public's anxieties about costs and safety or are expediently responsive to them, there is a constant threat to political acceptance. The paper describes how perceived risks are one of the costs of all technologies and a brief summary is made of research on comparative risk perception. This confirms the wide disparities often observed between lay and expert risk assessments and provides some explanations. When attention is focussed on particular hazards, the concept of attitude becomes more appropriate than risk perception because choice behaviour is determined by overall evaluations and these are a trade off between perceived risks and benefits. Each of the latter is acquired from two overlapping classes of received knowledge, i.e. factual information (e.g. nuclear power comes from the fission of uranium atoms), and beliefs (e.g. nuclear power will create/reduce employment). In addition, the attitude acquires emotional associations and behavioural dispositions, forming an organised complex based on the environmental object. This structural approach facilities the “unpacking” of public attitudes towards energy systems into their constituents and this is essential for understanding, for consultation and for inducing attitude change.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Royal Society of Edinburgh 1987

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alba, J. W. & Hasher, L. 1983. Is memory schematic? Psychological Bulletin 93, 203231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartlett, F. C. 1932. Remembering. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Brewer, W. F. & Treyens, J. C. 1981. Role of schemata in memory for places. Cognitive Psychology 13, 207230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fishbein, M. & Ajzen, I. 1975. Belief, Attitude, Intention and Behaviour: an Introduction to Theory and Research. Reading, Mass: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Greenwald, A. G. 1980. The totalitarian ego: Fabrication and revision of personal history. American Psychologist 35, 603618.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, T. R. 1986. Effective communication of information about chemical hazards. Science of the Total Environment 51, 149183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, T. R., Brown, J. & Henderson, J. L. 1984. The public's attitudes to nuclear power in the south west. Atom 336, 811.Google Scholar
Lingoes, J. C. 1973. The Guttman-Lingoes Nonmetric Program Series. Ann Arbor: M.A. thesis.Google Scholar
Loftus, E. F. 1979. Reactions to blatantly contradictory information. Memory and Cognition 7, 368374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paris, S. G. & Lindauer, B. K. 1977. Constructive aspects of children's comprehension and memory. In Perspective on the development of memory and cognition, eds. Kail, R. V. Jr and Hagen, J. W. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Royal Society 1983. Risk Assessment: A Study Group Report. London: Royal Society of London.Google Scholar
Slovic, P., Fischoff, B. & Lichtenstein, S. 1979. Environment 21 (3), 1420; 36–39.Google Scholar
Slovic, P., Fischoff, B. & Lichtenstein, S. 1980. In Societal Risk Assessment: How Safe is Safe Enough?, eds. Schwing, R. C. and Albers, W. A., pp. 181214, New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, S. E. & Crocket, J. 1981. Schematic bases of social information processing. In Social Cognition: The Ontario Symposium, eds. Higgins, E. T., Herman, C. P. and Zanna, M. P., Vol. 1. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Thomas, K. 1981. Comparative risk perception: how the public perceives the risks and benefits of energy systems. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London A376, 3550.Google Scholar
Thomas, K., Maurer, D., Fishbein, M. F., Otway, H. J., Hinkle, R. & Simpson, D. 1980. A Comparative Study of Public Beliefs about Five Energy Systems, RR 79-X. Laxenburg, Austria: International Institute for Applied Systems Analysis.Google Scholar