Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T13:45:05.223Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Do sleep disruptions promote social fragmentation?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 May 2023

John Holbein*
Affiliation:
University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA, USA
Charles Crabtree
Affiliation:
Dartmouth College, Hanover, NH, USA
*
Corresponding author: John Holbein; Email: [email protected]

Abstract

Sleep changes predate shifts in mood/affect, thought processing, mental and physical health, civic engagement, and contextual circumstances, among other things. Theory predicts that these changes may lead to shifts in political and social beliefs. Do sleep disruptions shape how individuals see the world, the people around them, and themselves in relation to others? In this article, we use daily survey data from the 77 waves (N $ \approx $ 460,000) of the University of California, Los Angeles’s 2019–2021 Nationscape Survey—a nationally representative political survey—to examine the effect of an exogenous short-term sleep disruption on measures of political views, polarization, and discriminatory beliefs. Using this data set, we leverage the modest sleep disruption that occurs at the start (and end) of Daylight Saving Time (DST) and employ a regression discontinuity in time design around the precise DST cutoff (which we supplement with event study models). Despite strong theoretical expectations and correlational connection between measures of sleep and many outcomes related to social fragmentation, we find that the DST change has little to no causal effect on citizens’ levels of polarization or their discriminatory attitudes. These effects are precise enough to rule out small effects, robust to a host of specification checks, and consistent across potential subgroups of interest. Our work adds to a small but growing body of research on the social and political effects of sleep disruptions.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the Association for Politics and the Life Sciences

Introduction

All humans need sleep. At its biological roots, sleep is a state of unconsciousness that is governed by neurotransmitters in our brains and required for basic human functioning (Crick & Mitchison, Reference Crick and Mitchison1983; Huber et al., Reference Huber, Felice Ghilardi, Massimini and Tononi2004; Saper et al., Reference Saper, Scammell and Lu2005; Tobler et al., Reference Tobler, Gaus, Deboer, Achermann, Fischer, Rülicke, Moser, Oesch, McBride and Manson1996). As with all important constructs, different definitions of sleep exist (Cleveland Clinic, n.d.; National Institutes of Health, n.d.). Why we sleep, specifically—whether for reasons articulated by inactivity theory, energy conservation theory, restorative theory, or brain plasticity theory (Frank, Reference Frank2006; Porkka-Heiskanen, Reference Porkka-Heiskanen1999; Siegel, Reference Siegel2005)—is disputed. What is not disputed, though, is that this behavior is necessary at regular, repeated intervals. Sleep finds itself in a core group of behaviors necessary for survival, alongside things like eating, drinking, and breathing. The scientific importance of studying sleep is evidenced by the awarding of the Nobel Prize in Physiology and Medicine in 2017 for research on circadian rhythms and sleep patterns (The Nobel Prize, 2017). Sleep problems are especially important today because many people in industrialized nations struggle to get enough sleep; for example, some estimates suggest that between 50 million and 70 million Americans suffer from chronic sleep deprivation every year (National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute, 2022).Footnote 1 (Sleep issues are not restricted to the United States; see, e.g., Hafner et al., Reference Hafner, Stepanek, Taylor, Troxel and Van Stolk2017.)

A large and rapidly growing literature suggests that sleep levels and quality influence virtually every aspect of our modern lives—from how we feel to how we spend our time. Specifically, prior research shows that people who get insufficient sleep or whose sleep patterns are disrupted in negative ways have worse moods (Abele et al., Reference Abele, Gendofla and Petzold1998; Haack & Mullington, Reference Haack and Mullington2005; Hansen et al., Reference Hansen, Sonderskov, Hageman, Dinesen and Ostergaard2017; Heboyan et al., Reference Heboyan, Stevens and McCall2019; Kuehnle & Wunder, Reference Kuehnle and Wunder2016; Vandekerckhove et al., Reference Vandekerckhove, Weiss, Schotte, Exadaktylos, Haex, Verbraecken and Cluydts2011), more mental and physical health challenges (Dickinson et al., Reference Dickinson, Wolkow, Rajaratnam and Drummond2018; Giuntella & Mazzonna, Reference Giuntella and Mazzonna2015; Gu et al., Reference Gu, Xu, Devesa, Zhang, Klerman, Graubard and Caporaso2017; Hansen et al., Reference Hansen, Sonderskov, Hageman, Dinesen and Ostergaard2017; Heissel et al., Reference Heissel, Levy and Adam2017; Horne, Reference Horne1993; Kolla et al., Reference Kolla, Coombes, Morgenthaler and Mansukhani2021; Koopman et al., Reference Koopman, Rauh, t Riet, Groeneveld, Van Der Heijden, Elders, Dekker, Nijpels, Beulens and Rutters2017; Roenneberg et al., Reference Roenneberg, Allebrandt, Merrow and Vetter2012; Wong et al., Reference Wong, Hasler, Kamarck, Muldoon and Manuck2015), lower productivity (Giuntella & Mazzonna, Reference Giuntella and Mazzonna2019; Jiddou et al., Reference Jiddou, Pica, Boura, Qu and Franklin2013), and hindered cognitive capacity (Chee & Chuah, Reference Chee and Chuah2008; Dickinson & McElroy, Reference Dickinson and McElroy2010; Giuntella et al., Reference Giuntella, Han and Mazzonna2017; Heissel et al., Reference Heissel, Levy and Adam2017; Heissel & Norris, Reference Heissel and Norris2018; Lim & Dinges, Reference Lim and Dinges2010; Muzur et al., Reference Muzur, Pace-Schott and Hobson2002; Stickgold, Reference Stickgold2005; Wagner et al., Reference Wagner, Gais, Haider and Born2004). These individual changes also seem to create macro-level effects, as communities that sleep less have worse economic performance (Berument et al., Reference Berument, Dogan and Onar2010; Giuntella & Mazzonna, Reference Giuntella and Mazzonna2019), higher crime (Doleac & Sanders, Reference Doleac and Sanders2015; Domínguez & Asahi, Reference Domínguez and Asahi2019), lower student test scores (Giuntella et al., Reference Giuntella, Han and Mazzonna2017; Heissel et al., Reference Heissel, Levy and Adam2017; Heissel & Norris, Reference Heissel and Norris2018), and lower participation in democratic elections (Holbein et al., Reference Holbein, Schafer and Dickinson2019; Potoski & Urbatsch, Reference Potoski and Urbatsch2017; Schafer & Holbein, Reference Schafer and Holbein2020; Urbatsch, Reference Urbatsch2014), to name a few effects (Bliwise, Reference Bliwise2008).

In addition to sleep levels, sleep disruptions—the focus of this article—also seem to be important.Footnote 2 Specifically, it appears that both short-term (which we leverage in this article) and longer-term sleep disruptions matter. For example, the relatively short-term disruptions that happen when travelers are jet-lagged (Foster et al., Reference Foster, Peirson, Wulff, Winnebeck, Vetter and Roenneberg2013; Levandovski et al., Reference Levandovski, Dantas, Fernandes, Caumo, Torres, Roenneberg, Hidalgo and Allebrandt2011; Taillard et al., Reference Taillard, Sagaspe, Philip and Bioulac2021) or when time changes occur in the spring and fall (Berument et al., Reference Berument, Dogan and Onar2010; Hansen et al., Reference Hansen, Sonderskov, Hageman, Dinesen and Ostergaard2017; Heboyan et al., Reference Heboyan, Stevens and McCall2019; Jiddou et al., Reference Jiddou, Pica, Boura, Qu and Franklin2013; Kolla et al., Reference Kolla, Coombes, Morgenthaler and Mansukhani2021; Kountouris, Reference Kountouris2021; Kuehnle & Wunder, Reference Kuehnle and Wunder2016; Laliotis et al., Reference Laliotis, Moscelli and Monastiriotis2019; Munyo, Reference Munyo2018; Rivers, Reference Rivers2018; Roenneberg et al., Reference Roenneberg, Winnebeck and Klerman2019; Sexton & Beatty, Reference Sexton and Beatty2014; Smith, Reference Smith2016) and longer-term disruptions such as living on the eastern side of time-zone boundaries, which interrupts normal circadian rhythms (Giuntella et al., Reference Giuntella, Han and Mazzonna2017; Gu et al., Reference Gu, Xu, Devesa, Zhang, Klerman, Graubard and Caporaso2017; Holbein et al., Reference Holbein, Schafer and Dickinson2019; Roenneberg et al., Reference Roenneberg, Winnebeck and Klerman2019; Schafer & Holbein, Reference Schafer and Holbein2020), both appear to have important consequences (Roenneberg, Reference Roenneberg2013; Sadeh et al., Reference Sadeh, Raviv and Gruber2000; Tikotzky & Sadeh, Reference Tikotzky and Sadeh2001).

What previous research has not told us, though, is whether sleep disruptions of these varieties (and their downstream consequences) will drive society to continue to fracture along important fault lines. With societal cleavages seeming to be at all-time highs (Cramer, Reference Cramer2020; Iyengar et al., Reference Iyengar, Lelkes, Levendusky, Malhotra and Westwood2019), macro-level trends toward worse sleep potentially overlapping with rising trends toward more division, and preliminary work linking sleep with the breakdown of societal norms and connections, the question remains: does disrupted sleep play a role in causing the social fragmentation we see?

Unfortunately, few researchers have tried to answer this question directly. We have little knowledge about whether sleep-disrupted individuals see the world around them and orient themselves within it differently. People who have disrupted sleep are less likely to engage with the world around them in political and civic domains—that is, the literature shows that a lack of sleep influences social engagement levels (Anderson & Dickinson, Reference Anderson and Dickinson2010; Holbein et al., Reference Holbein, Schafer and Dickinson2019; Potoski & Urbatsch, Reference Potoski and Urbatsch2017; Schafer & Holbein, Reference Schafer and Holbein2020). However, we do not know the extent to which sleep disruptions influence how individuals engage with the world around them and the people in it, nor do we know whether sleep disruptions affect how individuals position themselves in that world. Although changes to mood, mental and physicial health, productivity, thought processing, and social context cumulatively come together to predict an effect on social and political views, little empirical research has examined these links directly. Within that literature, rigorous quantitative study is the exception rather than the rule—after all, much of the research in this space is correlational in nature—as it is based on conditional-on-observables research designs. Moreover, much of the research on the effects of sleep and sleep disruptions comes from small and nonrepresentative samples. Therefore, we do not yet know whether sleep disruptions causally influence the general public’s political and social beliefs, including all-important levels of polarization and discrimination.

Do changes to sleep also shape how people see the world, the people around them, and themselves in relation to those groups? To explore this question, we use a uniquely large and broad data set—the Nationscape Survey (NS) conducted by the University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA). The NS is a rolling, nationally representative political survey conducted during 2019–2021. This data set measures a range of outcomes that are theoretically connected to sleep (as we describe in the next section), such as measures of citizens’ polarization—that is, their ideology, policy positions, evaluations of candidate performance, and evaluations of the out-party—and discrimination—that is, racial resentment, gender resentment, and evaluations of many racial/ethnic/religious/social majority and minority groups, including feelings toward Black people, White people (and White males specifically), Latinx people, Asian people, socialists, evangelicals, Muslims, labor unions, the police, undocumented immigrants, LGBT+ people, and Jewish people. In Figure S22 in the Appendix, we display the correlations across individual items analyzed here. The matrix reveals two broader constructs that are related to social fragmentation: those that involve political attitudes and those that revolve around broader social attitudes. For ease of interpretation, we label these two dimensions polarization and discrimination. We examine the individual measures in order to respect the idiosyncrasies of individual relationships between outcomes, but the results do not change if we construct two scales. All of these outcomes are theoretically connected to sleep disruptions (as we describe in the next section). We examine the entire range of outcomes—instead of just one or two—both because doing so (1) helps us make our analysis more complete and (2) serves as a check against selective reporting of results, which can be the result of HARKing (hypothesizing after the results are known; see Kerr, Reference Kerr1998) and lead to publication bias (Easterbrook et al., Reference Easterbrook, Gopalan, Berlin and Matthews1991; Franco et al., Reference Franco, Malhotra and Simonovits2014; Gerber & Malhotra, Reference Gerber and Malhotra2008; Lakens, Reference Lakens2015; Open Science Collaboration, 2015).

To understand the effect of sleep disruptions on these outcomes, we leverage the (modest) sleep shock that occurs at the start (and end) of Daylight Saving Time (DST). This article employs a regression discontinuity in time design (RDiT). Regression discontinuity designs, in general, allow researchers to make causal inferences with modest assumptions (as we describe later). The RDiT employed here specifically takes advantage of the daily measures in the NS data and the fact that the shift to DST in the spring is exogenous within a limited temporal range. As a robustness check, we test for any effects that arise in the transition from DST to Standard Time that occurs in the fall; this is not our main specification, however, given that prior research shows inconsistent effects of “falling back” on sleep. Given the close overlap with the election that occurred in 2020—which is not a problem in the spring period—we look for effects at both the 2019 and 2020 fall cutoffs.Footnote 3 (There is no NS data for the spring change to DST in 2021.) We also explore the possible heteterogeneous effects of sleep by survey respondents’ political party, race, gender, religion, age, education, and income.

We find that despite strong theoretical expectations, DST-induced sleep changes have little to no effect on citizens’ attitudes about the political system or the world around them. Specifically, the DST shock does not systematically affect or polarize political outcomes such as citizens’ ideology, policy positions, evaluations of the performance of politicians, or evaluations of people who belong to the other political party. It also does not affect their discriminatory beliefs as captured in measures such as racial/gender resentment and discriminatory attitudes towards racial/ethnic/religious/social out-groups. Using equivalence testing, we show that the effects we observe are precise enough to rule out very small effects. The effects are also consistent across a host of reasonable specification and robustness checks, including the use of the fall time change instead of the spring, a donut RDiT that adjusts for any precise manipulation around the cutoff, the inclusion of day-of-the-week fixed effects, conceptualizing treatment as the Monday after DST, and running event study models that more flexibly examine the evolution of effects pre- and post-treatment. Our results are consistently present across all of the outcomes tested, even when similar items are scaled together to reduce residual variance. The null effects are consistent—being present across different outcomes, the political spectrum, and across various racial/ethnic groups, age, gender, religious, education, and income groups. Our null effects are highly precise. This is because our study is very well powered—with nearly a half million observations overall and approximately 40,000 observations for most of our outcomes within a month of the time change cutoff. This means that our effects are both very precisely estimated and highly unlikely to be the result of Type M (magnitude) and S (sign) errors (Gelman & Carlin, Reference Gelman and Carlin2014).

In short, our results suggest that while sleep disruptions of the variety that we can readily observe and study affect a host of attitudes and behaviors—including, but not limited to, mood, stress, cognitive processes, physical activity, health, crime, civic engagement, and economic behaviors—these effects do not drive social fragmentation. Sleep disruptions—at least of the modest variety that we explore here—do not influence individuals’ core social and political beliefs about the world, the people around them, how well leaders of import are performing, or their views on how various social problems should be solved.

These precise null effects are an important contribution. The broader (non)consequences of sleep disruptions are vital to understand given the core role that sleep plays. We need to consider not only what sleep disruptions do influence, but also what they do not influence, as this can (1) help researchers better calibrate theoretical models about the role that sleep (and sleep disruption) plays in human behavior, (2) inform future research, and (3) help contribute to policy discussions about the benefits and the nonbenefits of public and private interventions to improve the quality and quantity of sleep. Our work also contributes to an active debate on the merits of changes to DST; for example, in March 2023, the U.S. Senate passied legislation to shift time changes from the status quo by making DST permanent. Beyond these contributions to the study of sleep and its effects, our research also provides empirical findings that should interest many scholars studying discrimination (Cramer, Reference Cramer2020) and political polarization (Iyengar et al., Reference Iyengar, Lelkes, Levendusky, Malhotra and Westwood2019). Our research helps push forward our understanding of what does and what does not influence two key drivers of social fragmentation.

Background and conceptual framework

To better understand the causal effect of disruptions to sleep, we leverage the (modest) exogenous shock to sleep that occurs around changes to and from DST. Changes to and from DST in the spring and fall (respectively) represent a shock to sleep that is exogenous (i.e., orthogonal) to many other factors thought to drive the outcomes we examine (Kamstra et al., Reference Kamstra, Kramer and Levi2000). Changes to and from DST occur in the middle of the night and are, functionally, about changing the hours that people have available in their restful versus their waking states. Time changes also potentially disrupt normal sleeping patterns. Importantly for our identification strategy, shifting the periods of light and dark (arguably) gets as close to a random shift to individual sleep patterns as is possible in the real world.Footnote 4

Consistent with this conceptualization, prior research has shown that humans are most vulnerable to sleep deprivation in early March, as they transition from Standard Time to DST. People lose one hour of time in the spring by setting their clocks forward in the early hours of the morning. This shift forward means that there will be more light later into the evening, thus presenting a disruption to individuals’ circadian rhythms (Roenneberg et al., Reference Roenneberg, Winnebeck and Klerman2019). This shock represents a modest change to sleep patterns. While an hour is lost, there is evidence that people exhibit some compensating behavior around DST changes (Sexton & Beatty, Reference Sexton and Beatty2014). Still, previous research suggests that the spring transition from Standard Time to DST results in up to 40 minutes less sleep (Barnes & Wagner, Reference Barnes and Wagner2009; Sexton & Beatty, Reference Sexton and Beatty2014). This effect represents upwards of two-thirds of the total time lost in the spring transition. The relationship between sleep and the fall change back to Standard Time is more complex. Summarizing prior work, the Sleep Foundation puts it thus: “Major sleep disruptions are less likely to occur in November when DST ends and Standard Time begins. In fact, gaining an extra hour of sleep often leaves people feeling more refreshed following the end of DST. However, people may experience some moderate [negative sleep] effects” (Pacheco, Reference Pacheco2020). (This fact and the close overlap between “fall back” and elections are the reasons why we leverage spring changes to DST as our primary identification strategy and fall changes only as a robustness check.)

Outcomes and their theoretical connection to sleep disruptions

There are multiple reasons to expect that people who experience a sleep disruption of the variety that we study here may exhibit attitudes that drive social fragmentation. This may show up in their different political and social attitudes, and it may be seen in enhanced political polarization and heightened levels of discrimination. Specifically, in this article, we examine people’s attitudinal patterns in terms of their political ideology, policy positions, evaluation of political leaders (belonging to both in- and out-groups), and views toward in- and out-parties. We also examine individuals’ discriminatory attitudes toward racial/ethnic/religious/social majority and minority groups. Each of these outcomes has a strong theoretical connection to sleep disruptions. We describe how prior research and theory connect sleep disruptions with the outcomes that we explore. (In the next section, we outline how each of our outcomes is measured.)

Our outcomes have a similar set of mechanisms in play, which include propensity to participate, mood/affect, personality characteristics, stress/threat, thought processes, and social context. The mechanisms here are likely many and complex; therefore, precisely identifying exact mechanisms is inherently difficult (Green et al., Reference Green, Ha and Bullock2010). It is not our intention to tease apart these individual mechanisms here. Rather, our goal is to lay the theoretical justification for examining sleep disruptions and people’s views about the world around them, their fellow citizens, and themselves. The many mechanisms at play here justify examination, but these mechanisms cannot be easily disentangled.

We test for treatment effect heterogeneity across several dimensions, such as race, party, gender, religion, age, education, and income, as these are likely strong predictors of our outcomes of interest. Looking at effects by political party, in particular, allows us to see whether DST promotes political polarization. However, the other dimensions also allow us to see whether sleep disruption fragments society along other potential cleavages. Moreover, we note that some of the outcomes we examine are likely more malleable than others. Some attitudes harden as people age; hence, in the analyses that follow, one of the checks we also run is to see whether the effects of DST vary by age.

Sleep disruptions and political positions/political polarization

There are several reasons why sleep disruptions could affect individuals’ political views—where people stand on specific policy issues, where they see themselves ideologically, how they evaluate political leaders, and how they approach individuals in their own political party and the opposing party. These mechanisms include: first, increases in engagement levels and all of the experiences that follow this change; second, changes to psychological beliefs; and third, shifts in contextual circumstances.

First, prior research has shown that sleep shocks influence levels of engagement in politics (Holbein et al., Reference Holbein, Schafer and Dickinson2019; Ksiazkiewicz & Erol, Reference Ksiazkiewicz and Erol2022b; Potoski & Urbatsch, Reference Potoski and Urbatsch2017; Schafer & Holbein, Reference Schafer and Holbein2020). For example, Urbatsch (Reference Urbatsch2014) leverages the DST transition in the fall to examine turnout in U.S. presidential elections and finds that longer sleep in the fall increases turnout. This shift in participation results in changes to vote shares in elections (Schafer & Holbein, Reference Schafer and Holbein2020); however, it could also result in attitudinal changes that predate partisan voting. Those who participate in politics enter into an arena that is highly politicized and polarized. Exposure to this environment may be transformative. Participation, then, is only one logical step away from the direction of said participation; after all, those who participate actively tend to be stronger in their ideological/policy views (Bartels, Reference Bartels2000). If this relationship is causal, a shock to an individual’s participation levels may change their political beliefs. Being exposed to or removed from political systems, and everything that comes with those changes (e.g., changes to information streams, social networks, etc.), may spur people’s political views to change as well.Footnote 5

Second, it is possible that people’s political positions change when they experience the psychological changes that follow sleep disruptions. Prior research has shown that sleep disruptions decrease individual mood/affect (Haack & Mullington, Reference Haack and Mullington2005; Hansen et al., Reference Hansen, Sonderskov, Hageman, Dinesen and Ostergaard2017; Heboyan et al., Reference Heboyan, Stevens and McCall2019; Kuehnle & Wunder, Reference Kuehnle and Wunder2016; Vandekerckhove et al., Reference Vandekerckhove, Weiss, Schotte, Exadaktylos, Haex, Verbraecken and Cluydts2011), spark changes to personality characteristics (Anderson & Dickinson, Reference Anderson and Dickinson2010; Castillo et al., Reference Castillo, Dickinson and Petrie2017; Dickinson & McElroy, Reference Dickinson and McElroy2017; McKenna et al., Reference McKenna, Dickinson, Orff and Drummond2007), catalyze changes in thought processes (Dickinson & McElroy, Reference Dickinson and McElroy2010; Heissel et al., Reference Heissel, Levy and Adam2017; Heissel & Norris, Reference Heissel and Norris2018; Lim & Dinges, Reference Lim and Dinges2010), and lead to a heightened sense of social threat (Dahl & Lewin, Reference Dahl and Lewin2002; Prather et al., Reference Prather, Bogdan and Hariri2013; Sadeh, Reference Sadeh1996; Straus et al., Reference Straus, Dolsen, Nishimi, Neylan and O’Donovan2022; Tashjian and Galván, Reference Tashjian and Galván2020). This research on the psychological effects of sleep disruptions can be married with studies showing that mood/affect (Bassi, Reference Bassi2013; Bernardi et al., Reference Bernardi, Mattila, Papageorgiou and Rapeli2023; Ojeda, Reference Ojeda2015), personality characteristics (Dahl & Lewin, Reference Dahl and Lewin2002; Gerber et al., Reference Gerber, Huber, Doherty and Dowling2011; Mondak, Reference Mondak2010; Verhulst et al., Reference Verhulst, Hatemi and Martin2010), thought processes (Hodson & Busseri, Reference Hodson and Busseri2012; Onraet et al., Reference Onraet, Van Hiel, Dhont, Hodson, Schittekatte and De Pauw2015; Zmigrod et al., Reference Zmigrod, Rentfrow and Robbins2020), health (Dickinson et al., Reference Dickinson, Wolkow, Rajaratnam and Drummond2018; Giuntella & Mazzonna, Reference Giuntella and Mazzonna2015; Gu et al., Reference Gu, Xu, Devesa, Zhang, Klerman, Graubard and Caporaso2017; Hansen et al., Reference Hansen, Sonderskov, Hageman, Dinesen and Ostergaard2017; Heissel et al., Reference Heissel, Levy and Adam2017; Horne, Reference Horne1993; Kolla et al., Reference Kolla, Coombes, Morgenthaler and Mansukhani2021; Koopman et al., Reference Koopman, Rauh, t Riet, Groeneveld, Van Der Heijden, Elders, Dekker, Nijpels, Beulens and Rutters2017; Roenneberg et al., Reference Roenneberg, Allebrandt, Merrow and Vetter2012; Wong et al., Reference Wong, Hasler, Kamarck, Muldoon and Manuck2015), and threat perceptions (Enos, Reference Enos2016; Giles & Hertz, Reference Giles and Hertz1994; Homola, Reference Homola2021; Keen & Jacobs, Reference Keen and Jacobs2009; Malhotra & Popp, Reference Malhotra and Popp2012; Reny & Newman, Reference Reny and Newman2018) may shape political preferences. (We note that much of this research in political science relies on conditional-on-observables research designs, leaving us uncertain whether these forces actually cause changes in political beliefs. We return to this point in the Discussion.) This link makes it easy to imagine how a shock to sleep might change political beliefs. For example, people who have a more negative mood may evaluate members of the political out-group more negatively. Similarly, prior work has shown that sleep disruption/deprivation increases the odds that a person has a major depressive episode (e.g., Boakye et al., Reference Boakye, Olechowski, Rashiq, Verrier, Kerr, Witmans, Baker, Joyce and Dick2016). And depression correlates (conditional-on-observables) with political beliefs and actions (Ojeda, Reference Ojeda2015). Prior research has also shown that sleep disruption/deprivation shapes individuals’ risk preferences (Castillo et al., Reference Castillo, Dickinson and Petrie2017; McKenna et al., Reference McKenna, Dickinson, Orff and Drummond2007), which have also been linked with political attitudes and behaviors (Fowler, Reference Fowler2006; Milosh et al., Reference Milosh, Painter, Sonin, Van Dijcke and Wright2021).

Related to this line of research is a body of work that relates individual chronotype—a person’s time-of-sleep preference, or where a person falls in terms of “morningness preference (early to bed, early to rise) [or] eveningness preference (late to bed, late to rise)” (Ksiazkiewicz, Reference Ksiazkiewicz2020, p. 367)—to political views (Ksiazkiewicz, Reference Ksiazkiewicz2022; Ksiazkiewicz & Erol, Reference Ksiazkiewicz and Erol2022b). For example, using seven American samples and one British sample, Ksiazkiewicz (Reference Ksiazkiewicz2020) shows that morningness predicts conservatism. This relationship is “robust to controls for openness, conscientiousness, and demographics, including age, sex, income, and education” (Ksiazkiewicz, Reference Ksiazkiewicz2020, p. 367). Likewise, in a study of representative samples from 10 countries, Ksiazkiewicz and Erol (Reference Ksiazkiewicz and Erol2022a) show that in all contexts, chronotype predicts ideology, with a majority of contexts pointing toward morning chronotype having significant links with political conservatism. Chronotype might also contribute to the development of individual personality traits (e.g., Jonason et al., Reference Jonason, Jones and Lyons2013; Randler et al., Reference Randler, Schredl and Göritz2017; You et al., Reference You, Laborde, Dosseville, Salinas and Allen2020), which, in turn, contribute to political views (Gerber et al., Reference Gerber, Huber, Doherty and Dowling2011).

Finally, it is possible that political views are changed—even if only for a short period— by the fluctuations in social context that occur when a society experiences a collective disruption of sleep. Prior research has shown that when a community experiences a sleep shock, crime increases (Doleac & Sanders, Reference Doleac and Sanders2015; Munyo, Reference Munyo2018), traffic accidents (perhaps) increase (Laliotis et al., Reference Laliotis, Moscelli and Monastiriotis2019; Smith, Reference Smith2016; Varughese & Allen, Reference Varughese and Allen2001), economic performance decreases (Giuntella & Mazzonna, Reference Giuntella and Mazzonna2019), as do educational outcomes (Giuntella et al., Reference Giuntella, Han and Mazzonna2017; Heissel et al., Reference Heissel, Levy and Adam2017; Heissel & Norris, Reference Heissel and Norris2018). Changing the world in such a fundamental and multifaceted way may combine to prompt individuals to change their political views. A long literature in political science shows that when people perceive negative performance shocks—even if they are not real—to the overall economy (Fiorina, Reference Fiorina1978; Healy & Lenz, Reference Healy and Lenz2014, Reference Healy and Lenz2017), to nearby schools (Berry & Howell, Reference Berry and Howell2007; Holbein, Reference Holbein2016; Holbein & Hassell, Reference Holbein and Hassell2019), or to crime or safety (Arnold & Carnes, Reference Arnold and Carnes2012; Bateson, Reference Bateson2012), their political beliefs and actions may change. This literature on “retrospective voting” (Healy & Malhotra, Reference Healy and Malhotra2013) would lead us to expect that shared sleep-induced declines—like those that occur around changes to DST—may result in reduced societal well-being, and this would lead individuals to evaluate the political environment—for example, the leaders who govern it—differently.

Finally, we note that the direction of any political shift is ambiguous. It is possible that enhanced participation, changes to psychological attachments, and a shift in social context push people to be more conservative or more liberal. There may be polarizing effects here, too, whereby people who experience a sleep shock double down on their beliefs. On the flip side, it could be the case that people who are sleep disrupted are more tired and, as a result, more likely to acquiesce to the viewpoints of those on the opposing political team. While the direction of any political preference changes is uncertain, this much is clear: there are strong reasons to suspect that sleep disruptions (even if they are brief) may elicit changes (even if they are brief) to political positions, whether it be a uniform or a polarizing shift.

Sleep disruptions and discriminatory attitudes

A long literature has theorized that changes to sleep could enhance discriminatory attitudes and behaviors. Some have argued that sleep disruptions cause individuals to turn inward and focus on their own needs at the expense of others (Holbein et al., Reference Holbein, Schafer and Dickinson2019). Moreover, those who lose sleep may experience a threat-like state (Dahl & Lewin, Reference Dahl and Lewin2002; Prather et al., Reference Prather, Bogdan and Hariri2013; Sadeh, Reference Sadeh1996; Straus et al., Reference Straus, Dolsen, Nishimi, Neylan and O’Donovan2022; Tashjian & Galván, Reference Tashjian and Galván2020), which may lead individuals to view out-groups more negatively (Enos, Reference Enos2016; Giles & Hertz, Reference Giles and Hertz1994; Homola, Reference Homola2021; Keen & Jacobs, Reference Keen and Jacobs2009; Malhotra & Popp, Reference Malhotra and Popp2012; Reny & Newman, Reference Reny and Newman2018). Individuals who are sleep disrupted may also be less cooperative with others generally (Anderson & Dickinson, Reference Anderson and Dickinson2010). In addition, individuals who lack sleep may also lack the inhibitory control that causes people to self-filter; therefore, sleep may reduce the social desirability bias of respondents. When they are tired, respondents may answer questions probing their discriminatory beliefs more truthfully because they lack a strong cognitive filter to keep socially unacceptable biases at bay.

Consistent with this theoretical connection, evidence from conditional-on-observable research designs documents an association between sleep and discrimination, whereby those who get less sleep are more likely to lash out in discriminatory ways toward other groups. In fact, in a review of 17 studies on the topic of discrimination and sleep, Slopen et al. (Reference Slopen, Lewis and Williams2016) show that all of their studies document this association between sleep problems and discrimination. However, as Slopen et al. note, the research that has been done in this space has distinct data limitations. One potential limitation of this research is omitted variable bias, which occurs when we cannot rule out other factors that are related to both sleep and outcomes. Another potential limitation relates to endogenity, or the possibility that each factor influences the other. Given this limitation in the empirical record, there is value to examining whether an exogenous shock to sleep levels (even if it is modest in size) enhances discrimination.

It is unclear which groups could face more discrimination as a result of a disruption to sleep patterns. Discrimination or bias could occur on multiple dimensions—on racial, ethnic, religious, or social grounds. Given this ambiguity, it is preferable to examine many potential targets of discrimination (as we do here). Moreover, as with our other outcomes, the relationship could be uniform or polarizing—separating along partisan and/or majority/minority lines.

Data and methods

To examine the effects of DST on our outcomes of interest, we use the Nationscape Survey (NS) conducted by the Democracy Fund and UCLA (Tausanovitch et al., Reference Tausanovitch, Vavreck, Reny, Hayes and Rudkin2019). The NS is a large-scale weekly survey (N = 6,250 per week) that began in late 2019 and ended in early 2021. The survey draws samples from the Lucid platform, which has been shown to benchmark well to population parameters (e.g., Coppock & McClellan, Reference Coppock and McClellan2019; Peyton et al., Reference Peyton, Huber and Coppock2022). The NS is reflective of the national adult population in the United States. It is not a panel data set, but a pooled cross-section. It has been widely used in the political and social sciences (e.g., Naeim et al., Reference Naeim, Baxter-King, Wenger, Stanton, Sepucha and Vavreck2021; Reny & Barreto, Reference Reny and Barreto2022), including in evaluations of the effect of discrete events (like police killings) on citizen attitudes (like attitudes toward the police) using the methods that we employ (an RDiT design; for examples, see Reny, Reference Reny2020; Reny & Newman, Reference Reny and Newman2021). Because the NS was fielded daily, with averages of about N = 900 people responding to the survey per day, we can precisely estimate the effect of DST changes on the outcomes of interest. To do so, we use the full set of 77 waves of the NS—comprising just under a half a million individual responses—and draw specific attention to the changes to DST that overlap with the NS time series.

We evaluate the effect of sleep disruptions on a handful of outcomes related to how individuals view the world around them and their place within it. First, we look at citizen ideology. This is coded on a 1–5 scale ranging from very liberal to very conservative. We standardize this outcome and other outcomes in our analyses that follow.

Second, we have measures of policy positions on a host of policies. Individually, these may not have as strong a connection to sleep disruption; however, collectively, they allow us to see whether the direction of policy preferences moves in response to sleep as theory would predict. We examine support for the following issues: building a border wall, capping carbon emissions, requiring background checks, repealing the estate tax, raising taxes on those earning above $600,000 per year, ensuring that all students can graduate college debt-free, never permitting abortion, allowing late-term abortions, replacing private health insurance, providing a public option for health insurance, subsidizing health insurance, offering a path to citizenship to all undocumented immigrants, providing a path to citizenship for undocumented immigrants brought to the United States as children (so-called Dreamers), deporting all undocumented immigrants, banning all guns, banning assault rifles, and limiting gun magazines to 10 bullets (all are agree/disagree, coded in the conservative policy direction, and not standardized).

Third, we investigate evaluations of the incumbent candidate for president (Donald Trump) and the challenger (Joe Biden) in the 2020 presidential election. This is measured on a 1–4 scale, ranging from very favorable to very unfavorable. Fourth, we examine evaluations of both major political parties. This is measured on a 1–4 scale, ranging from very favorable to very unfavorable. In disaggregating these results by party, we see whether there is any effect of sleep disruptions on a measure of political polarization.

Fifth, we construct racial resentment and gender (women) resentment scales using the various measures for these available in the NS. For racial resentment, the NS asks whether Blacks should do the same as other minority groups without having any special favors, whether generations of slavery and discrimination have created conditions that make it difficult for Blacks to work their way out of the lower class, whether the respondent would prefer that close relatives marry spouses from their same race, and whether the respondent thinks that it is alright for Blacks and Whites to date each other. For gender resentment, the NS asks whether the respondent would be more comfortable having a man as a boss than a woman, whether women are just as capable of thinking logically as men, whether increased opportunities for women have significantly improved the quality of life in the United States, and whether women who complain about harassment often cause more problems than they solve. All of these are measured on a 1–5 scale ranging from strongly agree to strongly disagree. They are rescaled so that they are in the same conceptual direction. Then, they are combined into factor weighted scales using principal factor models.

Finally, we inspect the effect of a sleep disruption on evaluations of various majority/minority groups. All of these questions start with the stem “Here are the names of some groups that are in the news from time to time. How favorable is your impression of each group or haven’t you heard enough to say?” The groups evaluated include Whites, Blacks, Latinos, Asians, evangelicals, socialists, Muslims, labor unions, the police, undocumented immigrants, LGBT+, and White men. All of these are measured on a 1–4 scale from very favorable to very unfavorable, which we standardize in our analyses. These combined with the resentment scales allow us to investigate whether those who experience a sleep disruption are more discriminatory in their stated preferences, on average.

We examine effects among the population as a whole and also among several relevant subgroups. These subgroups include Republicans, Democrats, independents, people who belong to another political party, as well as Black, Asian, and White individuals. We look at race and party because there are reasons to expect heterogeneous effects on beliefs across individuals’ starting political affiliation. We disaggregate by race because there are reasons to expect heterogeneous effects on views toward other racial/ethnic/social majority/minority groups. To be thorough, however, we look for heterogeneous effects across all outcomes and all subgroups.

To identify the causal effect of starting DST on political beliefs, we use an RDiT approach. Regression discontinuity designs (RDDs) leverage as-good-as-random variation and continuity in potential outcomes around an arbitrary cutoff to estimate a causal treatment effect (Calonico et al., Reference Calonico, Cattaneo and Titiunik2014; De la Cuesta & Imai, Reference De la Cuesta and Imai2016; Hahn et al., Reference Hahn, Todd and Van der Klaauw2001; Hausman & Rapson, Reference Hausman and Rapson2018; Imbens & Lemieux, Reference Imbens and Lemieux2008; Lee & Lemieux, Reference Lee and Lemieux2010; Thistlethwaite & Campbell, Reference Thistlethwaite and Campbell1960). RDDs have been shown to benchmark well to randomized control trials (Wing & Cook, Reference Wing and Cook2013).Footnote 6 In all of the RDiT models, we use the standard procedures in the rdrobust package in Calonico et al. (Reference Calonico, Cattaneo and Titiunik2014; CCT), which employs local linear regression to construct the point estimator and local quadratic regression to construct the bias correction and employs CCT’s optimal bandwidths, which are the default option in the rdrobust package (Calonico et al., Reference Calonico, Cattaneo and Titiunik2014). With our primary identification strategy—which leverages the spring 2020 time shock—we run the model estimates across four bandwidths: the CCT optimal bandwidth for our unconstrained time series (which is typically 60 days on either side of the cutoff), the CCT optimal bandwidth restricting our sample to 2020 (which is typically about 28 days), 30 days, and 120 days.

The discontinuity we use is the one that appears in time around the transition from Standard Time to DST in the spring. (As a robustness check, we also look at fall changes. We use this as a robustness check as the sleep disruption in the fall is more modest.) This discontinuity has been used before as it shows good properties for causal identification (Kountouris, Reference Kountouris2021; Kuehnle & Wunder, Reference Kuehnle and Wunder2016; Rivers, Reference Rivers2018). We replicate these validation checks, showing first that there is a lack of substantive bunching around the cutoff. In the NS data, there are slightly more respondents after DST. However, the results hold if we take a a so-called donut RDD approach that accounts for precise sorting around the cutoff (Barreca et al., Reference Barreca, Lindo and Waddell2016). Moreover, we show covariate balance, too (see Figure S1b). Out of the 43 controls tested, 42 allow us to rule out substantively meaningful effects using equivalence testing (Hartman & Hidalgo, Reference Hartman and Hidalgo2018; Lakens et al., Reference Lakens, Scheel and Isager2018). And the (null) results hold if we include controls in our models.

Before we proceed to the results, a word about multiple hypothesis testing and statistical power is in order. Our research question requires that we examine multiple outcomes. Looking at only one social/political outcome would give an incomplete picture of DST’s impact on how people see the world and themselves in it. As has long been established, when scholars examine multiple outcomes, the rules of probability mean that some estimates will be statistically significant simply by chance (Dudoit et al., Reference Dudoit, Shaffer and Boldrick2003). Given the results that we present here—overwhelmingly, precisely-estimated coefficients that are not statistically different from zero—we want to be cautious to not over-interpret the presence of some coefficients that are statistically significant by chance. That said, since we are arguing for the null, using the traditional approaches for correcting for multiple hypothesis testing—be it the Bonferroni method, the Šidák approach, or the free step-down resampling correction (Abdi et al., Reference Abdi and Salkind2007; VanderWeele & Mathur, Reference VanderWeele and Mathur2019; Westfall & Young, Reference Westfall and Young1993)—is actually a less conservative approach. By not employing them, we give the best chance for the null to be rejected. This provides a hard test for the null. In all the null results that we present here, these multiple hypothesis corrections, if applied, would only strengthen the case for the null even more than it is without any corrections.

The NS data also has plenty of statistical power—having tens of thousands of observations for our outcome variables with a bandwidth of one month around the cutoff, for example. This is the case because of the NS’s large repeating cross sections. Even if we consider the data to be nested at the level of the running variable and adjust our standard errors accordingly (Lee & Card, Reference Lee and Card2008), the results are still quite precise. As we show, the nulls are not due to an insufficient sample size or large standard errors that overlap with zero. To show this point, we employ equivalence testing. This approach allows us to pay specific attention to the effect sizes and 95% confidence intervals (Hartman & Hidalgo, Reference Hartman and Hidalgo2018; Lakens et al., Reference Lakens, Scheel and Isager2018). In most applications, we are able to rule out even very modest effects. Additionally, to reduce residual variance, when appropriate (e.g., with conservative policy positions and racial and gender resentment), we create scales in our outcomes. These allow even more precision (Ansolabehere et al., Reference Ansolabehere, Rodden and Snyder2008). In short, the null effects we present are not due to a lack of statistical power, which often mars null effect studies.

Finally, we note that in all the visualizations presented here, we use a common range across the outcome axis for ease of comparing similar outcomes. Moreover, when it is not visually possible to do so, we label the coefficient plots with the important model estimates. Where appropriate—that is, in our standardized outcomes—we also mark an often-used benchmark for equivalence testing (Cohen, Reference Cohen1992). Though this approach is certainly not the only standard for which substantive effects should be judged, it does give us a reference point for what effect sizes can be ruled out. Acknowledging that what constitutes a large or small effect is, to a certain extent, in the eye of the beholder, in all cases we note the effects that can or cannot be ruled out based on the 95% confidence intervals.

Results

Theory and prior research provide strong reasons to expect that sleep disruptions cause people to look at the world around them, the other people in it, and their own place in the world differently. There are reasons to expect that this will lead to social fragmentation. There are likely downstream effects on outcomes such as ideology, policy positions, leadership evaluations, racial/gender resentment, and attitudes toward other minority and majority in- and out-groups. Do sleep disruptions affect these social and political beliefs? The answer, based on our research, appears to be “no”.

To illustrate this, we start with a figure that summarizes the results across all outcomes and subgroups. (The equivalent figure for the fall 2019 change from Standard Time to DST can be found in the Appendix, Figure S11.) Figure 1 displays a summary of 1,152 model results (36 outcomes * 8 groups * 4 specifications). Each cell—marked with either a square or a circle—is a separate RDiT model estimate. Squares are model estimates that allow us to rule out a certain effect size—either 20% (of a standard deviation or probability increase, depending on whether the outcome is dichotomous or standardized) or 10%. The first two panels are from models without covariates; the next two panels include covariates. Shapes are shaded depending on whether the effect is statistically significant at the (unadjusted) 5% level. Black circles are significant effects in which we cannot rule out medium-sized effects (based on Cohen’s rules of thumb). Hence, if DST were changing people’s views in a meaningful way, we would expect to see many black circles in Figure 1.

Figure 1. Summarizing the effect of starting DST on social views. (a) Rule out 20%? (no covariates) (b) Rule out 10%? (no covariates) (c) Rule out 20%? (with covariates) (d) Rule out 10%? (with covariates).

Each cell summarizes an RDiT model specification. Points are shaped by whether we can rule out the effect size specified in each of the panel titles (20% or 10% of a standard deviation); squares can rule out the effects listed in each panel title, whereas circles cannot. Points are shaded by whether they are statistically significant at the (unadjusted) 5% level. Hence, effects that are statistically significant and substantively meaningful are black circles. Takeaway: The general pattern of evidence is precisely estimated nulls. This can be seen in most of the cells being gray squares (i.e., effects where we can rule out meaningful effects and the estimates are not significant at traditional levels), the next most common being black squares (i.e., effects where we can rule out meaningful effects, but that are still statistically distinct from 0), and only a minority being black circles (i.e., effects that are both statistically and substantively meaningful).

In fact, this is not what we find. As Figure 1 shows, only in very rare circumstances are effects statistically significant, substantively large, or too imprecise to not rule out medium-sized effects. In the first panel—no covariates and testing against a 20% effect—94.1% of the effects (271 of 288) allow us to rule out what Cohen calls a medium-sized effect. Among the 17 effects for which we cannot rule out a 20% effect, only 3 (1.04%) are statistically significant at traditional levels. The general finding can be seen in the fact that a dominant majority of the shapes are gray—not significant at traditional (unadjusted) levels—and a dominant majority can rule out effects of very modest size. In short, the general pattern is precisely estimated nulls. This is true regardless of whether we look at a 20% effect or an even smaller 10% effect. Testing against an estimate that is half the size of Cohen’s threshold for a small/medium-sized effect, we would expect that fewer models would have sufficient power to hit this, as it is a particularly harsh equivalence test. Still, even with this high bar, we are able to rule out a 10% effect in 72% of cases. Among those, only 10 (3.5% of all the models) estimates are statistically significant at traditional levels.

When we add covariates, the results become even more precise—as we would expect—and even closer to zero. As a result, fewer of the coefficients show signs of a meaningful effect. In the bottom-left panel, we can rule out 20% effects in 96.2% of models and the more punitive 10% threshold in 75% of cases. Among the remaining coefficients, only 0.69% and 2.08% (respectively) are statistically significant at traditional (unadjusted) levels. Recall from the last section that not adjusting for multiple comparisons across models makes our estimates of the number of effects that are signficant conservative. If we were to apply the Bonferroni, Šidák, or free step-down resampling methodology for correcting for multiple comparisons, even fewer effects would be significant.

Digging into the subgroups shown in Figure 1, we can see whether there is heterogeneity in the treatment effect estimates.Footnote 7 Looking at the first column—that for the entire sample (“all”)—it is clear that there is little to no evidence of systematic effects in the public as a whole, on average. None of the 144 model estimates (36 outcomes * 4 specifications) in the first columns of panels (a)–(d) are significant and all of them can rule out very modest effects—20% or even 10% (i.e., all the shapes are squares). Only when we break our sample apart into individual subgroups do we find significant effects and ones that cannot rule out medium-sized effects. One interesting pattern is that many of the effects that are too imprecise to rule out medium-sized effects (i.e., the gray and black circles) are found in the smallest subsets of the population—those in other political parties or in the Asian/American category. Indeed, among the effects that are too imprecise to rule out medium-sized effects, 15 of 17 in the top left panel are in either the Asian or other party subgroup, with all 11 of these in the third panel being in these two subgroups. That we see significant but less precise results in cases when we have the least statistical power suggests that these exceptional cases might be a product of the noisier estimates that come with less statistical power, rather than evidence for a true effect. This is further buttressed by the fact that adding controls—and the precision that comes with doing so—makes the results even more null.

One thing that Figure 1 does not do is display the size of the effects themselves. Figure 2 provides a summary of the effect sizes. Rather than plot all the effects individually, which would take up too much space, Figure 2 shows the distribution of estimated effects. The effects are broken by whether we have a standardized (top row) or a binary (bottom row) outcome. The columns differ in whether we are looking at the effect of shifting from Standard to DST in the spring (left column) or DST to Standard Time in the fall (right column). As can be seen, most of the estimated effects are quite small—falling within the range of what most standards would conceive to be small effects. Indeed, the average/median effect falls close to zero in all cases, with each of the spring distributions being even more spiked at zero than what we would see with a normal distribution—that is, there are even more effects closer to zero. The fall distributions look quite normally distributed around zero—meaning that there are roughly the same numbers of negative and positive effects. As we would expect, the effects are somewhat noisier when we look at binary outcomes. Not surprisingly, the fall treatment estimates (perhaps being confounded with the election itself) are also noisier than the spring. However, still, most of the effects themselves are small and consistent with a small, rather than meaningful effect size in virtually all cases. The equivalent figure for the fall 2019 time change can be found in the second row of Figure S11 in the Appendix. It largely shows null effects, too.

Figure 2. Distribution of effects at the spring and fall cutoffs. (a) Distribution of effect sizes, spring (b) Distribution of effect sizes, fall (c) Distribution of effects on binary outcomes, spring (d) Distribution of effects on binary outcomes, fall.

Distribution of effect sizes for standardized outcomes broken by whether we have a standardized (top row) or a binary (bottom row) outcome. The columns signal whether we look at the effect of shifting from Standard Time to DST in the spring (left column) or DST to Standard Time in the fall (right column). Each of the histograms has a standard kernel density function (black lines) and a normal distribution (pink line) overlayed on top of it. Takeaway: Most of the estimated effects are quite small—falling within the bounds of what most standards would conceive to be small effects. Not surprisingly the fall treatment estimates (perhaps being confounded with the election itself) are also more noisy than the spring. Most of the effects themselves are still small.

We next turn to the effects on individual outcomes. This allows us to see any patterns that Figures 1 and 2 cannot show. Figure 3 shows the effects of starting DST (the spring treatment) on the ideology of all respondents scaled such that higher values mean that a person is more conservative. Despite strong theoretical connections through enhanced engagement, changes to mood/affect, changes to personality characteristics, changes in thought processes, declines in health, a heightened sense of social threat, and negative contextual forces, our results suggest that sleep disruptions appear to have little to no effect. The top panel allows for a visualization of what happens at the cutoff (denoted by a dashed line), but also what happens around the cutoff. As can be seen, there is little evidence of a change in individual ideology that corresponds with a sleep disruption that occurs at the start of DST. Moreover, ideology is a smooth function of the date of the survey—this is a positive thing for the identification strategy used here, given that competing treatments around the cutoff can, sometimes, bias RDiT estimates.

Figure 3. The effect of starting DST on conservative ideology. (a) Effect on conservatism of all citizens (b) Effect on subgroups.

The first panel shows bin averages in the dependent variable—conservative ideology—along with confidence intervals for these estimates. It also plots a smooth function separately on either side of the DST cutoff. The second panel shows the effects of DST on conservative ideology on all participants and four partisan subgroups (denoted on the x-axis). The y-axis is in standard deviation units, with higher values meaning the effect is to make people in that group more conservative and lower values meaning the opposite. Coefficients and standard errors are labeled for each of the estimates. Cohen’s effect sizes and a zero effect are shown as reference points with dotted and dashed lines, respectively. Points are sized as a function of sample size. Takeaway: There is no effect of the DST sleep disruption on individuals’ ideology, regardless of their political party.

The second panel of Figure 3 allows us to see whether there are any polarizing effects of the sleep shock introduced by the transition to DST. To do so, it runs the RDiT models breaking the sample by individuals’ political party. As can be seen, there is limited evidence of any polarizing ideological effects of the sleep disruption DST’s start provides. The estimated effect of Republican is statistically significant at the 5% level; however, its p-value comes close to what we would consider significant at traditional unadjusted levels (p = .03). Moreover, once we include controls, the effect is no longer significant at traditional unadjusted levels (p = .07). The effect estimate is also small, allowing us to rule out effects larger than 14.5% of a standard deviation in the models without controls and 13.1% of a standard deviation in the models with controls. In short, if there is an effect of the sleep shock that occurs around DST on Republicans, it moves them ever so slightly towards being more conservative. However, we cannot be certain that this is the case; and the effects are small, by most standards. We also find that the results are not substantively different across age groups.

The results are very similar if, instead of looking at ideology, we look at policy positions. Figures S2 and S3 in the Appendix do so. As can be seen there, there is little evidence that sleep disruptions change or polarize the public’s views on the many things that government can or should do. All of these effects are small and precisely estimated, allowing us to rule out meaningful effects. This holds true regardless of whether we scale the individual policy items together or look at the outcomes individually. This conclusion is also consistent across the political spectrum. Sleep disruptions, in short, do not change or polarize policy positions.

Do sleep disruptions change how individuals view the performance of their leaders and/or polarize leader views? Despite strong theoretical connection via changes to mood/affect and social context, there appears to be little to no effect of sleep disruptions on evaluations of one’s leaders. Figure 4 shows the estimates for evaluations of the incumbent (Trump) and the challenger (Biden). The results are broken by political party, as there are strong reasons to expect that one’s political party moderates the effect of negative shocks on candidate evaluations (Bartels, Reference Bartels2016). There appears to be little to no relationship. None of the effects are statistically significant at traditional unadjusted levels, much less adjusted levels. All are very close to zero, and all of them allow us to rule out very modest effects. Losing an hour of sleep has no corresponding effect on how respondents evaluate the performance of their leaders—regardless of whether the leader is part of the respondent’s “team” or the other “team.”

Figure 4. The effect of starting DST on incumbent and challenger evaluations. (a) Effect of starting DST on incumbent evaluations (Trump) (b) Effect of starting DST on challenger evaluations (Biden).

The first panel shows the effects on Trump approval and the second shows it for Biden approval. Estimates run for all participants and four partisan subgroups (denoted on the x-axis). The y-axis is in standard deviation units. Directional effects are labeled and shaded by their partisan implications. Coefficients and standard errors are labeled for each of the estimates. Cohen’s effect sizes and a zero effect are shown as reference points with dotted and dashed lines, respectively. Points are sized as a function of sample size. Takeaway: There is no effect of the DST sleep disruption on individuals’ evaluation of political leaders, regardless of their political party.

What about views toward members of the other political party? It is possible that the known effects on mood/affect, threat, and other attitudinal/contextual transformations after a sleep disruption predate a willingness to see the other party in a more negative light and one’s party in a more favorable light—thus polarizing views. In fact, as Figure 5 shows, there is some evidence of a small polarization in the views of Democrats. When they get less sleep over the short-run, Republicans see Democrats (slightly) less favorably ( $ \unicode{x03B2} =0.072\unicode{x03C3} $ ; p = .046) and Democrats see members of their own party (slightly) more favorably ( $ \unicode{x03B2} =-0.04\unicode{x03C3} $ ; p = .025). However, these effects are not very far below the $ \unicode{x03B1} =0.05 $ level at the unadjusted levels (neither are significant at adjusted levels). Moreover, these effects are not significant at unadjusted levels when we add controls (Republican views of Democrats: $ \unicode{x03B2} =0.043\unicode{x03C3} $ ; p = .182; Democratic views of Democrats: $ \unicode{x03B2} =-0.035\unicode{x03C3} $ ; p = .065). And regardless of specification, views toward Republicans—among Republicans and Democrats—stay the same, with the coefficients being close to zero, not significant, and precise enough to rule out very marginal effects. In short, it looks like disrupting sleep does not meaningfully polarize the public’s views toward the other political party. Short-run changes to sleep have no corresponding effect on measures of what some scholars have called affective polariztion (Iyengar et al., Reference Iyengar, Lelkes, Levendusky, Malhotra and Westwood2019).

Figure 5. The effect of starting DST on polarization of out-party/in-party. Effect of DST’s sleep disruption on evaluations of the two major political parties. Estimates run for both major parties (denoted on the x-axis). The y-axis is in standard deviation units. Directional effects are labeled on the figure. Coefficients and standard errors are labeled for each of the estimates. Cohen’s effect sizes and a zero effect are shown as reference points with dotted and dashed lines, respectively. Takeaway: There is little to no effect of the DST sleep disruption on individuals’ evaluation of the other/their own political party.

What about views toward other minority-status groups? Figure S7 in the Appendix shows the effects of the DST sleep disruption on attitudes toward racial minorities and women, broken by the political party of the respondent. Despite a theoretical moderating connection here—given large differences in baseline levels of these variables by party (i.e., Republicans have higher gender and racial resentment than Democrats)—there are no differences in the effects of a sleep disruption by party.

Figure 6 shows the effects of DST starting on racial resentment broken by the race of the participant. Here, again, there is no evidence of a meaningful effect, either within in- or out-groups. The closest effect is Asians’ response to member of their in-group, which at first glance appears to improve when Asians experience an exogenous sleep shock. However, this effect is not significant at the 5% unadjusted level (p = .061; 95% confidence interval: [–0.004, 0.164]), much less the adjusted levels. Moreover, when we add controls, the effect completely disappears (p = .217; 95% confidence interval: [–0.029, 0.129]). The other effects also allow us to rule out meaningful effects. In short, sleep disruptions—despite introducing negative shocks to mood/affect, social threat, and context (to name a few)—have little to no effect on racial animus across different groups.

Figure 6. The effect of starting DST on racial in- and out-group views. Effect of DST’s sleep disruption on evaluations of racial groups. Estimates run for both major parties (denoted on the x-axis). The y-axis is in standard deviation units. Directional effects are labeled on the figure. Coefficients and standard errors are labeled for each of the estimates. Cohen’s effect sizes and a zero effect are shown as reference points with dotted and dashed lines, respectively. Takeaway: There is little to no effect of the DST sleep disruption on individuals’ evaluation of the other/their own race.

Does the same hold for in- and out-group views along gender lines? Figure 7 shows the effects on gender resentment (i.e., resentment against females) broken by gender. As can be seen, there is precisely no effect of the DST sleep disruption on views toward the same or the opposite gender. These results are all precise and allow us to rule out very modest effects. Sleep disruptions do not change overall views toward women, nor how men or women view women in society more generally. Despite moving mood/affect, social threat, context, and other potential mechanisms, there is no polarizing or aggregate effect of sleep disruptions.

Figure 7. The effect of starting DST on in- and out-group gender resentment. Effect of DST’s sleep disruption on evaluations of women (i.e., gender resentment). Estimates run for both major parties (denoted on the x-axis). The y-axis is in standard deviation units. Directional effects are labeled on the figure. Coefficients and standard errors are labeled for each of the estimates. Cohen’s effect sizes and a zero effect are shown as reference points with dotted and dashed lines, respectively. Takeaway: There is little to no effect of the DST sleep disruption on individuals’ gender resentment, regardless of gender.

Does the same hold for in- and out-group views along religious lines? Figure 8 shows the effects on evaluations of various religious groups by the religion of the respondent. As can be seen, there is precisely no effect of the DST sleep disruption on views towards the same or the opposite religion in three largest religious groups—Protestants, Catholics, and other Christians. The effects among Jewish people and Muslim people are much noisier—reflecting the smaller number of these individuals in society and in the NS data—however, none of these effects is significant. In-group effects for both Jewish people and Muslim people are in the increased favorability direction, though neither is significant. The rest of the results, however, once adequately powered are all precise and allow us to rule out very modest effects. Like race and gender, sleep disruptions do not change overall views toward religious groups, nor how people of various religions groups view their/other religious groups more generally.

Figure 8. The effect of starting DST on religious in- and out-group views. Effect of DST’s sleep disruption on evaluations of religious groups. Estimates run for both major parties (denoted on the x-axis). The y-axis is in standard deviation units. Directional effects are labeled on the figure. Coefficients and standard errors are labeled for each of the estimates. Cohen’s effect sizes and a zero effect are shown as reference points with dotted and dashed lines, respectively. Takeaway: There is little to no effect of the DST sleep disruption on individuals’ evaluation of the other/their own religion.

Robustness checks

The results just documented are robust to various reasonable alternative approaches. For example, beyond the groups examined, we continue to see precise nulls effects if we look at all other majority-minority group views (see Figures S4 and S5 in the Appendix). Moreover, we see precise nulls if we tweak the conceptualization of the treatment to be the Monday after DST starts, based on the rationale that any sleep effects may appear only when people start their normal work week. This is shown in Figure S10 in the Appendix.

The results also remain (precisely) nulls if we run a donut RDiT, which is designed to address any potential heaping biases or precise manipulation around the cutoff (Barreca et al., Reference Barreca, Lindo and Waddell2016). This is shown in Figure S8 in the Appendix. With this check, the effect sizes are all small, no more are significant than what we would expect by chance, and we can rule out effects of any meaningful size. The systemic nulls remain (and often get even more null) with covariates (see Figures 1 and S6), they are present across bandwidths (see Figures S12 and S13), and they are present around the fall DST change as well (see Figures 1 and S11). The effects are precisely null if we include day-of-week fixed effects to account for any inherent/unobservable fixed intra-weekly differences between those who responded to the survey on different days of the week. This check is provide in Figure S9 in the Appendix. With this check, the effect sizes are very similar, as are the number of effects that are significant, and as also is our ability to rule out effects of any meaningful size. The results remain null if we scale our individual items together into a politics (i.e., a composite measure we can use to look at polarization) and a social (i.e., a composite measure we can can use to look at discrimination) scale (see Figures S23 and S24).

Our effects are also very similar if, instead of a RDiT, we conceptualize our setup as that of an event study and focus in on the immediate days preceding and following DST; Figures S16S21 show stable patterns in each of the individual days that make up the week before and after DST. Finally, the results are very similar by age (see Figure S14)—suggesting that a hardening across the later life cycle does not drive our findings—education, and income (see Figure S15).

In short, these effects are very robust. None of the changes outlined earlier alters the general pattern that reflects no effect of DST’s sleep disruption in how individuals view the world, other people in it, and themselves relative to that world. There appears to be little evidence that sleep disruptions cause people to become more polarized or more discriminatory. In fact, the evidence is much more consistent with the null hypothesis—sleep disruptions appear to have no effect.

Discussion

To what extent does sleep influence the political polarization, social fragmentation, and discriminatory beliefs that we see in our modern society? There are at least two reasons why the length of sleep would influence these important outcomes. One is that macro-level trends toward less sleep coincide with changes in polarization and discrimination. A second is that theory and conditional-on-observables studies suggest that the changes that sleep disruption has on individuals’ propensity to participate, mood/affect, personality characteristics, feelings of stress/threat, thought processes, and their social context also make individuals more discriminatory and polarized toward potential out-groups.

In this article, we assess the extent to which the effects of sleep extend past the outcomes mentioned earlier, focusing specifically on changes in how people see the world, the people around them, and themselves in relation to those groups. To do so, we used a uniquely large and broad data set of 77 waves survey data from UCLA’s 2019–2021 Nationscape Survey, a regular nationally-representative political survey. Using an RDiT design, we find that—contrary to expectations and previous research—sleep does not polarize, nor does it make people more discriminatory. The overall null result is remarkably robust, the effects are quite precise, and the evidence for the null is fairly consistent across subgroups.

Our work makes several important contributions—to both the study of sleep and the study of the outcomes we explore. On the sleep side, the broader (non)consequences of sleep are vital given the core role that rest plays in human life and the broader macrosocietal trend toward both less and lower-quality sleep. To better understand the role of sleep, it is important not only to consider what sleep does influence, but also what it does not influence. By doing so, we can (1) refine theoretical models about the role that sleep plays in human behavior, (2) inform future research, and (3) contribute to policy discussions about the benefits and the nonbenefits of public and private interventions to improve sleep. Our work shows that sleep disruption’s myriad effects do not include changes to how people approach the world around them, at least not in this context.

On the outcomes side, our research contributes to the large, growing literature on discrimination (for an overview of this literature, see Cramer, Reference Cramer2020) and political polarization (for an overview of this literature, see Iyengar et al., Reference Iyengar, Lelkes, Levendusky, Malhotra and Westwood2019). Scholars have long tried to understand why some humans develop discriminatory attitudes and to develop interventions that address them (e.g., Paluck et al., Reference Paluck, Green and Green2019). Though macro-level trends of sleep deprivation and discrimination and polarization (potentially) overlap, it appears that sleep disruptions do not cause these social problems. (Though the reverse could still be true; future work is needed to know for sure.) This pushes forward our understanding of what does and what does not influence the key societal outcomes of polarization and discrimination and suggests that interventions targeting sleep improvements are unlikely to reduce these issues.

This study has a few important limitations. First, as is true in all RDDs, the effects we estimate are local to the cutoff(s) that we observe. We mitigate this concern somewhat by adding an event study design (in the Appendix), which allows for effects that evolve over time away from the DST cutoff. However, it is possible that effects emerge in other contexts. Second, our effects relate to short-term changes in sleeping patterns and therefore only shed light indirectly on longer periods of sleep loss or variability. In short, this should not be the last study that uses a causal identification strategy to study the question at hand. Third, it is possible that sleep might have heterogeneous effects on outcomes that we do not observe in our survey data. We have tested for heterogeneity in, arguably, the most important dimensions for the outcomes of our study—race, gender, political party, religion, age, education, and income. There could be, however, effects that emerge in other smaller subsets of the population. Finally, we readily admit that our study was (mostly) conducted during the COVID-19 pandemic (aside from the fall 2019 results), and our results may not generalize beyond this period.

Future research would do well to further document the boundaries of sleep’s influence. After all, “despite its centrality to human life, health, and happiness, sleep has never been a central topic of concern to political scientists” (Ksiazkiewicz, Reference Ksiazkiewicz2020, p. 367). Taking the interdisciplinary literature on sleep seriously would lead us to believe that sleep could affect (virtually) every outcome of any importance. Our results, however, show that there are limits to what sleep and sleep variations affect. Though our study shows that sleep disruptions do not affect polarization or discrimination, we are unable to explain why this is. Mechanism testing is inherently difficult—if not impossible—in many cases (Green et al., Reference Green, Ha and Bullock2010), and that is no different in the case of studying sleep’s effects. This limitation highlights the need for future research on why sleep disruptions of the variety that we study do not change core social and political views. One avenue for this research is to better unpack sleep’s effects on the many dimensions of individual mood and emotion. While we have a general sense that sleep disruptions make people feel worse, we do not know have a clear sense of sleep’s effects across the many dimensions of individual emotion (e.g., anger, anxiety, confusion, fear, disgust, etc.). And the lingering lack of causal identification in research on psychological attachments and political views looms large. Pushing these areas forward will better allow for a connection to the research that studies the effect of mood/emotion on political behaviors (Valentino et al., Reference Valentino, Brader, Groenendyk, Gregorowicz and Hutchings2011; Webster, Reference Webster2020).

Our work is vital in the current moment of soaring levels of social fragmentation. The results represent, ultimately, a hopeful finding, which, if replicated in the context of other sleep disruptions, suggests that our society will not become more discriminatory or politically polarized because of the recent steep decline in sleep levels. Our work suggests that we cannot sleep our way to solutions in this domain.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/pls.2023.7.

Open scientific practices statement

This article earned the Open Data and Open Materials badges. All data and replication code for this study are available on the Harvard Dataverse at https://dataverse.harvard.edu/dataset.xhtml?persistentId=doi:10.7910/DVN/6WN41J. This study was not preregistered.

Footnotes

This article earned the Open Data and Open Materials badges for open scientific practices. For details, see the Open Scientific Practices Statement.

1 Whether sleep duration has declined over time is disputed (Hirshkowitz et al., Reference Hirshkowitz, Whiton, Albert, Alessi, Bruni, DonCarlos, Hazen, Herman, Hillard, Katz, Kheirandish-Gozal, Neubauer, O’Donnell, Ohayon, Peever, Rawding, Sachdeva, Setters, Vitiello and Ware2015; Jones, Reference Jones2013; Watson et al., Reference Watson, Badr, Belenky, Bliwise, Buxton, Buysse, Dinges, Gangwisch and Grandner2015; Youngstedt et al., Reference Youngstedt, Goff, Reynolds, Kripke, Irwin, Bootzin, Khan and Jean-Louis2016). However, many citizens are increasingly bombarded with strong forces seemingly intended to disrupt to their sleep, including heightened levels of light pollution (Raap et al., Reference Raap, Pinxten and Eens2015), ever-growing noise pollution (Halperin, Reference Halperin2014), rising patterns of substance dependence and abuse (Bertazzo-Silveira et al., Reference Bertazzo-Silveira, Kruger, De Toledo, Porporatti, Dick, Flores-Mir and Canto2016), a marked rise in stress and health issues (some, but not all, of this is related to the COVID-19 pandemic) that make sleeping difficult (Gupta et al., Reference Gupta, Dahiya and Bhatia2009; Xiong et al., Reference Xiong, Lipsitz, Nasri, Lui, Gill, Phan, Chen-Li, Iacobucci, Ho, Majeed and McIntyre2020; Jahrami et al., Reference Jahrami, BaHammam, Bragazzi, Saif, Faris and Vitiello2021), and many technological developments that disrupt circadian rhythms (Calamaro et al., Reference Calamaro, Mason and Ratcliffe2009).

2 Following prior work, by “sleep disruptions,” we mean an interruption to one’s sleep patterns that results in worse sleep.

3 The fall 2019 data predate the COVID-19 pandemic.

4 The closest other exogenous shift to sleep in an ecologically valid controlled environment occurs at time-zone boundaries, where individuals living on the marginally eastern border sleep less because of disruptions to circadian rhythms. While designs that exploit this shift have some desirable properties, it is hard to know for certain whether changes across time zones are attributable to sleep alone (Giuntella & Mazzonna, Reference Giuntella and Mazzonna2019; Holbein et al., Reference Holbein, Schafer and Dickinson2019; Schafer & Holbein, Reference Schafer and Holbein2020). Lab/pseudo-lab experiments of insufficient sleep exist (Dickinson & McElroy, Reference Dickinson and McElroy2010, Reference Dickinson and McElroy2017; Holbein et al., Reference Holbein, Schafer and Dickinson2019), but they often involve small, nonrepresentative samples. Just as important, participants in sleep studies know they are being monitored. This environment may lead to Hawthorne effects or encourage social desirability and, therefore, cause people to alter their discriminatory and polarizing beliefs.

5 Interestingly, Holbein et al. (Reference Holbein, Schafer and Dickinson2019) provide evidence that participatory losses in sleep do not seem to be about political start-up (i.e., interest in politics), but rather are about follow-up (i.e., translating interest into action) on civic participation. Our results mirror theirs, indicating that the effect of DST’s sleep treatment has null effects on measures of political interest. Sadly, the NS data set only measures actual follow-through in political behaviors—such as volunteering or holding political discussions—in a few intervals that do not overlap with the start or end of DST.

6 Some scholars have noted that RDiTs are conceptually similar to interrupted time series (Hausman & Rapson, Reference Hausman and Rapson2018).

7 Models with controls in Figure 1 include whether the person is unemployed, foreign born, speaks English only in their house, gender, income, and education, as well as indicators for different religious traditions and census regions.

References

Abdi, H., et al. (2007). Bonferroni and Šidák corrections for multiple comparisons. In Salkind, N. L. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of measurement and statistics (Vol. 3, pp. 103107). Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Abele, A., Gendofla, G. H., & Petzold, P. (1998). Positive mood and in-group–out-group differentiation in a minimal group setting. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24(12), 13431357.10.1177/01461672982412008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, C., & Dickinson, D. L. (2010). Bargaining and trust: The effects of 36-h total sleep deprivation on socially interactive decisions. Journal of Sleep Research, 19(1 Pt. 1), 5463.10.1111/j.1365-2869.2009.00767.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ansolabehere, S., Rodden, J., & Snyder, J. M. (2008). The strength of issues: Using multiple measures to gauge preference stability, ideological constraint, and issue voting. American Political Science Review, 102(2), 215232.10.1017/S0003055408080210CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnold, R. D., & Carnes, N. (2012). Holding mayors accountable: New York’s executives from Koch to Bloomberg. American Journal of Political Science, 56(4), 949963.10.1111/j.1540-5907.2012.00603.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnes, C. M., & Wagner, D. T. (2009). Changing to Daylight Saving Time cuts into sleep and increases workplace injuries. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94(5), 13051317.10.1037/a0015320CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barreca, A. I., Lindo, J. M., & Waddell, G. R. (2016). Heaping-induced bias in regression-discontinuity designs. Economic Inquiry, 54(1), 268293.10.1111/ecin.12225CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartels, L. M. (2000). Partisanship and voting behavior, 1952–1996. American Journal of Political Science, 44(1), 3550.10.2307/2669291CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartels, L. M. (2016). Unequal democracy. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Bassi, A. (2013, May 22). Weather, mood, and voting: an experimental analysis of the effect of weather beyond turnout. https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=2273189 10.2139/ssrn.2273189CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bateson, R. (2012). Crime victimization and political participation. American Political Science Review, 106(3), 570587.10.1017/S0003055412000299CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernardi, L., Mattila, M., Papageorgiou, A., & Rapeli, L. (2023). Down but not yet out: Depression, political efficacy, and voting. Political Psychology, 44(2), 21723310.1111/pops.12837CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berry, C. R., & Howell, W. G. (2007). Accountability and local elections: Rethinking retrospective voting. The Journal of Politics, 69(3), 844858.10.1111/j.1468-2508.2007.00579.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bertazzo-Silveira, E., Kruger, C. M., De Toledo, I. P., Porporatti, A. L., Dick, B., Flores-Mir, C., & Canto, G. D. L. (2016). Association between sleep bruxism and alcohol, caffeine, tobacco, and drug abuse: A systematic review. The Journal of the American Dental Association, 147(11), 859866.10.1016/j.adaj.2016.06.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berument, M. H., Dogan, N., & Onar, B. (2010). Effects of Daylight Saving Time changes on stock market volatility. Psychological Reports, 106(2), 632640.10.2466/pr0.106.2.632-640CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bliwise, D. L. (2008). Invited commentary: Cross-cultural influences on sleep—Broadening the environmental landscape. American Journal of Epidemiology, 168(12), 13651366.10.1093/aje/kwn336CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boakye, P. A., Olechowski, C., Rashiq, S., Verrier, M. J., Kerr, B., Witmans, M., Baker, G., Joyce, A., & Dick, B. D. (2016). A critical review of neurobiological factors involved in the interactions between chronic pain, depression, and sleep disruption. The Clinical Journal of Pain, 32(4), 327336.10.1097/AJP.0000000000000260CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calamaro, C. J., Mason, T., & Ratcliffe, S. J. (2009). Adolescents living the 24/7 lifestyle: Effects of caffeine and technology on sleep duration and daytime functioning. Pediatrics, 123(6), e1005e1010.10.1542/peds.2008-3641CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calonico, S., Cattaneo, M. D., & Titiunik, R. (2014). Robust nonparametric confidence intervals for regression-discontinuity designs. Econometrica, 82(6), 22952326.10.3982/ECTA11757CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castillo, M., Dickinson, D. L., & Petrie, R. (2017). Sleepiness, choice consistency, and risk preferences. Theory and Decision, 82(1), 4173.10.1007/s11238-016-9559-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chee, M. W., & Chuah, L. Y. (2008). Functional neuroimaging insights into how sleep and sleep deprivation affect memory and cognition. Current Opinion in Neurology, 21(4), 417423.10.1097/WCO.0b013e3283052cf7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cleveland Clinic. (n.d.). Sleep basics. Retrieved April 25, 2023, from https://my.clevelandclinic.org/health/articles/12148-sleep-basics.Google Scholar
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 155159.10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coppock, A., & McClellan, O. A. (2019). Validating the demographic, political, psychological, and experimental results obtained from a new source of online survey respondents. Research & Politics, 6(1), 2053168018822174.10.1177/2053168018822174CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cramer, K. (2020). Understanding the role of racism in contemporary us public opinion. Annual Review of Political Science, 23, 153169.10.1146/annurev-polisci-060418-042842CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crick, F., & Mitchison, G. (1983). The function of dream sleep. Nature, 304(5922), 111114.10.1038/304111a0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dahl, R. E., & Lewin, D. S. (2002). Pathways to adolescent health sleep regulation and behavior. Journal of Adolescent Health, 31(6), 175184.10.1016/S1054-139X(02)00506-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De la Cuesta, B., & Imai, K. (2016). Misunderstandings about the regression discontinuity design in the study of close elections. Annual Review of Political Science, 19, 375396.10.1146/annurev-polisci-032015-010115CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dickinson, D. L., & McElroy, T. (2010). Rationality around the clock: Sleep and time-of-day effects on guessing game responses. Economics Letters, 108(2), 245248.10.1016/j.econlet.2010.04.029CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dickinson, D. L., & McElroy, T. (2017). Sleep restriction and circadian effects on social decisions. European Economic Review, 97, 5771.10.1016/j.euroecorev.2017.05.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dickinson, D. L., Wolkow, A. P., Rajaratnam, S. M., & Drummond, S. P. (2018). Personal sleep debt and daytime sleepiness mediate the relationship between sleep and mental health outcomes in young adults. Depression and Anxiety, 35(8), 775783.10.1002/da.22769CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doleac, J. L., & Sanders, N. J. (2015). Under the cover of darkness: How ambient light influences criminal activity. Review of Economics and Statistics, 97(5), 10931103.10.1162/REST_a_00547CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Domínguez, P., & Asahi, K. (2019). Crime time: How ambient light affects crime (Working Paper No. DB-WP-991). Inter-American Development Bank. https://doi.org/10.18235/0001701CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dudoit, S., Shaffer, J. P., & Boldrick, J. C. (2003). Multiple hypothesis testing in microarray experiments. Statistical Science, 18(1), 71103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Easterbrook, P. J., Gopalan, R., Berlin, J., & Matthews, D. R. (1991). Publication bias in clinical research. The Lancet, 337(8746), 867872.10.1016/0140-6736(91)90201-YCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enos, R. D. (2016). What the demolition of public housing teaches us about the impact of racial threat on political behavior. American Journal of Political Science, 60(1), 123142.10.1111/ajps.12156CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiorina, M. P. (1978). Economic retrospective voting in american national elections: A microanalysis. American Journal of Political Science, 22(2), 426443.10.2307/2110623CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foster, R. G., Peirson, S. N., Wulff, K., Winnebeck, E., Vetter, C., & Roenneberg, T. (2013). Sleep and circadian rhythm disruption in social jetlag and mental illness. Progress in Molecular Biology and Translational Science, 119, 325346.10.1016/B978-0-12-396971-2.00011-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fowler, J. H. (2006). Elections and markets: The effect of partisanship, policy risk, and electoral margins on the economy. The Journal of Politics, 68(1), 89103.10.1111/j.1468-2508.2006.00372.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franco, A., Malhotra, N., & Simonovits, G. (2014). Publication bias in the social sciences: Unlocking the file drawer. Science, 345(6203), 15021505.10.1126/science.1255484CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frank, M. G. (2006). The mystery of sleep function: Current perspectives and future directions. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 17(4), 375392.10.1515/revneuro.2006.17.4.375CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gelman, A., & Carlin, J. (2014). Beyond power calculations: Assessing Type S (sign) and Type M (magnitude) errors. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9(6), 641651.10.1177/1745691614551642CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerber, A. S., Huber, G. A., Doherty, D., & Dowling, C. M. (2011). The Big Five personality traits in the political arena. Annual Review of Political Science, 14, 265287.10.1146/annurev-polisci-051010-111659CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerber, A. S., & Malhotra, N. (2008). Publication bias in empirical sociological research: Do arbitrary significance levels distort published results? Sociological Methods & Research, 37(1), 330.10.1177/0049124108318973CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giles, M. W., & Hertz, K. (1994). Racial threat and partisan identification. American Political Science Review, 88(2), 317326.10.2307/2944706CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giuntella, O., Han, W., & Mazzonna, F. (2017). Circadian rhythms, sleep, and cognitive skills: Evidence from an unsleeping giant. Demography, 54(5), 17151742.10.1007/s13524-017-0609-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giuntella, O., & Mazzonna, F. (2015). If you don’t snooze you lose health and gain weight: Evidence from a regression discontinuity design (Discussion Paper No. 9773). Institute for the Study of Labor. https://ftp.iza.org/dp9773.pdfGoogle Scholar
Giuntella, O., & Mazzonna, F. (2019). Sunset time and the economic effects of social jetlag: Evidence from US time zone borders. Journal of Health Economics, 65, 210226.10.1016/j.jhealeco.2019.03.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, D. P., Ha, S. E., & Bullock, J. G. (2010). Enough already about “black box” experiments: Studying mediation is more difficult than most scholars suppose. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 628(1), 200208.10.1177/0002716209351526CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gu, F., Xu, S., Devesa, S. S., Zhang, F., Klerman, E. B., Graubard, B. I., & Caporaso, N. E. (2017). Longitude position in a time zone and cancer risk in the United States. Cancer Epidemiology and Prevention Biomarkers, 26(8), 13061311.10.1158/1055-9965.EPI-16-1029CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gupta, R., Dahiya, S., & Bhatia, M. S. (2009). Effect of depression on sleep: Qualitative or quantitative? Indian Journal of Psychiatry, 51(2), 117121.10.4103/0019-5545.49451CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haack, M., & Mullington, J. M. (2005). Sustained sleep restriction reduces emotional and physical well-being. Pain, 119(1–3), 5664.10.1016/j.pain.2005.09.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hafner, M., Stepanek, M., Taylor, J., Troxel, W. M., & Van Stolk, C. (2017). Why sleep matters—The economic costs of insufficient sleep: A cross-country comparative analysis. RAND Health Quarterly, 6(4), 11.Google ScholarPubMed
Hahn, J., Todd, P., & Van der Klaauw, W. (2001). Identification and estimation of treatment effects with a regression-discontinuity design. Econometrica, 69(1), 201209.10.1111/1468-0262.00183CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halperin, D. (2014). Environmental noise and sleep disturbances: A threat to health? Sleep Science, 7(4), 209212.10.1016/j.slsci.2014.11.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hansen, B. T., Sonderskov, K. M., Hageman, I., Dinesen, P. T., & Ostergaard, S. D. (2017). Daylight Savings Time transitions and the incidence rate of unipolar depressive episodes. Epidemiology, 28(3), 346353.10.1097/EDE.0000000000000580CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hartman, E., & Hidalgo, F. D. (2018). An equivalence approach to balance and placebo tests. American Journal of Political Science, 62(4), 10001013.10.1111/ajps.12387CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hausman, C., & Rapson, D. S. (2018). Regression discontinuity in time: Considerations for empirical applications. Annual Review of Resource Economics, 10, 533552.10.1146/annurev-resource-121517-033306CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Healy, A., & Lenz, G. S. (2014). Substituting the end for the whole: Why voters respond primarily to the election-year economy. American Journal of Political Science, 58(1), 3147.10.1111/ajps.12053CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Healy, A., & Lenz, G. S. (2017). Presidential voting and the local economy: Evidence from two population-based data sets. The Journal of Politics, 79(4), 14191432.10.1086/692785CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Healy, A., & Malhotra, N. (2013). Retrospective voting reconsidered. Annual Review of Political Science, 16, 285306.10.1146/annurev-polisci-032211-212920CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heboyan, V., Stevens, S., & McCall, W. V. (2019). Effects of seasonality and daylight savings time on emergency department visits for mental health disorders. American Journal of Emergency Medicine, 37(8), 14761481.10.1016/j.ajem.2018.10.056CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heissel, J. A., Levy, D. J., & Adam, E. K. (2017). Stress, sleep, and performance on standardized tests: Understudied pathways to the achievement gap. AERA Open, 3(3), 2332858417713488.10.1177/2332858417713488CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heissel, J. A., & Norris, S. (2018). Rise and shine: The effect of school start times on academic performance from childhood through puberty. Journal of Human Resources, 53(4), 957992.10.3368/jhr.53.4.0815-7346R1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirshkowitz, M., Whiton, K., Albert, S. M., Alessi, C., Bruni, O., DonCarlos, L., Hazen, N., Herman, J., Hillard, P. J. A., Katz, E. S., Kheirandish-Gozal, L.Neubauer, D. N.O’Donnell, A. E.Ohayon, M.Peever, J., Rawding, R.Sachdeva, R. C.Setters, B.Vitiello, M. V., & Ware, J. C. (2015). National Sleep Foundation’s updated sleep duration recommendations. Sleep Health, 1(4), 233243.10.1016/j.sleh.2015.10.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hodson, G., & Busseri, M. A. (2012). Bright minds and dark attitudes: Lower cognitive ability predicts greater prejudice through right-wing ideology and low intergroup contact. Psychological Science, 23(2), 187195.10.1177/0956797611421206CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holbein, J. (2016). Left behind? Citizen responsiveness to government performance information. American Political Science Review, 110(2), 353368.10.1017/S0003055416000071CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holbein, J. B., & Hassell, H. J. (2019). When your group fails: The effect of race-based performance signals on citizen voice and exit. Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory, 29(2), 268286.10.1093/jopart/muy075CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holbein, J. B., Schafer, J. P., & Dickinson, D. L. (2019). Insufficient sleep reduces voting and other prosocial behaviours. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(5), 492500.10.1038/s41562-019-0543-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Homola, J. (2021). Partisanship and perceived threats about immigration. Party Politics, 27(5), 977982.10.1177/1354068820918773CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horne, J. A. (1993). Human sleep, sleep loss and behaviour: Implications for the prefrontal cortex and psychiatric disorder. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 162(3), 413419.10.1192/bjp.162.3.413CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huber, R., Felice Ghilardi, M., Massimini, M., & Tononi, G. (2004). Local sleep and learning. Nature, 430(6995), 7881.10.1038/nature02663CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Imbens, G. W., & Lemieux, T. (2008). Regression discontinuity designs: A guide to practice. Journal of Econometrics, 142(2), 615635.10.1016/j.jeconom.2007.05.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iyengar, S., Lelkes, Y., Levendusky, M., Malhotra, N., & Westwood, S. J. (2019). The origins and consequences of affective polarization in the United States. Annual Review of Political Science, 22, 129146.10.1146/annurev-polisci-051117-073034CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jahrami, H., BaHammam, A. S., Bragazzi, N. L., Saif, Z., Faris, M., & Vitiello, M. V. (2021). Sleep problems during the COVID-19 pandemic by population: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, 17(2), 299313.10.5664/jcsm.8930CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jiddou, M. R., Pica, M., Boura, J., Qu, L., & Franklin, B. A. (2013). Incidence of myocardial infarction with shifts to and from Daylight Savings Time. American Journal of Cardiology, 111(5), 631635.10.1016/j.amjcard.2012.11.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jonason, P. K., Jones, A., & Lyons, M. (2013). Creatures of the night: Chronotypes and the dark triad traits. Personality and Individual Differences, 55(5), 538541.10.1016/j.paid.2013.05.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, J. M. (2013, December 19). In US, 40% get less than recommended amount of sleep. Gallup. https://news.gallup.com/poll/166553/less-recommended-amount-sleep.aspxGoogle Scholar
Kamstra, M. J., Kramer, L. A., & Levi, M. D. (2000). Losing sleep at the market: The Daylight Saving anomaly. American Economic Review, 90(4), 10051011.10.1257/aer.90.4.1005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keen, B., & Jacobs, D. (2009). Racial threat, partisan politics, and racial disparities in prison admissions: A panel analysis. Criminology, 47(1), 209238.10.1111/j.1745-9125.2009.00143.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kerr, N. L. (1998). Harking: Hypothesizing after the results are known. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2(3), 196217.10.1207/s15327957pspr0203_4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kolla, B. P., Coombes, B. J., Morgenthaler, T. I., & Mansukhani, M. P. (2021). Increased patient safety-related incidents following the transition into Daylight Savings Time. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 36(1), 5154.10.1007/s11606-020-06090-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koopman, A. D., Rauh, S. P., van ‘t Riet, E., Groeneveld, L., Van Der Heijden, A. A., Elders, P. J., Dekker, J. M., Nijpels, G., Beulens, J. W., & Rutters, F. (2017). The association between social jetlag, the metabolic syndrome, and type 2 diabetes mellitus in the general population: the new hoorn study. Journal of Biological Rhythms, 32(4), 359368.10.1177/0748730417713572CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kountouris, Y. (2021). An assessment of the relationship between Daylight Saving Time, disruptions in sleep patterns and dwelling fires. Fire Technology, 57(1), 123144.10.1007/s10694-020-00983-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ksiazkiewicz, A. (2020). Conservative larks, liberal owls: The relationship between chronotype and political ideology. The Journal of Politics, 82(1), 367371.10.1086/705927CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ksiazkiewicz, A. (2022). Sleeping giant: A research agenda for politics and chronobiology. Politics and the Life Sciences, 41(2), 298302.10.1017/pls.2022.16CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ksiazkiewicz, A., & Erol, F. (2022a). Linking sleep, political ideology, and religious observance: A multi-national comparison. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 34(3), edac020.10.1093/ijpor/edac020CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ksiazkiewicz, A. and Erol, F. (2022b). Too tired to vote: A multi-national comparison of election turnout with sleep preferences and behaviors. Electoral Studies, 78, 102491.10.1016/j.electstud.2022.102491CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuehnle, D., & Wunder, C. (2016). Using the life satisfaction approach to value Daylight Savings Time transitions: evidence from Britain and Germany. Journal of Happiness Studies, 17(6), 22932323.10.1007/s10902-015-9695-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakens, D. (2015). Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science, 349(6251), 110.Google Scholar
Lakens, D., Scheel, A. M., & Isager, P. M. (2018). Equivalence testing for psychological research: A tutorial. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 1(2), 259269.10.1177/2515245918770963CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laliotis, I., Moscelli, G., & Monastiriotis, V. (2019). Summertime and the drivin’ is easy? Daylight Saving Time and vehicle accidents (LEQS Paper No. 150). London School of Economics and Political Science. https://ideas.repec.org/p/eiq/eileqs/150.htmlGoogle Scholar
Lee, D. S., & Card, D. (2008). Regression discontinuity inference with specification error. Journal of Econometrics, 142(2), 655674.10.1016/j.jeconom.2007.05.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, D. S., & Lemieux, T. (2010). Regression discontinuity designs in economics. Journal of Economic Literature, 48(2), 281355.10.1257/jel.48.2.281CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levandovski, R., Dantas, G., Fernandes, L. C., Caumo, W., Torres, I., Roenneberg, T., Hidalgo, M. P. L., & Allebrandt, K. V. (2011). Depression scores associate with chronotype and social jetlag in a rural population. Chronobiology International, 28(9), 771778.10.3109/07420528.2011.602445CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lim, J., & Dinges, D. F. (2010). A meta-analysis of the impact of short-term sleep deprivation on cognitive variables. Psychological Bulletin, 136(3), 375389.10.1037/a0018883CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malhotra, N., & Popp, E. (2012). Bridging partisan divisions over antiterrorism policies: The role of threat perceptions. Political Research Quarterly, 65(1), 3447.10.1177/1065912910385251CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McKenna, B. S., Dickinson, D. L., Orff, H. J., & Drummond, S. P. (2007). The effects of one night of sleep deprivation on known-risk and ambiguous-risk decisions. Journal of Sleep Research, 16(3), 245252.10.1111/j.1365-2869.2007.00591.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milosh, M., Painter, M., Sonin, K., Van Dijcke, D., & Wright, A. L. (2021). Unmasking partisanship: Polarization undermines public response to collective risk. Journal of Public Economics, 204, 104538.10.1016/j.jpubeco.2021.104538CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mondak, J. J. (2010). Personality and the foundations of political behavior. Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511761515CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Munyo, I. (2018). Daylight Saving Time and crime: Does tiredness also affect criminal behavior? Journal of Applied Biobehavioral Research, 23(3), e12115.10.1111/jabr.12115CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muzur, A., Pace-Schott, E. F., & Hobson, J. A. (2002). The prefrontal cortex in sleep. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(11), 475481.10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01992-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naeim, A., Baxter-King, R., Wenger, N., Stanton, A. L., Sepucha, K., & Vavreck, L. (2021). Effects of age, gender, health status, and political party on COVID-19–related concerns and prevention behaviors: Results of a large, longitudinal cross-sectional survey. JMIR Public Health and Surveillance, 7(4), e24277.10.2196/24277CrossRefGoogle Scholar
National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute. (2022, March 24). What are sleep deprivation and deficiency? https://www.nhlbi.nih.gov/health/sleep-deprivation#:~:text=Sleep%20deficiency%20is%20linked%20to,adults%2C%20teens%2C%20and%20children.Google Scholar
National Institutes of Health. (n.d.). Brain basics: Understanding sleep. Retrieved April 25, 2023, from https://www.ninds.nih.gov/Disorders/Patient-Caregiver-Education/Understanding-sleep.Google Scholar
The Nobel Prize. (2017). The Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine 2017. NobelPrize.org. 2017. https://www.nobelprize.org/prizes/medicine/2017/press-release/Google Scholar
Ojeda, C. (2015). Depression and political participation. Social Science Quarterly, 96(5), 12261243.10.1111/ssqu.12173CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Onraet, E., Van Hiel, A., Dhont, K., Hodson, G., Schittekatte, M., & De Pauw, S. (2015). The association of cognitive ability with right-wing ideological attitudes and prejudice: A meta-analytic review. European Journal of Personality, 29(6), 599621.10.1002/per.2027CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Open Science Collaboration. (2015). Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science, 349(6251), aac4716.Google Scholar
Pacheco, D. (2020). Daylight saving time—how time change affects sleep. Sleep Foundation. Retrieved October 27, 2020, from https://www.sleepfoundation.org/circadian-rhythm/daylight-saving-time.Google Scholar
Paluck, E. L., Green, S. A., & Green, D. P. (2019). The contact hypothesis re-evaluated. Behavioural Public Policy, 3(2), 129158.10.1017/bpp.2018.25CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peyton, K., Huber, G. A., & Coppock, A. (2022). The generalizability of online experiments conducted during the COVID-19 pandemic. Journal of Experimental Political Science, 9(3), 379394.10.1017/XPS.2021.17CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Porkka-Heiskanen, T. (1999). Adenosine in sleep and wakefulness. Annals of Medicine, 31(2), 125129.10.3109/07853899908998788CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Potoski, M., & Urbatsch, R. (2017). Entertainment and the opportunity cost of civic participation: Monday Night Football game quality suppresses turnout in US elections. The Journal of Politics, 79(2), 424438.10.1086/688174CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prather, A. A., Bogdan, R., & Hariri, P. A. R. (2013). Impact of sleep quality on amygdala reactivity, negative affect, and perceived stress. Psychosomatic Medicine, 75(4), 350358.10.1097/PSY.0b013e31828ef15bCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raap, T., Pinxten, R., & Eens, M. (2015). Light pollution disrupts sleep in free-living animals. Scientific Reports, 5(1), 18.10.1038/srep13557CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Randler, C., Schredl, M., & Göritz, A. S. (2017). Chronotype, sleep behavior, and the Big Five personality factors. Sage Open, 7(3), 2158244017728321.10.1177/2158244017728321CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reny, T. T. (2020). Masculine norms and infectious disease: The case of COVID-19. Politics & Gender, 16(4), 10281035.10.1017/S1743923X20000380CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reny, T. T., & Barreto, M. A. (2022). Xenophobia in the time of pandemic: Othering, anti-Asian attitudes, and COVID-19. Politics, Groups, and Identities, 10(2), 209232.10.1080/21565503.2020.1769693CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reny, T. T., & Newman, B. J. (2018). Protecting the right to discriminate: The second great migration and racial threat in the American West. American Political Science Review, 112(4), 11041110.10.1017/S0003055418000448CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reny, T. T., & Newman, B. J. (2021). The opinion-mobilizing effect of social protest against police violence: Evidence from the 2020 George Floyd protests. American Political Science Review, 115(4), 14991507.10.1017/S0003055421000460CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rivers, N. (2018). Does Daylight Savings Time save energy? Evidence from Ontario. Environmental and Resource Economics, 70(2), 517543.10.1007/s10640-017-0131-xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roenneberg, T. (2013). The human sleep project. Nature, 498(7455), 427428.10.1038/498427aCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roenneberg, T., Allebrandt, K. V., Merrow, M., & Vetter, C. (2012). Social jetlag and obesity. Current Biology, 22(10), 939943.10.1016/j.cub.2012.03.038CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roenneberg, T., Winnebeck, E. C., & Klerman, E. B. (2019). Daylight Saving Time and artificial time zones—A battle between biological and social times. Frontiers in Physiology, 10, 944.10.3389/fphys.2019.00944CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sadeh, A. (1996). Stress, trauma, and sleep in children. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 5, 685700.10.1016/S1056-4993(18)30356-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sadeh, A., Raviv, A., & Gruber, R. (2000). Sleep patterns and sleep disruptions in school-age children. Developmental Psychology, 36(3), 291301.10.1037/0012-1649.36.3.291CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saper, C. B., Scammell, T. E., & Lu, J. (2005). Hypothalamic regulation of sleep and circadian rhythms. Nature, 437(7063), 12571263.10.1038/nature04284CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schafer, J., & Holbein, J. B. (2020). When time is of the essence: A natural experiment on how time constraints influence elections. The Journal of Politics, 82(2), 418432.10.1086/706110CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sexton, A. L., & Beatty, T. K. (2014). Behavioral responses to Daylight Savings Time. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 107(Pt. A), 290307.10.1016/j.jebo.2014.03.012CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegel, J. M. (2005). Clues to the functions of mammalian sleep. Nature, 437(7063), 12641271.10.1038/nature04285CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slopen, N., Lewis, T. T., & Williams, D. R. (2016). Discrimination and sleep: A systematic review. Sleep Medicine, 18, 8895.10.1016/j.sleep.2015.01.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, A. C. (2016). Spring forward at your own risk: Daylight Saving Time and fatal vehicle crashes. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics, 8(2), 6591.Google Scholar
Stickgold, R. (2005). Sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Nature, 437(7063), 12721278.10.1038/nature04286CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Straus, L. D., Dolsen, E. A., Nishimi, K., Neylan, T. C., & O’Donovan, A. (2022). Worse sleep, worsening post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD): Poor sleep quality associated with increases in PTSD symptoms amongst those experiencing high threat of the COVID-19 pandemic. Journal of Sleep Research, 31(5), e13568.10.1111/jsr.13568CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taillard, J., Sagaspe, P., Philip, P., & Bioulac, S. (2021). Sleep timing, chronotype and social jetlag: Impact on cognitive abilities and psychiatric disorders. Biochemical Pharmacology, 191, 114438.10.1016/j.bcp.2021.114438CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tashjian, S. M., & Galván, A. (2020). Neural recruitment related to threat perception differs as a function of adolescent sleep. Developmental Science, 23(5), e12933.10.1111/desc.12933CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tausanovitch, C., Vavreck, L., Reny, T., Hayes, A. R., & Rudkin, A. (2019). Democracy Fund + UCLA Nationscape: Methodology and representativeness assessment. Democracy Fund Voter Study Group. https://www.voterstudygroup.org/uploads/reports/Data/NS-Methodology-Representativeness-Assessment.pdfGoogle Scholar
Thistlethwaite, D. L., & Campbell, D. T. (1960). Regression-discontinuity analysis: An alternative to the ex post facto experiment. Journal of Educational psychology, 51(6), 309.10.1037/h0044319CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tikotzky, L., & Sadeh, A. (2001). Sleep patterns and sleep disruptions in kindergarten children. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 30(4), 581591.10.1207/S15374424JCCP3004_13CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tobler, I., Gaus, S., Deboer, T., Achermann, P., Fischer, M., Rülicke, T., Moser, M., Oesch, B., McBride, P., & Manson, J. (1996). Altered circadian activity rhythms and sleep in mice devoid of prion protein. Nature, 380(6575), 639642.10.1038/380639a0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Urbatsch, R. (2014). Time regulations as electoral policy. American Politics Research, 42(5), 841855.10.1177/1532673X14523034CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valentino, N. A., Brader, T., Groenendyk, E. W., Gregorowicz, K., & Hutchings, V. L. (2011). Election night’s alright for fighting: The role of emotions in political participation. The Journal of Politics, 73(1), 156170.10.1017/S0022381610000939CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vandekerckhove, M., Weiss, R., Schotte, C., Exadaktylos, V., Haex, B., Verbraecken, J., & Cluydts, R. (2011). The role of presleep negative emotion in sleep physiology. Psychophysiology, 48(12), 17381744.10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01281.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
VanderWeele, T. J., & Mathur, M. B. (2019). Some desirable properties of the Bonferroni correction: Is the Bonferroni correction really so bad? American Journal of Epidemiology, 188(3), 617618.10.1093/aje/kwy250CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Varughese, J., & Allen, R. P. (2001). Fatal accidents following changes in Daylight Savings Time: The American experience. Sleep Medicine, 2(1), 3136.10.1016/S1389-9457(00)00032-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verhulst, B., Hatemi, P. K., & Martin, N. G. (2010). The nature of the relationship between personality traits and political attitudes. Personality and Individual Differences, 49(4), 306316.10.1016/j.paid.2009.11.013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, U., Gais, S., Haider, H., Born, J., et al. (2004). Sleep inspires insight. Nature, 427(6972), 352355.10.1038/nature02223CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, N. F., Badr, M. S., Belenky, G., Bliwise, D. L., Buxton, O. M., Buysse, D., Dinges, D. F., Gangwisch, J., & Grandner, M. A. (2015). Recommended amount of sleep for a healthy adult: A joint consensus statement of the american academy of sleep medicine and sleep research society. Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, 11(6), 591592.Google ScholarPubMed
Webster, S. W. (2020). American rage: How anger shapes our politics. Cambridge University Press.10.1017/9781108868303CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Westfall, P. H., & Young, S. S. (1993). Resampling-based multiple testing: Examples and methods for p-value adjustment. John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Wing, C., & Cook, T. D. (2013). Strengthening the regression discontinuity design using additional design elements: A within-study comparison. Journal of Policy Analysis and Management, 32(4), 853877.10.1002/pam.21721CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wong, P. M., Hasler, B. P., Kamarck, T. W., Muldoon, M. F., & Manuck, S. B. (2015). Social jetlag, chronotype, and cardiometabolic risk. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 100(12), 46124620.10.1210/jc.2015-2923CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xiong, J., Lipsitz, O., Nasri, F., Lui, L. M., Gill, H., Phan, L., Chen-Li, D., Iacobucci, M., Ho, R., Majeed, A., & McIntyre, R. S. (2020). Impact of COVID-19 pandemic on mental health in the general population: A systematic review. Journal of Affective Disorders, 277, 5564.10.1016/j.jad.2020.08.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
You, M., Laborde, S., Dosseville, F., Salinas, A., & Allen, M. S. (2020). Associations of chronotype, Big Five, and emotional competences with perceived stress in university students. Chronobiology International, 37(7), 10901098.10.1080/07420528.2020.1752705CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Youngstedt, S. D., Goff, E. E., Reynolds, A. M., Kripke, D. F., Irwin, M. R., Bootzin, R. R., Khan, N., & Jean-Louis, G. (2016). Has adult sleep duration declined over the last 50+ years? Sleep Medicine Reviews, 28, 6985.10.1016/j.smrv.2015.08.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zmigrod, L., Rentfrow, P. J., & Robbins, T. W. (2020). The partisan mind: Is extreme political partisanship related to cognitive inflexibility? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 149(3), 407–18.10.1037/xge0000661CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Summarizing the effect of starting DST on social views. (a) Rule out 20%? (no covariates) (b) Rule out 10%? (no covariates) (c) Rule out 20%? (with covariates) (d) Rule out 10%? (with covariates).Each cell summarizes an RDiT model specification. Points are shaped by whether we can rule out the effect size specified in each of the panel titles (20% or 10% of a standard deviation); squares can rule out the effects listed in each panel title, whereas circles cannot. Points are shaded by whether they are statistically significant at the (unadjusted) 5% level. Hence, effects that are statistically significant and substantively meaningful are black circles. Takeaway: The general pattern of evidence is precisely estimated nulls. This can be seen in most of the cells being gray squares (i.e., effects where we can rule out meaningful effects and the estimates are not significant at traditional levels), the next most common being black squares (i.e., effects where we can rule out meaningful effects, but that are still statistically distinct from 0), and only a minority being black circles (i.e., effects that are both statistically and substantively meaningful).

Figure 1

Figure 2. Distribution of effects at the spring and fall cutoffs. (a) Distribution of effect sizes, spring (b) Distribution of effect sizes, fall (c) Distribution of effects on binary outcomes, spring (d) Distribution of effects on binary outcomes, fall.Distribution of effect sizes for standardized outcomes broken by whether we have a standardized (top row) or a binary (bottom row) outcome. The columns signal whether we look at the effect of shifting from Standard Time to DST in the spring (left column) or DST to Standard Time in the fall (right column). Each of the histograms has a standard kernel density function (black lines) and a normal distribution (pink line) overlayed on top of it. Takeaway: Most of the estimated effects are quite small—falling within the bounds of what most standards would conceive to be small effects. Not surprisingly the fall treatment estimates (perhaps being confounded with the election itself) are also more noisy than the spring. Most of the effects themselves are still small.

Figure 2

Figure 3. The effect of starting DST on conservative ideology. (a) Effect on conservatism of all citizens (b) Effect on subgroups.The first panel shows bin averages in the dependent variable—conservative ideology—along with confidence intervals for these estimates. It also plots a smooth function separately on either side of the DST cutoff. The second panel shows the effects of DST on conservative ideology on all participants and four partisan subgroups (denoted on the x-axis). The y-axis is in standard deviation units, with higher values meaning the effect is to make people in that group more conservative and lower values meaning the opposite. Coefficients and standard errors are labeled for each of the estimates. Cohen’s effect sizes and a zero effect are shown as reference points with dotted and dashed lines, respectively. Points are sized as a function of sample size. Takeaway: There is no effect of the DST sleep disruption on individuals’ ideology, regardless of their political party.

Figure 3

Figure 4. The effect of starting DST on incumbent and challenger evaluations. (a) Effect of starting DST on incumbent evaluations (Trump) (b) Effect of starting DST on challenger evaluations (Biden).The first panel shows the effects on Trump approval and the second shows it for Biden approval. Estimates run for all participants and four partisan subgroups (denoted on the x-axis). The y-axis is in standard deviation units. Directional effects are labeled and shaded by their partisan implications. Coefficients and standard errors are labeled for each of the estimates. Cohen’s effect sizes and a zero effect are shown as reference points with dotted and dashed lines, respectively. Points are sized as a function of sample size. Takeaway: There is no effect of the DST sleep disruption on individuals’ evaluation of political leaders, regardless of their political party.

Figure 4

Figure 5. The effect of starting DST on polarization of out-party/in-party. Effect of DST’s sleep disruption on evaluations of the two major political parties. Estimates run for both major parties (denoted on the x-axis). The y-axis is in standard deviation units. Directional effects are labeled on the figure. Coefficients and standard errors are labeled for each of the estimates. Cohen’s effect sizes and a zero effect are shown as reference points with dotted and dashed lines, respectively. Takeaway: There is little to no effect of the DST sleep disruption on individuals’ evaluation of the other/their own political party.

Figure 5

Figure 6. The effect of starting DST on racial in- and out-group views. Effect of DST’s sleep disruption on evaluations of racial groups. Estimates run for both major parties (denoted on the x-axis). The y-axis is in standard deviation units. Directional effects are labeled on the figure. Coefficients and standard errors are labeled for each of the estimates. Cohen’s effect sizes and a zero effect are shown as reference points with dotted and dashed lines, respectively. Takeaway: There is little to no effect of the DST sleep disruption on individuals’ evaluation of the other/their own race.

Figure 6

Figure 7. The effect of starting DST on in- and out-group gender resentment. Effect of DST’s sleep disruption on evaluations of women (i.e., gender resentment). Estimates run for both major parties (denoted on the x-axis). The y-axis is in standard deviation units. Directional effects are labeled on the figure. Coefficients and standard errors are labeled for each of the estimates. Cohen’s effect sizes and a zero effect are shown as reference points with dotted and dashed lines, respectively. Takeaway: There is little to no effect of the DST sleep disruption on individuals’ gender resentment, regardless of gender.

Figure 7

Figure 8. The effect of starting DST on religious in- and out-group views. Effect of DST’s sleep disruption on evaluations of religious groups. Estimates run for both major parties (denoted on the x-axis). The y-axis is in standard deviation units. Directional effects are labeled on the figure. Coefficients and standard errors are labeled for each of the estimates. Cohen’s effect sizes and a zero effect are shown as reference points with dotted and dashed lines, respectively. Takeaway: There is little to no effect of the DST sleep disruption on individuals’ evaluation of the other/their own religion.

Supplementary material: PDF

Holbein and Crabtree supplementary material

Figures S1-S24 and Tables S1-S43
Download Holbein and Crabtree supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 850.3 KB