Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-v9fdk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T09:21:58.747Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Why liquid energy results in overconsumption

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 February 2011

Cees de Graaf*
Affiliation:
Division of Human Nutrition, Wageningen University, PO Box 8129, 6700 EV Wageningen, The Netherlands
*
Corresponding author: Professor Cees de Graaf, fax: +31 317 483342, email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Liquids have been shown to have a low satiating efficiency. The may be related to the high rate of consumption for liquids which may be higher than 200 g/min. In a number of studies, we showed that the positive relationship between eating rate and energy intake is mediated by oro-sensory exposure time. Longer sensory exposure times are consistently associated with lower food intakes. This observation maybe linked to the role of cephalic phase responses to foods. Cephalic phase responses are a set of physiological responses, which are conceived to prepare the digestive system for the incoming flow of nutrients after ingestion, with the aim of maintaining homeostasis. Results from various studies suggest that cephalic phase responses are much smaller (absent) for liquids compared to solids. It is hypothesised that the absence of cephalic phase responses to liquid foods may be one of the causes why liquid energies enter the body undetected and lead to weak energy intake compensation. This idea fits with the concept of the taste system as a nutrient-sensing system that informs the brain and the gastro-intestinal system about what is coming into our body. With liquids, this system is bypassed. Slower eating may help the human body to associate the sensory signals from food with their metabolic consequences. Foods that are eaten quickly may impair this association, and may therefore lead to overconsumption of energy, and ultimately to weight gain.

Type
Symposium on ‘Nutrition: getting the balance right in 2010’
Copyright
Copyright © The Author 2011

Abbreviations:
CPR

cephalic phase response

MSF

modified sham feeding

In the last few decades it has become clear that the high prevalence of obesity in developing countries is a normal behavioural response to an abnormal obesogenic environment. This obesogenic environment facilitates food (energy) intake and limits energy expenditure. Recent evidence suggests that changes in energy intake are the dominant factors in increasing prevalence of obesity since the 1980s(Reference Swinburn, Sacks and Lo1, Reference Westerterp and Speakman2). Frequent exposure to food cues, large portion sizes, large available variety, high energy density and high palatability of foods are implicated in this respect(Reference Carnell and Wardle3).

This paper focuses on one additional element of our food supply and our eating environment, i.e. the high eating rate at which we can ingest a vast majority of foods in our current food supply. It is hypothesised that the high eating rate undermines our body's capacity to regulate energy intake at healthy levels. This may work by impairing the congruent relationship between sensory input and metabolic consequences. The physiological background for this may be that foods that are consumed quickly like liquids do not lead to functional cephalic phase responses (CPR). CPR are the predominantly learned physiological responses to the sensory signals from food. Before focusing on the role of eating rate in the regulation of food intake, we first discuss a simple psycho-biological model of eating behaviour that helps to explain the later reasoning in this paper.

Sensory and metabolic signals involved in eating behaviour

Fig. 1 illustrates how eating behaviour is guided by sensory and metabolic signals. In addition, sensory signals of foods are linked in the brain to the metabolic consequences of eating. These (mostly unconscious) learning processes that take place within the context of our living/eating environment shape our food pattern. They guide us throughout the day as to what to eat and also what to expect about the consequences of eating and drinking(Reference Woods4). For example, people know what to consume for lunch in order to stay satiated until dinner. People know what to expect from a cup of coffee in the morning or a glass of wine in the evening.

Fig. 1. Factors effecting eating behaviour. Sensory processes determine what we eat, and are also responsible for variety in the diet. Metabolic processes determine how much we eat. In the brain, sensory signals during eating are linked to the metabolic consequences. These (largely unconscious) learning processes shape our nutrition pattern. The soft-background of foods represents our current-day food environment (Source: de Graaf and Kok(Reference de Graaf and Kok75)).

Sensory signals play an important role in food choice; in general, we choose the foods we like, and we avoid the foods we dislike(Reference de Graaf, Kramer and Meiselman5). Sensory signals are also involved in the drive for variety in the diet. Repeated exposure to a particular sensory signal (e.g. a meat flavour; a fruit flavour) during a meal leads to a decline in the reward value of that food, and a shift in interest towards other foods. This phenomenon is called sensory-specific satiation and/or satiety, and has been demonstrated in a large number of studies (e.g.Reference Rolls, Rowe and Rolls6Reference Griffioen-Roose, Finalyson and Mars12).

Metabolic signals are involved in the regulation of energy intake. Meal initiation is guided by learned associations (time on a clock; a dressed table), which are modulated, for example, by an empty stomach, high ghrelin levels and/or short transient declines in glucose levels in the blood(Reference Strubbe and Woods13Reference Drazen, Vahl and D'Alessio15). During eating, hormonal and neural signals from the gastro-intestinal tract to the brain increase our feelings of satiation. Stretch receptors in the stomach and various hormones such as cholecystokinin, glucagon-like peptide-1 and insulin are involved in this process(Reference Strubbe and Woods13Reference Drazen, Vahl and D'Alessio15). After the meal we stay satiated for some time depending on the amount and the composition of the foods that we ingest. After a couple of hours the cycle repeats itself. Apart from the short-term regulation of food intake, there is also a longer term regulation of energy balance based on signals coming from the adipose and lean tissue mass(Reference Strubbe and Woods13Reference Drazen, Vahl and D'Alessio15). This is, however, outside the scope of this paper.

Various learning processes in food choice and food intake have been well documented. One example of such a learning process is flavour-nutrient learning, which leads to a preference for foods that are high in carbohydrates(Reference Birch, McPhee and Steinberg16), fat(Reference Johnson, McPhee and Birch17, Reference Kern, McPhee and Fisher18) and/or energy density(Reference Appleton, Gentry and Sheperd19). The absence of energy taste conditioning may also explain the low preference for the pure taste vegetables in children and adults(Reference Zeinstra, Koelen and Kok20).

Another example of learning in relation to hunger and satiety is the notion that our meal patterns are highly adjustable to external constraints/environmental circumstances. We usually get hungry just before the regular dinner time, and many people get an appetite for something sweet just before a coffee break. Environmental circumstances provide external cues (e.g. the time on a clock, the emptying of a soup bowl and the smell of food) related to eating behaviour(Reference Woods4). These patterns and the accompanying anticipatory physiological signals (e.g. ghrelin levels; see e.g. Drazen et al.(Reference Drazen, Vahl and D'Alessio15)), can be easily changed with varying environmental circumstances. This flexibility and adaptability are reflected in the wide diversity of eating cultures in the world(Reference Woods4, Reference Menzel and D'Aluisio21), and also in the continuous change of eating patterns.

Liquid calories and the regulation of food intake and body weight

One important difference between our current food supply compared to the food supply of our ancestors is that many processed foods in our current diet can be eaten at a high rate. The Neolithic diet compared to our current diet consisted of a relatively high-protein diet (meat and fish) with many foods with a high-fibre content (fruit, vegetables and root plant foods)(Reference Cordain, Eaton and Sebastian22). Meat products and high-fibre products are typically foods that require chewing and are eaten at a relatively low rate. The Neolithic diet did also not contain any liquid calories, except for milk for infants. Infancy is a period of rapid growth of body weight, where body weight at 1 year after birth is usually more than two times the weight at birth(Reference Kuczmarski, Ogden and Grummer-Strawn23).

Liquids are consumed at much faster rates than solid foods(Reference Viskaal-Van Dongen, Kok and de Graaf24). This can be seen in Fig. 2, which shows that liquid foods stand out compared to other foods in terms of eating rate. They are consumed at rates higher than 200 g/min, with the exception of soup, which has an eating rate that is comparable to many solid foods (<100 g/min)(Reference Viskaal-Van Dongen, Kok and de Graaf24). In this sense, liquid foods can be considered as fast foods, foods that can be eaten quickly. One typical example of the effect of processing food on eating rate is illustrated by a study of Haber et al.(Reference Haber25). This showed that it takes about 17 min to consume 500 g apples, whereas 500 g apple juice is consumed in just 1·5 min(Reference Haber25). Eating 8368 kJ (2000 kcal), i.e. the average energy requirement for a sedentary woman in the industrialised world, would take more than 2 h for eating apples, whereas for the apple juice the required time would be about 18 min.

Fig. 2. Eating rate (g/min) of forty-five food products across the domain of our food supply. Eating rate was assessed by asking subjects to consume 50 g of each product and recording the time (seconds) necessary to ingest this 50 g (Source: Viskaal-van Dongen M, Kok FJ & de Graaf C (2011) Appetite (In the Press)).

From the appetite/satiety literature it has become clear that liquid foods have a lower satiating capacity than solid foods, with the exception of soup. Some early studies have shown that hunger, measured through subjective scales(Reference Blundell, de Graaf and Hulshof26) is more suppressed after consumption of solid fruits compared to an equivalent amount of fruit juices with similar energy and macronutrient content(Reference Haber25, Reference Bolton, Heaton and Burroughs27). The results of Hulshof et al.(Reference Hulshof, de Graaf and Weststrate28) reinforced this idea by comparing the effects of hunger on liquid fat preloads with the effects of preloads made solid with gelatin or locust bean gum. The weak effects of liquid calories on energy intake compensation were recently confirmed for liquid and solid proteins, fats and carbohydrates(Reference Mourao, Bressan and Campbell29, Reference Martens, Lemmens and Born30). The exceptional role of soup is illustrated by a study which showed that soups in a variety of forms reduce energy intake at a test meal(Reference Flood and Rolls31). This issue is worked out in the next section on oro-sensory exposure time and satiety.

The low satiating effect of liquids is not only clear from short preload test meal studies but also from longer term intervention studies. For example, a ten-week study showed that the energy from sugar-sweetened beverages added up to the total energy content of a regular diet(Reference Raben, Vasilaras and Moller32). In a four-week cross-over study, fifteen subjects consumed 1·9 MJ of carbohydrates in either a solid or liquid form. The results showed that subjects ate less throughout the remainder of the day when the solid calories were consumed, but subjects did not lower their intake when the calories were consumed in the form of liquids(Reference DiMeglio and Mattes33). Body-weight changes varied accordingly; subjects gained weight in the liquid calorie condition, but not in the solid calorie condition. Also with respect to long-term weight change soup is different from caloric beverages. In a one-year weight loss/maintenance study, the provision of two soups led to higher weight loss after one year than providing one soup per day or two snacks(Reference Rolls, Roe and Beach34).

In a recently published long-term (18 months) randomised controlled behavioural trial among 810 subjects, it was shown that a reduction in liquid calorie intake had a stronger effect on weight loss than did a reduction in calorie intake(Reference Chen, Appel and Loria35). Apparently, people detected the solid calories, but not the liquid calories, even after repeated long-term exposure. The notion that liquid calories are not well detected is in line with the results of a number of large-scale prospective epidemiological studies that show a positive association between sugar-sweetened beverage consumption and weight gain and obesity(Reference Schulze, Manson and Ludwig36Reference Hu and Malik38).

The effect of oro-sensory exposure time on appetite and food intake

One of the fundamental differences between the apple and the apple juice in the studies of Haber et al.(Reference Haber25), Mattes(Reference Mattes39) and Mattes and Campbell(Reference Mattes and Campbell40) is the oral sensory-exposure time. With the apple juice there is only a brief moment of sensory stimulation; the vast majority of the sugar and aroma molecules pass the oral/pharyngeal cavity without contact with the senses of smell and taste. The big difference in the duration of aroma (=olfactory) stimulation between various liquid and solid foods was recently confirmed by Ruijschop et al.(Reference Ruijschop, Burgering and Jacobs41), who measured aroma molecules in the exhaled air during food consumption. The duration of aroma release was about ten times longer for mature cheese (solid) than for a strawberry-flavoured dairy liquid(Reference Ruijschop, Burgering and Jacobs41).

As noted earlier, the exception to the rule concerning the low satiating capacity of liquids is soup. Soup has consistently shown to be a high satiating liquid. Mattes compared the hunger response after whole apples, apple juice and apple ‘soup’, which was the apple juice consumed using a spoon(Reference Mattes39). Consuming apple juice using a spoon enhances the sensory exposure time to the taste system. The apple ‘soup’ yielded a comparable satiating effect to the apples; the whole apples and the apple soup were much more satiating than the apple juice. One possible explanation for this effect is the observation that oro-sensory exposure with soup is much higher than oro-sensory exposure with a drink. This was confirmed by a study where vegetable soup and tomato soup had eating rate well below 100 g/min(Reference Viskaal-Van Dongen, Kok and de Graaf24) (see Fig. 2).

When the effects of sensory signals on food intake are considered, it may be good to realise that sensory signals are mainly operational during eating. This makes it probable that the sensory signals predominantly affect satiation (meal termination) and to a lesser degree satiety (absence of hunger in between meal, involved in meal initiation). This idea is also clear from the satiety cascade from Blundell(Reference Blundell, de Graaf and Hulshof26), which distinguishes between sensory, cognitive, post-ingestive and post-absorptive factors in appetite.

The role of eating rate and oro-sensory exposure in satiation was recently studied in a series of experiments in which eating rate and oro-sensory exposure were systematically varied. In a first within-subjects cross-over study in a cinema setting 108 subjects consumed ad libitum a liquid (milk) or semi-solid (custard) chocolate dairy product which were equal in palatability, energy density and macronutrient composition(Reference Zijlstra, Mars and de Wijk42). Subjects consumed the products with a broad straw (1·2 cm diameter) from 1·5 litre carton boxes which prevented weight cues and visual cues from playing an important role in intake. The ad libitum consumption of the chocolate-flavoured liquid was about 30% higher compared to a similarly flavoured semi-solid. In a subsequent study under laboratory conditions with about fifty subjects, we investigated whether the eating effort and/or eating rate were responsible for the 30% difference in intake. This was done with the help of peristaltic pumps, which eliminated the effort to get the milk/custard into the mouth, and which could be set at standardised rates. Eliminating the effort had little effect on the difference between ad libitum intakes between the liquid and semi-solid (29%). However, when we matched the eating rate for both products, the difference in ad libitum intakes disappeared to a large extent(Reference Zijlstra, Mars and de Wijk42) (see Fig. 3).

Fig. 3. (a) Ad libitum intake (g, with standard deviation) and energy intake (kJ) of the liquid, semi-liquid and semi-solid chocolate dairy products in a cinema setting (n 108; within subjects). Test products were similar in palatability, energy and macronutrient content, and consumed through a straw from 1·5 litre carton boxes over a period of about 90 min. Intake from the semi-solid was 30% less than from the liquid test-food. (b) Ad libitum intake (g, with standard deviation) and energy intake (kJ) of the liquid and semi-solid chocolate dairy product in a laboratory setting (sensory cabin) (n 49; within subjects) within a time frame of about 15 min. In the free eating rate, different effort, subjects consumed the products in the same way as in the cinema setting. In the free eating rate, no effort, subjects consumed the product from a tube making use of a peristaltic pump; in this condition subjects could adjust the rate of delivery. In the fixed eating rate, no effort condition, investigators set the rate of delivery of the test-products into the mouth of the subjects. (Source: Zijlstra et al.(Reference Zijlstra, Mars and de Wijk42)).

In a subsequent study, it was shown that changing the oro-sensory exposure of chocolate custard from 3 to 9 s/bite led to a 10% lower ad libitum intake(Reference Zijlstra, de Wijk and Mars43). The suppressive effect of oro-sensory exposure time on ad libitum intake was confirmed in another study with orangeade, where we held the eating rate constant (150 g/min) but only varied the relative duration of sensory exposure (long: 30 s/min of consumption; short 15 s/min of consumption)(Reference Weijzen, Smeets and de Graaf44). In a later experimental study, harder and softer versions of solid foods were compared with respect to their effect on ad libitum intake(Reference Zijlstra, Mars and Stafleu45). Again, foods that were eaten more slowly resulted in lower food and energy intakes(Reference Zijlstra, Mars and Stafleu45).

In a recent study, subjects (n 105) were repeatedly (ten times) exposed to liquid and semi-solid yoghurt (drinks) for breakfast. Subjects consumed the breakfast either in a liquid form with a straw (n 35), a liquid form with a spoon (n 35) or in a semi-solid form with a spoon (n 35). Eating rate and energy/food intake were higher in the straw condition compared to the spoon conditions(Reference Hogenkamp, Mars and Stafleu46). This result suggests, as the results of the other studies, that it is not the texture per se, but that the effect of texture on food intake works through oro-sensory exposure time.

In the studies described, the eating rate was varied within one particular food, and the foods used were mostly liquids and semi-solids. Viskaal-van Dongen et al.(Reference Viskaal-Van Dongen, Kok and de Graaf24) studied the relationship between eating rate and ad libitum food and energy intake across a range of forty-five different food products, representative of the current food supply. One of the most striking results was the magnitude of the differences in eating rate between various foods. Peanuts were consumed at a rate of about 14 g/min, whereas apple juice and diet coke were consumed at rates of more than 600 g/min. In this study, we observed a positive association between eating rate and ad libitum intake in g (R 2=0·37). With respect to peanuts, it is worth mentioning that peanuts and other nuts have a high energy density, and therefore they are expected to have a low satiating efficiency. However, they have a high satiating capacity(Reference Alper and Mattes47, Reference Mattes, Kris-Etherton and Foster48) which is attributable to their low consumption rate.

Altogether, these studies show that foods that can be eaten quickly lead to higher food intakes and lower satiating effects. With fast foods, it may be more difficult to learn to associate the sensory signals during eating with the metabolic consequences after eating (see Fig. 1). This idea was recently partly confirmed in a study in which people were capable of adjusting their intake after repeated exposure to high energy dense semi-solids, but not after repeated exposure to high energy dense liquids(Reference Mars, Hogenkamp and Gosses49).

To summarise, the effects of texture on appetite and food intake mainly work through oro-sensory exposure time, and thus oro-sensory stimulation. This finding raises the question: how do these oro-sensory signals exert their effect? The responses to sensory cues, such as the taste and smell of food, include a cascade of pre-absorptive physiological responses, which are collectively referred to as CPR(Reference Zafra, Moilia and Puerto50, Reference Smeets, Erkner and de Graaf51). CPR are a possible way by which we may explain the different satiating capacities of foods that are eaten quickly and foods that are eaten slowly.

The role of cephalic phase responses in the regulation of food intake

CPR are a set of physiological responses, which are conceived to prepare the digestive system for the incoming flow of nutrients after ingestion, with the aim of maintaining homeostasis(Reference Zafra, Moilia and Puerto50). One of the clearest examples is the salivation response after the sight of or the mere thinking about food. The famous Russian scientist Pavlov was one of the first investigators to study this response, showing that dogs started to salivate when they were exposed to meat(52). CPR are also thought to be learned, implying that they are adjustable to the type and size of the meal that is eaten. The responses are transient and often last for only a few minutes; however, their effects on post-prandrial metabolism can be of a longer duration(Reference Robertson53).

The homeostatic function of the CPR is clear from a study which showed that the glucose and insulin responses after an intragastric glucose load were lower when subjects chewed on a peanut butter sandwich compared to a no-chewing control condition(Reference Teff and Engelman54). On average plasma glucose levels returned to baseline earlier after oral sensory stimulation (106 min) than when oral stimulation did not occur. The results of this study indicate that the oral sensory stimulation helped in maintaining glucose homeostasis.

Cephalic stimulation works through the mere thought of food, and through the five senses involved in food perception, i.e. the senses of vision, taste, smell, touch and hearing. Cephalic stimulation ultimately activates the vagus nerve, which results in a myriad of autonomic responses such as the release of various physiologically active compounds, including saliva, gastric acid, pancreatic exocrine enzymes, as well as hormones from the endocrine pancreas(Reference Zafra, Moilia and Puerto50). Early CPR include saliva and later CPR include gastric acid secretion and the release of pancreatic polypeptide(Reference Zafra, Moilia and Puerto50). The pancreatic polypeptide response is usually considered as the biological marker for vagal stimulation(Reference Robertson53). Most CPR are instrumental for digestion such as hydrochloric acid, gastrin and lipase release in the stomach and digestive enzymes from the pancreas and the duodenum. They have little to do with appetite(Reference Smeets, Erkner and de Graaf51).

The CPR in human subjects is usually assessed through a modified sham-feeding paradigm, where subjects chew on foods and spit the food out at the time where they would normally swallow a food. For a detailed explanation of this methodology, see papers by Teff and co-workers(Reference Teff and Engelman54Reference Crystal and Teff57) and Robertson and co-workers(Reference Robertson53, Reference Heath, Jones and Frayn58). Recovery rates of the chewed foods should be close to 100%(Reference Robertson53).

The role of cephalic responses in the regulation of food intake has hardly been studied (for a review see(Reference Smeets, Erkner and de Graaf51)). There are various possible ways in which CPR may affect appetite. There could be a direct centrally mediated effect on appetite. This direct effect is presumably a learned response based on the association between sensory signals and subsequent metabolic consequences. The notion that people can assess expected satiety from various foods immediately, i.e. within a few seconds after the first bite of food is in line with this idea(Reference Brunstrom, Shakeshaft and Scott-Samuel59, Reference Wilkinson and Brunstrom60). The neuro-biological mechanism behind this effect is not yet clear. The findings of studies showing that tasting and ingesting glucose have an impact on the activity of the hypothalamus within 2 min may be related to this mechanism(Reference Smeets, de Graaf and Stafleu61, Reference Smeets, de Graaf and Stafleu62).

CPR may also affect appetite through the release of gastro-intestinal hormones such as insulin, pancreatic polypeptide, leptin and ghrelin(Reference Smeets, Erkner and de Graaf51). In a study where subjects were given a 50 g intragastric fat load, subjective fullness ratings were increased after 1 h modified sham feeding (MSF) of pizza(Reference Heath, Jones and Frayn58). In this study, a 25% suppression in ghrelin concentration was observed after the fat load, which was enhanced further in the MSF condition. The lower ghrelin levels in the MSF condition were accompanied by lower hunger ratings.

In two studies, Smeets and colleagues showed that the 15–20 min oro-sensory stimulation with fat increases satiety and a relative increase in glucose and insulin levels(Reference Smeets, Lejeune and Westerterp-Plantenga63), and may decrease ghrelin levels relative to a water control lunch(Reference Smeets and Westerterp-Plantenga64). Crystall and Teff(Reference Crystal and Teff57) showed that a 3 min MSF of high-fat cakes led to higher pancreatic polypeptide levels than MSF of non-fat cakes. The 3 min MSF is much shorter than the 15–20 min of Smeets and colleagues(Reference Smeets, Lejeune and Westerterp-Plantenga63, Reference Smeets and Westerterp-Plantenga64) and the 60 min of Heath et al.(Reference Heath, Jones and Frayn58). Interestingly, the 3 min MSF feeding with high-fat cakes led to a higher intake of high-fat foods.

In one of her first studies on CPR, Teff et al.(Reference Teff, Devine and Engelman56) showed that sweet-tasting liquids are not adequate stimuli for the elicitation of the cephalic phase insulin response. However, in that same study, MSF apple pie did result in changes in glucose and insulin levels. In a very recent study, Teff(Reference Teff55) measured the pancreatic polypeptide response (as a measure of stimulation of the vagal nerve) to liquid stimuli, chewing gum and regular mixed nutrient food that required chewing. This study showed that the liquids and chewing gum were not good elicitors of a CPR, but the mixed nutrient foods on which people had to chew were more effective in eliciting a CPR. The results from another group(Reference Kokkinos, Le Roux and Alexiadou65) showed that consumption of a 2824·2 kJ (675 kcal) mixed meal during 30 min resulted in about 25% higher response of the satiety hormones PYY and glucagon-like peptide 1 compared to the consumption of the same meal within 5 min.

The role of oro-sensory stimulation in appetite regulation compared to the gastric contribution to satiety was also nicely shown in a series of elegant studies(Reference Cecil, Francis and Read66, Reference Cecil, Francis and Read67). The main characteristic of these studies was the independent manipulation of oral and gastric stimulation. In one of the first studies, it was shown that oral administration of soup produced the greatest suppression of appetite compared to intragastric delivery and intraduodenal delivery of soup(Reference Cecil, Francis and Read66). Overt intragastric delivery led to a higher suppression of hunger than covert intragastric delivery(Reference Cecil, Francis and Read66). Oral administration also delayed gastric emptying. These findings were later confirmed in other studies with oral and gastric administration of fats and carbohydrates(Reference Cecil, Francis and Read67) and sucrose v. maltose(Reference Lavin, French and Read68).

Discussion

The basic idea of this paper is that liquid foods and foods that can be eaten quickly undermine our body's capacity to regulate food intake at healthy levels. They do this because they provide insufficient sensory signalling to inform the brain and the gastro-intestinal tract about the inflow of nutrients. Therefore, they have a low-satiating capacity, which in turn may lead to excess energy intake, and ultimately to overweight.

The idea that we can estimate/know the satiating capacity of a familiar food immediately after the first bite(Reference Brunstrom, Shakeshaft and Scott-Samuel59, Reference Wilkinson and Brunstrom60) indicates that sensory signals have acquired meaning in terms of satiety after repeated consumption. Repeated consumption leads to an association between the sensory signals during consumption and the post-ingestive satiety effects, coming from the macronutrients in the food. This idea fits with the concept of the taste system as a nutrient sensing system that informs the brain and the gastro-intestinal system about what is coming into our body. With liquids, this system is bypassed.

The transformation in our food supply, especially during the last decades, has gone in the direction of foods that are easy to get and convenient to eat. The convenience to eat may increase with the amount of processing of food. Insight into the effects of processing of foods on the satiating efficiency may be instrumental for the food industry to reverse the direction of food design with respect to eating rate and food intake. A major challenge to the food industry is the design of foods that are pleasant to eat, but at the same time, either provide satiation early enough to prevent overeating and/or provide satiety long and strong enough to postpone the next eating occasion.

The role of the CPR in the explanation of the low satiating capacity of liquids needs further elucidation. There are relatively few data on the relationship between CPR and satiety. The data of Teff(Reference Teff55) and Teff et al.(Reference Teff55, Reference Teff, Devine and Engelman56) of oro-sensory stimulation on insulin and pancreatic polypeptide, the data of Heath et al.(Reference Heath, Jones and Frayn58) and Smeets et al.(Reference Smeets and Westerterp-Plantenga64) with respect to ghrelin are in line with the concept that these cephalic phase signals contribute to the satiating effect of foods. The data of Kokkinos et al.(Reference Kokkinos, Le Roux and Alexiadou65) indicate that eating slowly leads to higher levels of the satiety hormones glucagon-like peptide 1 and PYY. However, glucagon-like peptide 1 and PYY have not been implicated in the CPR(Reference Smeets, Erkner and de Graaf51).

The low satiating capacity of liquid calories has been a controversial issue for some time(Reference Almiron-Roig, Chen and Drewnowski69, Reference Drewnowski and Bellisle70). Almiron-Roig et al.(Reference Almiron-Roig, Chen and Drewnowski69) conclude that ‘the evidence that liquids have less impact on satiety than do solid foods remains inconclusive’. Drewnowski and Bellisle(Reference Drewnowski and Bellisle70) conclude that ‘research evidence comparing the short-term satiating power of different types of liquids and solids remains inconclusive’. In the US dietary guidelines of 2010(71), question 7 in Appendix E-1 Major conclusions with respect to energy balance and weight management, reads, ‘What is the impact of liquid versus solid foods on energy intake and body weight?’. The answer is ‘A limited body of evidence show conflicting results about whether liquid and solid foods differ in their effect on energy intake and body weight except that liquids in the form of soups may lead to decreased energy intake and body weight’(71). This controversy is resolved when we take the eating rate and duration of oro-sensory stimulation into account. Soup is a liquid, but its eating rate is equivalent to a solid food(Reference Viskaal-Van Dongen, Kok and de Graaf24). This is the reason why soup is more satiating than energetic beverages which may be consumed at much higher rates.

The higher satiating effect of soup may be primarily determined by its eating rate comparable to other solid foods. Another issue that may be involved here is the warm temperature at which soup is eaten. A warmer temperature may lead to a stronger sensory response and therefore also a stronger CPR and a higher satiety value. A recent study showed that the cephalic phase insulin response was absent in response to a cold glucose solution(Reference Shinozaki, Shimizu and Shiina72), while a room temperature glucose solution did induce these responses. A study in mice found increased gustatory nerve responses with increasing temperature(Reference Talavera, Yasumatsu and Voets73), and a study in human subjects showed reductions in perceived sweetness intensity due to cooling of the tongue(Reference Green and Frankmann74). Taken together, these data suggest that chilled energetic beverages might be processed suboptimal, and warm foods like soup may have stronger CPR. Further research on this issue is warranted, also in relation to the widespread consumption of chilled beverages and ice cream.

Coming back to the model presented in Fig. 1, it is clear that sensory signals acquire meaning with respect to liking (hedonic value) and satiation/satiety after repeated exposure to foods. One of the exciting research questions for the near future is where and how the sensory signals during consumption and peripheral signals after ingestion are integrated. Which signals from the gastro-intestinal tract are involved in the process and where do they go to in the brain?

There is an increased need to understand the psycho-biological, neurological and molecular mechanisms through which our current eating environment impairs our control of food intake, and this is the domain of experimental studies. The current state of the art in the field of nutri-genomics, neuro-endocrinology and neuro-imaging techniques may help in unravelling these basic mechanisms. This is an exciting prospect for a growing field that has to deal with an increasing societal problem of obesity.

Acknowledgements

The author declares no conflict of interest. This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

References

1.Swinburn, BA, Sacks, G, Lo, SK et al. (2009) Estimating the energy flux that characterize the rise of obesity. Am J Clin Nutr 89, 17231728.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
2.Westerterp, KR & Speakman, JR (2008) Physical activity energy expenditure has not declined since the 1980s and matches energy expenditures of wild mammals. Int J Obes 32, 12561263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
3.Carnell, S & Wardle, J (2008) Appetitive traits and childhood obesity: measurement, origins and implications for intervention. Proc Nutr Soc 67, 343355.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4.Woods, SC (1991) The eating paradox: how we tolerate food. Psychol Rev 98, 488505.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5.de Graaf, C, Kramer, FM, Meiselman, HL et al. . (2005) Food acceptability in field studies with US Army men and women: relationship with food intake and food choice after repeated exposures. Appetite 44, 2331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6.Rolls, BJ, Rowe, EA, Rolls, ET et al. (1981) Variety in a meal enhances food intake in man. Physiol Behav 26, 215221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
7.Higgs, S, Williamson, AC, Rotshtein, P et al. (2008) Sensory-specific satiety is intact in amnesics who eat multiple meals. Psychol Sci 19, 623628.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8.Remick, AK, Polivy, J & Pliner, P (2009) Internal and external moderators of the effect of variety on food intake. Psychol Bull 135, 434451.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9.Nolan, LJ & Hetherington, MM (2009) The effects of sham feeding-induced sensory specific satiation and food variety on subsequent food intake in humans. Appetite 52, 720725.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10.Brondel, L, Lauraine, G, van Wymelbeke, V et al. (2010) Alternation between Foods within a meal. Influence on satiation and consumption in humans. Appetite 53, 203209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
11.Havermans, RC, Hermanns, J & Jansen, A (2010) Eating without a nose: olfactory dysfunction and sensory-specific satiety. Chem Senses 35, 735741.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12.Griffioen-Roose, S, Finalyson, G, Mars, M et al. . (2010) Measuring food reward and the transfer effect of sensory specific satiety. Appetite 55, 648655.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13.Strubbe, JH & Woods, SC (2004) The timing of meals. Psychol Rev 111, 128141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14.Woods, SC (2009) The control of food intake: behavioral v. molecular approaches. Cell Metab 9, 489498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
15.Drazen, DL, Vahl, TP, D'Alessio, DA et al. (2006) Effects of a fixed meal pattern on ghrelin secretion: evidence for a learned response independent of nutrient status. Endocrinol 147, 2330.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16.Birch, LL, McPhee, LL, Steinberg, LL et al. (1990) Conditioned flavor preferences in young children. Physiol Behav 47, 501505.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17.Johnson, SL, McPhee, L & Birch, LL (1991) Conditioned preferences: young children prefer flavors associated with high dietary fat. Physiol Behav 50, 12451251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
18.Kern, DL, McPhee, L, Fisher, J et al. (1993) The postingestive consequences of fat condition preferences for flavors associated with high dietary fat. Physiol Behav 54, 7176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19.Appleton, KM, Gentry, RC & Sheperd, R (2006) Evidence of a role for conditioning in the development of liking for flavours in human in everyday life. Physiol Behav 87, 478486.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
20.Zeinstra, GG, Koelen, MA, Kok, FJ et al. (2009) Children's hard wired aversion to the pure vegetables tastes. A ‘failed’ flavour-nutrient learning study. Appetite 52, 528530.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
21.Menzel, P & D'Aluisio, F (2006) Hungry Planet: What the World Eats. Napa, CA: Material World books.Google Scholar
22.Cordain, L, Eaton, SB, Sebastian, A et al. (2005) Origins and evolution of the Western diet: health implication for the 21st century. Am J Clin Nutr 81, 341354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
23.Kuczmarski, RJ, Ogden, CL, Grummer-Strawn, LM et al. (2000) CDC Growth Charts: United States. CDC, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Advance Data, No. 314.Google Scholar
24.Viskaal-Van Dongen, M, Kok, FJ & de Graaf, C (2011) Eating rate of commonly consumed foods promotes food and energy intake. Appetite 56, 2531.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25.Haber, GB (1977) Depletion and disruption of dietary fibre. Effects on satiety, plasma-glucose, and serum insulin. Lancet 2, 679682.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
26.Blundell, J, de Graaf, C, Hulshof, T et al. (2010) Appetite control: methodological aspects of the evaluation of foods. Obes Rev 11, 251270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27.Bolton, RP, Heaton, KW & Burroughs, LF (1981) The role of dietary fibre in satiety, glucose and insulin: studies with fruit and fruit juices. Am J Clin Nutr 34, 211217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
28.Hulshof, T, de Graaf, C & Weststrate, JA (1993) The effects of preloads varying in physical state and fat content on satiety and energy intake. Appetite 21, 273286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
29.Mourao, DM, Bressan, J, Campbell, WW et al. (2007) Effects of food form on appetite and energy intake in lean and obese young adults. Int J Obes 31, 16881695.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
30.Martens, MJI, Lemmens, SGT, Born, JM et al. . (2011) A solid high-protein meal evokes stronger hunger suppression than a liquefied high-protein meal. Obesity (In the Press).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
31.Flood, JE & Rolls, BJ (2007) Soup preloads in a variety of forms reduce meal energy intake. Appetite 49, 626634.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
32.Raben, A, Vasilaras, TH, Moller, AC et al. (2002) Sucrose compared with artificial sweeteners: different effects on ad libitum food intake and body weight after 10 weeks of supplementation in overweight subjects. Am J Clin Nutr 76, 721729.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
33.DiMeglio, DP & Mattes, RD (2000) Liquid versus solid carbohydrate: effects on food intake and body weight. Int J Obes Relat Metan Disord 24, 794800.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
34.Rolls, BJ, Roe, LS, Beach, AM et al. (2005) Provision of foods differing in energy density affects long-term weight loss. Obes Res 13, 10521060.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
35.Chen, L, Appel, LJ, Loria, C et al. (2009) Reduction in consumption of sugar-sweetened beverages is associated with weight loss: the PREMIER trial. Am J Clin Nutr 89, 12991306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
36.Schulze, MB, Manson, JE, Ludwig, DS et al. (2004) Sugar-sweetened beverages, weight gain, and incidence of type 2 diabetes in young and middle-aged women. J Am Med Assoc 292, 927934.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
37.Malik, VS, Popkin, BM, Bray, GA et al. . (2010) Sugar sweetened beverages and risk of metabolic syndrome and type 2 diabetes: a meta-analysis. Diabetes Care 33, 24772483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
38.Hu, FB & Malik, VS (2010) Sugar-sweetened beverages and risk of obesity and type 2 diabetes: epidemiological evidence. Physiol Behav 100, 4754.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
39.Mattes, R (2005) Soup and satiety. Physiol Behav 17, 739747.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
40.Mattes, RD & Campbell, WW (2009) Effects of food form and timing of ingestion on appetite and energy intake in lean young adults and in young adults with obesity. J Am Diet Assoc 109, 430437.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
41.Ruijschop, RM, Burgering, MJ, Jacobs, MA et al. (2009) Retro-nasal aroma release depends on both subject and product differences: a link to food intake regulation? Chem Senses 34, 395403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
42.Zijlstra, N, Mars, M, de Wijk, RA et al. (2008) The effect of viscosity on ad libitum food intake. Int J Obes 32, 676683.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
43.Zijlstra, N, de Wijk, RA, Mars, M et al. (2009) Effect of bite size and oral processing time of a semisolid food on satiation. Am J Clin Nutr 90, 269275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
44.Weijzen, PL, Smeets, PA & de Graaf, C (2009) Sip-size of orangeade: effects on intake and on sensory-specific satiation. Br J Nutr 102, 10911097.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
45.Zijlstra, N, Mars, M, Stafleu, A et al. . (2010) The effects of texture differences on satiation in 3 pairs of solid foods. Appetite 55, 490497.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
46.Hogenkamp, PS, Mars, M, Stafleu, A et al. (2010) Intake during repeated exposure to low- and high-energy-dense yoghurts by different means of consumption. Am J Clin Nutr 91, 841847.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
47.Alper, CM & Mattes, RD (2002) Effects of chronic peanut consumption on energy balance and hedonics. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 26, 11291137.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
48.Mattes, RD, Kris-Etherton, PM & Foster, GD (2008) Impact of peanuts and tree nuts on body weight and healthy weight loss in adults. J Nutr 138, 1741S1745S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
49.Mars, M, Hogenkamp, PS, Gosses, AM et al. (2009) Effect of viscosity on learned satiation. Physiol Behav 98, 6066.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
50.Zafra, MA, Moilia, F & Puerto, A (2006) The neural/cephalic phase reflexes in the physiology of nutrition. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 30, 10321044.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
51.Smeets, PAM, Erkner, A, de Graaf, C (2010) Cephalic phase responses and appetite. Nutr Rev 68, 643655.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
52. 2010 ‘Ivan Pavlov – Biography’. available at http://nobelprize.org/nobel_prizes/medicine/laureates/1904/pavlov-bio.html (accessed 22 August 2010).Google Scholar
53.Robertson, MD (2006) Food perception and postprandial lipid metabolism. Physiol Behav 89, 49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
54.Teff, Kl & Engelman, K (1996) Oral sensory stimulation improves glucose tolerance: effects on post-prandial glucose, insulin, C-peptide and glucagon. Am J Phsiol 270, R1371R3179.Google Scholar
55.Teff, KL (2010) Cehalic phase pancreatic polypeptide response to liquid and solid stimuli in humans. Physiol Behav 99, 317323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
56.Teff, KL, Devine, J & Engelman, K (1995) Sweet taste: effect on cephalic insulin release in men. Physiol Behav 57, 10891095.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
57.Crystal, SR & Teff, KL (2006) Tasting fat: Cephalic phase hormonal responses and food intake in restrained and unrestrained eaters. Physiol Behav 89, 213220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
58.Heath, RB, Jones, R, Frayn, KN et al. (2004) Vagal stimulation exaggerates the inhibitory ghrelin response to oral fat in humans. J Endocrinol 180, 273281.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
59.Brunstrom, JM, Shakeshaft, NG & Scott-Samuel, NE (2008) Measuring ‘expected satiety’ in a range of common foods using a methods of constant stimuli. Appetite 51, 604614.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
60.Wilkinson, LL & Brunstrom, JM (2009) Conditioning ‘fullness expectations’ in a novel dessert. Appetite 52, 780783.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
61.Smeets, PA, de Graaf, C, Stafleu, A et al. (2005) Functional MRI of human hypothalamic responses following glucose ingestion. Neuroimage 24, 363368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
62.Smeets, PA, de Graaf, C, Stafleu, A et al. (2005) Functional magnetic resonance imaging of human hypothalamic responses to sweet taste and calories. Am J Clin Nutr 82, 10111016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
63.Smeets, AJ, Lejeune, MP & Westerterp-Plantenga, MS (2009) Effects of oral fat perception by modified sham feeding on energy expenditure, hormones and appetite profiles in the postprandial state. Brit J Nutr 101, 13601368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
64.Smeets, AJPG & Westerterp-Plantenga, MS (2006) Satiety and substrate mobilization after oral fat stimulation. Br J Nutr 95, 795801.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
65.Kokkinos, A, Le Roux, CW, Alexiadou, K et al. (2010) Eating slowly increases the postprandial response of the anorexigenic gut hormones, peptide YY and glucagon-like peptide-1. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 95, 333337.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
66.Cecil, JE, Francis, J & Read, NW (1998) Relative contributions of intestinal, gastric, oro-sensory influences and information to changes in appetite induced by the same liquid meal. Appetite 31, 377390.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
67.Cecil, JE, Francis, J & Read, NW (1999) Comparison of the effects of a high-fat and high-carbohydrate soup delivered orally and intragastrically on gastric emptying, appetite, and eating behavior. Physiol Behav 67, 299306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
68.Lavin, JH, French, SJ & Read, NW (2002) Comparison of oral and gastric administration of sucrose and maltose on gastric emptying rate and appetite. Int J Obes 26, 8086.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
69.Almiron-Roig, E, Chen, Y & Drewnowski, A (2003) Liquid calories and the failure of satiety: how good is the evidence? Obes Rev 4, 201212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
70.Drewnowski, A & Bellisle, F (2007) Liquid calories, sugar and body weight. Am J Clin Nutr 85, 651661.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
71.Dietary Guidelines Advisory Committee (2010) Report of the dietary guidelines advisory committee on the dietary guidelines for Americans, 2010: Appendix E-1: Major conclusions. USDA Center for Nutrition Policy and Promotion.Google Scholar
72.Shinozaki, K, Shimizu, Y, Shiina, T et al. (2008) Relationship between taste-induced physiological reflexes and temperature of sweet taste. Physiol Behav 93, 10001004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
73.Talavera, K, Yasumatsu, K, Voets, T et al. (2005) Heat activation of TRPM5 underlies thermal sensitivity of sweet taste. Nature 438, 10221025.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
74.Green, BG & Frankmann, SP (1987) The effects of cooling the tongue in the perceived intensity of taste. Chem Senses 12, 609619.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
75.de Graaf, C & Kok, FJ (2010) Nat Rev Endocrinol 6, 290293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Factors effecting eating behaviour. Sensory processes determine what we eat, and are also responsible for variety in the diet. Metabolic processes determine how much we eat. In the brain, sensory signals during eating are linked to the metabolic consequences. These (largely unconscious) learning processes shape our nutrition pattern. The soft-background of foods represents our current-day food environment (Source: de Graaf and Kok(75)).

Figure 1

Fig. 2. Eating rate (g/min) of forty-five food products across the domain of our food supply. Eating rate was assessed by asking subjects to consume 50 g of each product and recording the time (seconds) necessary to ingest this 50 g (Source: Viskaal-van Dongen M, Kok FJ & de Graaf C (2011) Appetite (In the Press)).

Figure 2

Fig. 3. (a) Ad libitum intake (g, with standard deviation) and energy intake (kJ) of the liquid, semi-liquid and semi-solid chocolate dairy products in a cinema setting (n 108; within subjects). Test products were similar in palatability, energy and macronutrient content, and consumed through a straw from 1·5 litre carton boxes over a period of about 90 min. Intake from the semi-solid was 30% less than from the liquid test-food. (b) Ad libitum intake (g, with standard deviation) and energy intake (kJ) of the liquid and semi-solid chocolate dairy product in a laboratory setting (sensory cabin) (n 49; within subjects) within a time frame of about 15 min. In the free eating rate, different effort, subjects consumed the products in the same way as in the cinema setting. In the free eating rate, no effort, subjects consumed the product from a tube making use of a peristaltic pump; in this condition subjects could adjust the rate of delivery. In the fixed eating rate, no effort condition, investigators set the rate of delivery of the test-products into the mouth of the subjects. (Source: Zijlstra et al.(42)).