Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-lj6df Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-09T07:10:34.045Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Neuropolitics: Twenty years later

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 December 2017

Darren Schreiber*
Affiliation:
University of Exeter
*
Correspondence: Darren Schreiber, Department of Politics, University of Exeter, Mail Room, The Old Library, Prince of Wales Road, Exeter, Devon EX4 4SB, United Kingdom. Email: [email protected]
Get access

Abstract

Neuropolitics is the intersection of neuroscience and political science, and it has the interdisciplinary goal of transforming both disciplines. This article reviews the past 20 years of work in the field, identifying its roots, some overarching themes—reactions to political attitudinal questions and candidates faces, identification of political ideology based on brain structure or reactivity to nonpolitical stimuli, and racial attitudes—and obstacles to its progress. I then explore the methodological and analytical advances that point the way forward for the future of neuropolitics. Although the field has been slow to develop compared with neurolaw and neuroeconomics, innovations look ripe for dramatically improving our ability to model political behaviors and attitudes in individuals and predict political choices in mass publics. The coming advancements, however, pose risks to our current norms of democratic deliberation, and academics need to anticipate and mitigate these risks.

Type
Perspective
Copyright
© Association for Politics and the Life Sciences 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Converse, P., “The nature of belief systems in mass publics,” in Ideology and Discontent, Apter, D., ed. (New York: Free Press, 1964), pp. 206261.Google Scholar
Jost, J. T., Nam, H. H., Amodio, D. M., and Van Bavel, J. J., “Political neuroscience: The beginning of a beautiful friendship,” Political Psychology , 2014, 35: 342.Google Scholar
Fowler, J. H. and Schreiber, D., “Biology, politics, and the emerging science of human nature,” Science , 2008, 322(5903): 912914.Google Scholar
Schreiber, D., “Political cognition as social cognition: Are we all political sophisticates?,” in The Affect Effect: Dynamics of Emotion in Political Thinking and Behavior, Crigler, A., MacKuen, M., Marcus, G. E., and Neuman, W. R., eds. (Chicago: University of Chicago, 2007), pp. 4870.Google Scholar
Aristotle, The Politics and the Constitution of Athens, rev. student ed.(New York: Cambridge University Press, 1996).Google Scholar
Masters, R. D., The Nature of Politics (New Haven, CT: Yale University Press, 1989).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Waal, F. B. M., Chimpanzee Politics: Power and Sex among Apes, rev. ed. (Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press, 1998).Google Scholar
Holekamp, K. E., Sakai, S. T., and Lundrigan, B. L., “The spotted hyena (Crocuta crocuta) as a model system for study of the evolution of intelligence,” Journal of Mammalogy , 2007, 88(3): 545554.Google Scholar
Connor, R. C., Watson-Capps, J. J., Sherwin, W. B., and Krützen, M., “A new level of complexity in the male alliance networks of Indian Ocean bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp.),” Biology Letters , 2011, 7(4): 623626.Google Scholar
Schreiber, D., “Thinking about politics.” Unpublished seminar paper Department of Political Science, University of California, Los Angeles, 1998.Google Scholar
Aristotle, On the Soul (De Anima)(Grinnell, IA: Peripatetic Press, 1981).Google Scholar
Descartes, R., Meditations on First Philosophy: In Which the Existence of God and the Distinction of the Soul from the Body Are Demonstrated (Indianapolis, IN: Hackett, 1979).Google Scholar
Locke, J., An Essay Concerning Human Understanding: In Four Books (London: Eliz. Holt for Thomas Bassett, 1690).Google Scholar
Pinker, S., The Blank Slate: The Modern Denial of Human Nature (New York: Viking, 2002).Google Scholar
Zimmer, C., Soul Made Flesh: The Discovery of the Brain and How It Changed the World (London: Heinemann, 2004).Google Scholar
Freud, S., Zur auffassung der aphasien: Eine kritische studie: [On the concept of aphasia: A critical study] (Leipzig: F. Deuticke, 1891).Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F., The Behavior of Organisms: An Experimental Analysis (London: D. Appleton-Century, 1938).Google Scholar
Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., Miller, W. E., and Stokes, D. E., The American Voter (New York: Wiley, 1960).Google Scholar
Downs, A., An Economic Theory of Democracy (New York: HarperCollins, 1957).Google Scholar
Jackson, J. P. and Weidman, N. M., Race, Racism, and Science: Social Impact and Interaction (New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press, 2006).Google Scholar
Wahlke, J. C. and Lodge, M. G., “Psychophysiological measures of political attitudes and behavior,” Midwest Journal of Political Science , 1972, 16(4): 505537.Google Scholar
Eaves, L. J. and Eysenck, H. J., “Genetics and the development of social attitudes,” Nature , 1974, 249: 288289.Google Scholar
Sperry, R. W., Zaidel, E., and Zaidel, D., “Self recognition and social awareness in the disconnected minor hemisphere,” Neuropsychologia , 1979, 17: 153166.Google Scholar
Churchland, P. S. and Sejnowski, T. J., “Perspectives on cognitive neuroscience,” Science , 1988, 242(4879): 741745.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bandettini, P. A., “Functional MRI: A confluence of fortunate circumstances,” NeuroImage , 2012, 61(2): A3A11.Google Scholar
Broca, P. M., “The discovery of cerebral localization,” La Revue Du Practien , 1999, 49(16): 17251727.Google Scholar
Raichle, M. W., “Visualizing the mind,” Scientific American , 1994, 270: 5864.Google Scholar
Donders, F. C., “On the speed of mental processes,” in Attention and Performance, Koster, W. G., ed. (Amsterdam: North Holland, 1969), pp. 412431.Google Scholar
Zaller, J. R., The Nature and Origin of Mass Opinion (New York: Cambridge University Press, 1992).Google Scholar
Huckfeldt, R., Levine, J., Morgan, W., and Sprague, J., “Accessibility and political utility of partisan and ideological orientations,” American Journal of Political Science , 1999, 43(3): 888911.Google Scholar
Schreiber, D., “Looking into their minds: Latency in survey response as determined by political sophistication, issue publics, and cognitive conflict.” Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Midwest Political Science Association (Chicago, 2000).Google Scholar
Palmeri, T. J., “Theories of automaticity and the power law of practice,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition , 1999, 25(2): 543551.Google Scholar
Cabeza, R. and Nyberg, L., “Imaging cognition II: An empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studies,” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 2000, 12(1): 147.Google Scholar
Cabeza, R. and Nyberg, L., “Imaging cognition: An empirical review of PET studies with normal subjects,” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 1997, 9(1): 126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phelps, E. A., O’Connor, K. J., and Cunningham, W. et al. , “Perfomance on indirect measures of race evaluation predicts amygdala activation,” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 2000, 12(5): 729738.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Golby, A. J., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Chiao, J. Y., and Eberhardt, J. L., “Differntial responses in the fusiform region to same-race and other-race faces,” Nature , 2001, 4: 845850.Google Scholar
Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M., and Cohen, J. D., “An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment,” Science , 2001, 293(5537): 21052108.Google Scholar
Ochsner, K. N. and Lieberman, M. D., “The emergence of social cognitive neuroscience,” American Psychologist , 2001, 56(9): 717734.Google Scholar
Brooks, D., “The young and the neuro,” New York Times , 2009, October 12: A31.Google Scholar
Elshtain, J. B., “Consilience or creation? Roundtable review of E. O. Wilson’s Consilience: The Unity of Knowledge ,” Politics and the Life Sciences , 1999, 18(2): 341344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieberman, M. D., “A geographical history of social cognitive neuroscience,” NeuroImage , 2012, 61(2): 432436.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singer, T., “The past, present and future of social neuroscience: A European perspective,” NeuroImage , 2012, 61(2): 437449.Google Scholar
Raichle, M. E., “Social neuroscience: A role for brain imaging,” Political Psychology , 2003, 24(4): 759764.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cacioppo, J. T. and Visser, P. S., “Political psychology and social neuroscience: Strange bedfellows or comrades in arms?,” Political Psychology , 2003, 24(4): 647656.Google Scholar
Phelps, E. A. and Thomas, L., “Race, behavior, and the brain: The role of neuroimaging in understanding complex social behaviors,” Political Psychology , 2003, 24(4): 747758.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieberman, M., Schreiber, D., and Ochsner, K., “Is political cognition like learning to ride a bicycle? How cognitive neuroscience can inform research on political thinking,” Political Psychology , 2003, 24(4): 681704.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schreiber, D., Evaluating Politics: A Search for the Neural Substrates of Political Thought. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Department of Political Science, University of California, Los Angeles, 2005.Google Scholar
Westen, D., Blagov, P., Harenski, K., Kilts, C., and Hamann, S., “Neural bases of motivated reasoning: An fMRI study of emotional constraints on partisan political judgment in the 2004 U.S. presidential election,” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 2006, 18(11): 19471958.Google Scholar
Westen, D., The Political Brain: The Role of Emotion in Deciding the Fate of the Nation (New York: PublicAffairs, 2007).Google Scholar
Knutson, K. M., Wood, J. N., Spampinato, M. V., and Grafman, J., “Politics on the brain: An FMRI investigation,” Social Neuroscience , 2006, 1(1): 2540.Google Scholar
Spezio, M. L., Rangel, A., and Alvarez, R. M. et al. , “A neural basis for the effect of candidate appearance on election outcomes,” Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience , 2008, 3(4): 344352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaplan, J. T., Freedman, J., and Iacoboni, M., “Us versus them: Political attitudes and party affiliation influence neural response to faces of presidential candidates,” Neuropsychologia , 2007, 45(1): 5564.Google Scholar
Iacoboni, M., Freedman, J., and Kaplan, J. et al. , “This is your brain on politics,” New York Times , 2007, November 11.Google Scholar
Aron, A. R., Badre, D., and Brett, M. et al. , “Politics and the brain,” New York Times , 2007, November 14.Google Scholar
Axelrod, R., “Schema theory: An information processing model of perception and cognition,” American Political Science Review , 1973, 67(4): 12481266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amodio, D. M., Jost, J. T., Master, S. L., and Yee, C. M., “Neurocognitive correlates of liberalism and conservatism,” Nature Neuroscience , 2007, 10(10): 12461247.Google Scholar
Schreiber, D., Fonzo, G., and Simmons, A. N. et al. , “Red brain, blue brain: Evaluative processes differ in Democrats and Republicans,” PLOS ONE , 2013, 8: e52970. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0052970.Google Scholar
Oxley, D. R., Smith, K. B., and Alford, J. R. et al. , “Political attitudes vary with physiological traits,” Science , 2008, 321(5896): 16671670.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ahn, W. Y., Kishida, K. T., and Gu, X. et al. , “Nonpolitical images evoke neural predictors of political ideology,” Current Biology , 2014, 24(22): 26932699.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kanai, R., Feilden, T., Firth, C., and Rees, G., “Political orientations are correlated with brain structure in young adults,” Current Biology , 2011, 21(8): 677680.Google Scholar
Lieberman, M. D., Social: Why Our Brains Are Wired to Connect (New York: Crown, 2013).Google Scholar
Spezio, M. and Adolphs, R., “Politics and the evolving neuroscience literature,” in The Affect Effect: Dynamics of Emotion in Political Thinking and Behavior, Crigler, A., MacKuen, M., Marcus, G. E., and Neuman, W. R., eds. (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 2007), pp. 7195.Google Scholar
Jones, O. D., Marois, R., Farah, M. J., and Greely, H. T., “Law and neuroscience,” Journal of Neuroscience , 2013, 33(45): 1762417630.Google Scholar
Aharoni, E., Vincent, G. M., and Harenski, C. L. et al. , “Neuroprediction of future rearrest,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences , 2013, 110(15): 62236228.Google Scholar
Krueger, F., Grafman, J., and McCabe, K., “Neural correlates of economic game playing,” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B , 2008, 363(1511): 38593874.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawes, C. T., Loewen, P. J., and Schreiber, D. et al. , “Neural basis of egalitarian behavior,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences , 2012, 109(17): 64796483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ansolabehere, S. and Iyengar, S., Going Negative: How Attack Ads Shrink and Polarize the Electorate (New York: Free Press, 1995).Google Scholar
Delli Carpini, M. X. and Keeter, S., What Americans Know about Politics and Why It Matters (New Haven, CT: Yale University Press, 1996).Google Scholar
Amodio, D. M., “The neuroscience of prejudice and stereotyping,” Nature Reviews Neuroscience , 2014, 15: 670682.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schreiber, D. and Iacoboni, M., “Huxtables on the brain: An fMRI study of race and norm violation,” Political Psychology , 2012, 33(3): 307418.Google Scholar
Krastev, S., McGuire, J. T., and McNeney, D. et al. , “Do political and economic choices rely on common neural substrates? A systematic review of the emerging neuropolitics literature,” Frontiers in Psychology , 2016, 7: 264, doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00264.Google Scholar
Tingley, D., “Neurological imaging as evidence in political science: A review, critique, and guiding assessment,” Social Science Information , 2006, 45(1): 533.Google Scholar
Mendez, M. F., “A neurology of the conservative-liberal dimension of political ideology,” Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences , 2017, 29(2): 8694.Google Scholar
Tusche, A., Kahnt, T., Wisniewski, D., and Haynes, J.-D., “Automatic processing of political preferences in the human brain,” NeuroImage , 2013, 72: 174182.Google Scholar
Jenke, L. and Huettel, S. A., “Issues or identity? Cognitive foundations of voter choice,” Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 2016, 20(11): 794804.Google Scholar
Alford, J. R., Funk, C. L., and Hibbing, J. R., “Are political orientations genetically transmitted? American Political Science Review , 2005, 99(2): 153167.Google Scholar
Hibbing, J. R., Smith, K. B., and Alford, J. R., Predisposed: Liberals, Conservatives, and the Biology of Political Differences (New York: Routledge, 2013).Google Scholar
Haase, V. G. and Starling-Alves, I., “In search of the moral-psychological and neuroevolutionary basis of political partisanship,” Dementia & Neuropsychologia , 2017, 11(1): 1523.Google Scholar
Peterson, S. A. and Somit, A., eds., Handbook of Biology and Politics (Northhampton, MA: Edward Elgar, 2017).Google Scholar
Rosenberg, A., “Why social science is biological science,” Journal for General Philosophy of Science , 2017, 48(3): 341369.Google Scholar
Decety, J. and Cacioppo, S., “The speed of morality: A high-density electrical neuroimaging study,” Journal of Neurophysiology , 2012, 108: 30683072.Google Scholar
Yoder, K. J. and Decety, J., “Spatiotemporal neural dynamics of moral judgment: A high-density ERP study,” Neuropsychologia , 2014, 60: 3945.Google Scholar
Cui, X., Bray, S., Bryant, D. M., Glover, G. H., and Reiss, A. L., “A quantitative comparison of NIRS and fMRI across multiple cognitive tasks,” NeuroImage , 2011, 54(4): 28082821.Google Scholar
Gervain, J., Mehler, J., Werker, J. F., and Nelson, C. A. et al. , “Near-infrared spectroscopy: A report from the McDonnell infant methodology consortium,” Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience , 2011, 1(1): 2246.Google Scholar
Liu, N., Cui, X., Bryant, D. M., Glover, G. H., and Reiss, A. L., “Inferring deep-brain activity from cortical activity using functional near-infrared spectroscopy,” Biomedical Optics Express , 2015, 6(3): 10741089.Google Scholar
Gabriele Gratton, M. F., “Fast optical imaging of human brain function,” Frontiers in Human Neuroscience , 2010, 4: 52, doi:10.3389/fnhum.2010.00052.Google Scholar
Radhakrishnan, H., Vanduffel, W., Deng, H. P., and Ekstrom, L., “Fast optical signal not detected in awake behaving monkeys,” NeuroImage , 2009, 45(2): 410419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steinbrink, J., Kempf, F. C. D., Villringer, A., and Obrig, H., “The fast optical signal — Robust or elusive when non-invasively measured in the human adult? NeuroImage , 2005, 26(4): 9961008.Google Scholar
Macmillan, M., “Restoring Phineas Gage: A 150th retrospective,” Journal of the History of the Neurosciences , 2000, 9(1): 4666.Google Scholar
Lodge, M., McGraw, K., and Stroh, P., “An impression-driven model of candidate evaluation,” American Political Science Review , 1989, 83(2): 399419.Google Scholar
Coronel, J. C., Duff, M. C., and Warren, D. E. et al. , “Remembering and voting: Theory and evidence from amnesic patients,” American Journal of Political Science , 2012, 56(4): 837848.Google Scholar
Coronel, J., Memory and Voting: Neuropsychological and Electrophysiological Investigations of Voters Remembering Political Events. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Department of Political Science, University of Illinois, Champagne-Urbana, 2012.Google Scholar
Raymont, V., Salazar, A. M., Krueger, F., and Grafman, J., “‘Studying injured minds’: The Vietnam head injury study and 40 years of brain injury research,” Frontiers in Neurology , 2011, 2: 15, doi:10.3389/fneur.2011.00015.Google Scholar
Arciniegas, D. B. and Anderson, C. A., “Toward a neuroscience of politics,” Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences , 2017, 29(2): 8485.Google Scholar
King-Casas, B., Sharp, C., Lomax-Bream, L., Lohrenz, T., Fonagy, P., and Montague, P. R., “The rupture and repair of cooperation in borderline personality disorder,” Science , 2008, 321(5890): 806810.Google Scholar
Ruocco, A. C., Medaglia, J. D., Tinker, J. R., and Ayaz, H. et al. , “Medial prefrontal cortex hyperactivation during social exclusion in borderline personality disorder,” Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging , 2010, 181(3): 233236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunbar, E., “The relationship of DSM personality disorder criteria and Gough’s prejudice scale: Exploring the clinical manifestations of the prejudiced personality,” Cultural Diversity and Mental Health , 1997, 3(4): 247258.Google Scholar
Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Merikangas, K. R., and Walters, E. E., “Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication,” Archives of General Psychiatry , 2005, 62(6): 593602.Google Scholar
Young, L., Camprodon, J. A., Hauser, M., Pascual-Leone, A., and Saxe, R., “Disruption of the right temporoparietal junction with transcranial magnetic stimulation reduces the role of beliefs in moral judgments,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences , 2010, 107(15): 67536758.Google Scholar
Nitsche, M. A., Cohen, L. G., and Wassermann, E. M. et al. , “Transcranial direct current stimulation: State of the art 2008,” Brain Stimulation , 2008, 1(3): 206223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knoch, D., Nitsche, M. A., Fischbacher, U., Eisenegger, C., Pascual-Leone, A., and Fehr, E., “Studying the neurobiology of social interaction with transcranial direct current stimulation: The example of punishing unfairness,” Cerebral Cortex , 2008, 18(9): 19871990.Google Scholar
Horvath, J. C., Forte, J. D., and Carter, O., “Quantitative review finds no evidence of cognitive effects in healthy populations from single-session transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS),” Brain Stimulation , 2015, 8(3): 535550.Google Scholar
Grossman, N., Bono, D., and Dedic, N. et al. , “Noninvasive deep brain stimulation via temporally interfering electric fields,” Cell , 2017, 169(6): 10291041.Google Scholar
Hess, R. D. and Torney-Purta, J. V., The Development of Political Attitudes in Children (Chicago: Aldine, 1967).Google Scholar
Jennings, M. K. and Niemi, R. G., “The transmission of political values from parent to child,” American Political Science Review , 1968, 62(1): 169184.Google Scholar
Sapiro, V., “Not your parents’ political socialization: Introduction for a new generation,” Annual Review of Political Science , 2004, 7: 123.Google Scholar
Kagan, J., “Biology, context, and developmental inquiry,” Annual Review of Psychology , 2003, 54: 123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., and Lombardo, M., Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from Developmental Social Neuroscience (New York Oxford University Press, 2013).Google Scholar
Hatemi, P. K., Funk, C., and Medland, S. et al. , “Genetic and environmental transmission of political attitudes over a life time,” Journal of Politics , 2009, 71(3): 11411156.Google Scholar
Happé, F. and Frith, U., “Annual research review: Towards a developmental neuroscience of atypical social cognition,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry , 2013, 55(6): 553577.Google Scholar
Santos, A., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., and Deruelle, C., “Absence of racial, but not gender, stereotyping in Williams syndrome children,” Current Biology , 2010, 20(7): R307308.Google Scholar
Kurzban, R., Tooby, J., and Cosmides, L., “Can race be erased? Coalitional computation and social categorization,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences , 2001, 98(26): 1538715392.Google Scholar
Balconi, M. and Vanutelli, M. E., “Competition in the brain: The contribution of EEG and fNIRS modulation and personality effects in social ranking,” Frontiers in Psychology , 2016, 7: 1587, doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01587.Google Scholar
van der Zwaag, W., Da Costa, S. E., Zürcher, N. R., Adams, R. B., and Hadjikhani, N., “A 7 Tesla fMRI study of amygdala responses to fearful faces,” Brain Topography , 2017, 25(2): 125128.Google Scholar
Vu, A. T., Jamison, K., and Glasser, M. F. et al. , “Tradeoffs in pushing the spatial resolution of fMRI for the 7T Human Connectome Project,” NeuroImage , 2017, 154: 2332.Google Scholar
Beisteiner, R., Robinson, S., Wurnig, M., and Hilbert, M., “Clinical fMRI: Evidence for a 7T benefit over 3T,” NeuroImage , 2011, 57(3): 10151021.Google Scholar
Yoo, P. E., John, S. E., Farquharson, S., Cleary, J. O., and Wong, Y. T., “7T-fMRI: Faster temporal resolution yields optimal BOLD sensitivity for functional network imaging specifically at high spatial resolution,” NeuroImage , published online March 8, 2017, doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.03.002.Google Scholar
Vul, E., Harris, C., Winkielman, P., and Pashler, H., “Puzzlingly high correlations in fMRI studies of emotion, personality, and social cognition,” Perspectives on Psychological Science , 2009, 4(3): 274290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pereira, F., Mitchell, T., and Botvinick, M., “Machine learning classifiers and fMRI: A tutorial overview,” NeuroImage , 2009, 45(1 Suppl): S199S209.Google Scholar
Norman, K. A., Polyn, S. M., Detre, G. J., and Haxby, J. V., “Beyond mind-reading: Multi-voxel pattern analysis of fMRI data,” Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 2006, 10(9): 424430.Google Scholar
Henson, R., “Forward inference using functional neuroimaging: Dissociations versus associations,” Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 2006, 10(2): 6469.Google Scholar
Poldrack, R. A., “Can cognitive processes be inferred from neuroimaging data? Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 2006, 10(2): 5963.Google Scholar
Theodoridis, A. and Nelson, A., “Of BOLD claims and excessive fears: A call for caution and patience regarding political neuroscience,” Political Psychology , 2012, 33(1): 2743.Google Scholar
Haxby, J. V., Gobbini, M. I., Furey, M. L., Ishai, A., Schouten, J. L., and Pietrini, P., “Distributed and overlapping representations of faces and objects in ventral temporal cortex,” Science , 2001, 293(5539): 24252430.Google Scholar
Kay, K. N., Naselaris, T., Prenger, R. J., and Gallant, J. L., “Identifying natural images from human brain activity,” Nature , 2008, 452: 352355.Google Scholar
Huth, A. G., Lee, T., Nishimoto, S., Bilenko, N. Y., Vu, A. T., and Gallant, J. L., “Decoding the semantic content of natural movies from human brain activity,” Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience , 2016, 10: 81, doi:10.3389/fnsys.2016.00081.Google Scholar
Berkman, E. T. and Falk, E. B., “Beyond brain mapping: Using neural measures to predict real-world outcomes,” Current Directions in Psychological Science , 2013, 22(1): 4550.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Falk, E. B., Berkman, E. T., Mann, T., Harrison, B., and Lieberman, M. D., “Predicting persuasion-induced behavior change from the brain,” Journal of Neuroscience , 2010, 30(25): 84218424.Google Scholar
Falk, E. B., Berkman, E. T., Whalen, D., and Lieberman, M. D., “Neural activity during health messaging predicts reductions in smoking above and beyond self-report,” Health Psychology , 2011, 30(2): 177185.Google Scholar
Lieberman, M. D., Gaunt, R., Gilbert, D. T., and Trope, Y., “Reflection and reflexion: A social cognitive neuroscience approach to attributional inference,” Advances in Experimental Social Psychology , 2002, 34: 199249.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., Thinking, Fast and Slow (New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux, 2013).Google Scholar
Falk, E. B., Berkman, E. T., and Lieberman, M. D., “From neural responses to population behavior: Neural focus group predicts population-level media effects,” Psychological Science , 2012, 23(5): 439445.Google Scholar
Genevsky, A. and Knutson, B., “Neural affective mechanisms predict market-level microlending,” Psychological Science , 2015, 26(9): 14111422.Google Scholar
Venkatraman, V., Dimoka, A., and Pavlou, P. A., “Predicting advertising success beyond traditional measures: New insights from neurophysiological methods and market response modeling,” Journal of Marketing Research , 2015, 52(4): 436452.Google Scholar
Randall, K., “Neuropolitics, where campaigns try to read your mind,” New York Times , 2015, November, 4, p. A1.Google Scholar
Malkin, E. and Randall, K., “Mexico’s governing party vows to stop using neuromarketing to study voters,” New York Times , 2015, November, 12, p. A12.Google Scholar
Varshney, L., Chen, B., Paniagua, E., Hall, D., and Chklovskii, D., “Structural properties of the Caenorhabditis elegans neuronal network,” PLOS Computational Biology , 2011, 7: e1001066, doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1001066.Google Scholar
Wade, N., “In tiny worm, unlocking secrets of the brain,” New York Times , 2011, June 21, p. D1.Google Scholar