The elections of 1992 were proclaimed by the media (and some scholars; see Cook, Thomas, and Wilcox Reference Cook, Thomas and Wilcox1994) as The Year of the Woman. During an era when female candidates for high political office were still something of a novelty, Dianne Feinstein and Barbara Boxer (both Democrats) won U.S. Senate races against male Republican opponents, thereby making California the first state to be represented in the Senate by two women. In their benchmark study of campaign advertising, much of Ansolabehere and Iyengar's (Reference Ansolabehere and Iyengar1995) experimental work focused on these two races and one more, the 1990 contest in which incumbent Pete Wilson narrowly defeated Feinstein for governor. Although the major-party nominees in all three elections were of opposite gender, the authors were more concerned with assessing the effects of campaign advertising generally, and of negative advertising in particular, in races where one of the contestants happened to be a woman. The broader question of whether going negative is more (or less) effective for women or men was left unanswered and is the focus of our article.
Although women continue to be underrepresented in American public life, the political landscape has changed dramatically since 1992. More women are running for office today, and often winning, in part because they have become increasingly adept at the art of negative campaigning. That, at least, is one conclusion that might be drawn based on the insights of political insiders. Whether or not the overall tone of political discourse is more negative than in the past,Footnote 1 there is little doubt that negativity has become a staple of campaigns at all levels. While positive ads may be an important element in one's overall communications package (Franz et al. Reference Franz, Freedman, Goldstein and Ridout2008), attacks are more likely to be viewed as game changers or game deciders, especially for challengers, who are said to have little chance of winning unless they go negative (Geer Reference Geer2006; Mattes and Redlawsk Reference Mattes and Redlawsk2014).
All of this would seem to make negativity an indispensable weapon in the arsenal of female candidates, who are more likely than men to be challengers and therefore to lack the resources (such as money) that usually come with incumbency. The catch is that going negative may pose a special risk for women because it violates expectations about appropriate behavior that are rooted in the traditional gender stereotypes held by some voters even in the 21st century (Dolan Reference Dolan2014; but see Brooks Reference Brooks2013). As Trent, Friedenberg, and Denton (2011, 164–65) explain, “[w]hen women speak, they are expected to exhibit characteristics such as sensitivity to the needs of others, concern for family and relationships, compassion, emotionality, affection, and nurturing. They are not expected to employ harsh language or to be overtly assertive, either verbally or physically.”Footnote 2
Politicians whose behavior does not conform to these expectations are thought to risk having their message rejected by voters (Trent, Friedenberg, and Denton Reference Trent, Friedenberg and Denton2011, 165). As a result, political consultants once advised women—and perhaps some still do—that attacking their opponent could generate a backlash and jeopardize their chances for electoral success (Gordon, Shafie, and Cigler Reference Gordon, Shafie and Cigler2003, 36), especially if the attack is delivered personally by the candidate and delivered in a shrill or strident manner (Nelson Reference Nelson2009, 58–59).Footnote 3 Nevertheless, a review of televised campaign ads posted on YouTube, as well as some academic research (for example, see Herrnson and Lucas Reference Herrnson and Lucas2006; Nelson Reference Nelson2009; Panagopoulos Reference Panagopoulos2004), confirms the willingness of many female candidates to go negative. Whether they lose more than they gain in doing so is inconclusive.Footnote 4
In this article, we examine gender differences in the effectiveness of one attack made by a challenger against an incumbent of the opposite sex in a hypothetical race for the U.S. House of Representatives. Our interest, however, is not limited to the attack itself but extends to the question of how candidates should respond when they are attacked—and whether certain types of responses work better for men than they do for women, and vice versa, in terms of mitigating the damage inflicted by an initially successful negative ad. We know very little about the relative effectiveness of different types of responses and almost nothing about whether such effectiveness is a function of candidate gender. This article represents a modest first step in trying to rectify that situation.
NEGATIVE ADS AND CANDIDATE GENDER
No campaign ad is effective simply because of its negativity. In addition to being well-crafted (see Craig and Hill Reference Craig and Hill2011, 136–37), a negative ad should work best when it is hard-hitting (Fridkin and Kenney Reference Fridkin and Kenney2008, Reference Fridkin and Kenney2011), relevant (not tangential to “governing performance” or dealing with matters of little concern to voters; Fridkin and Kenney Reference Fridkin and Kenney2008, 697), more or less accurate (the facts presented may be exaggerated, distorted, or presented out of context, but they should not be demonstrably false; Johnson Reference Johnson2007, 59), specific about what the target has done wrong (broad accusations of misbehavior are not sufficient), and offered in support of a credible opponent (candidates with little chance of winning will have trouble getting voters’ attention).Footnote 5
What is the impact of campaign advertising on voter decision making? Whereas candidates, their consultants, party leaders, and political journalists believe that it is often substantial and occasionally decisive, academic studies have yielded mixed results regarding the effectiveness of political advertising generally, and of negative ads in particular. It seems likely that the inconsistent findings of this literature are due in some degree to the fact that negativity lies in the eye of the beholder; that is, “whether a tactic, a candidate, or a campaign is [perceived as] negative depends on whose ox is being gored” (Sigelman and Kugler Reference Sigelman and Kugler2003, 144). In particular, research shows that partisans react differently to attacks coming from the other side than they do to accusations made by candidates of their own party (Ansolabehere and Iyengar Reference Ansolabehere and Iyengar1995; Geer Reference Geer2006; Iyengar, Jackman, and Hahn Reference Iyengar, Jackman and Hahn2008; Stevens et al. Reference Stevens, Allen, Sullivan and Lawrence2015).
We suspect that partisanship also has much to do with how voters react to negative ads sponsored by women. As noted earlier, some individuals “may rely heavily on gender stereotypes when evaluating a female candidate who goes on the attack (Gordon, Shafie, and Cigler Reference Gordon, Shafie and Cigler2003, 36), withholding their support for women who violate traditional sex role expectations by attacking their opponents during a campaign. This tendency, to the extent that it exists, probably varies across voters, contexts, issues (see Ditonto, Hamilton, and Redlawsk Reference Ditonto, Hamilton and Redlawsk2014; Dolan Reference Dolan2014; Gordon, Shafie, and Cigler Reference Gordon, Shafie and Cigler2003), and other alleged transgressions that form the basis for campaign attacks. But above all else, we anticipate that it will vary with the party affiliation of the candidate.Footnote 6 According to a recent survey experiment by Krupnikov and Bauer (Reference Krupnikov and Bauer2014), voters are more likely to punish women relative to men (based on gender stereotypes) only when they (a) are seen as having instigated the negativity and (b) represent the opposite party. Setting aside the question of instigation,Footnote 7 we expect a similar result here:
H1:
A well-crafted attack by either a man or a woman is more effective among voters who share the attacking candidate's party affiliation.
H2:
Among voters who identify with the opposing/target party (not fellow partisans or independents), attacks by female candidates are less effective than those made by male candidates.
It is possible, of course, that shared gender also plays a role. Based on identity politics, one might anticipate that women will respond differently than men to attacks made by a female candidate precisely because the latter is a woman (e.g., Plutzer and Zipp Reference Plutzer and Zipp1996; Rosenthal Reference Rosenthal1995; Sanbonmatsu Reference Sanbonmatsu2002). This would presumably make female voters less likely to respond favorably to attacks made by men against women candidates and/or more likely to be moved by attacks made by women against their male opponents. On the other hand, prior research (Dolan Reference Dolan2008, 2014; Dolan and Lynch Reference Dolan and Lynch2014; Hayes Reference Hayes2011; Sanbonmatsu and Dolan Reference Sanbonmatsu and Dolan2009) has found that party stereotypes (and to a lesser extent, incumbency) largely override gender stereotypes in terms of their impact on voter decisions in mixed-gender races. Will we observe a similar imbalance with regard to the relative importance of shared partisanship versus shared gender in shaping citizens’ reactions to campaign communications? Playing devil's advocate, our third hypothesis suggests otherwise:
H3:
A well-crafted attack is more effective among voters who share the attacking candidate's gender.
Assuming that an attack inflicts some damage in terms of diminished vote support and lower favorability ratings (and the ad we tested certainly did that), what is the best way for a candidate to respond? One option is to stay on message while ignoring the specific charges. Contrary to what many practitioners believed in the early days of negative TV ads (Johnson-Cartee and Copeland Reference Johnson-Cartee and Copeland1991), this is no longer considered to be a wise strategy. A more promising approach involves denial: specifically, if the target can credibly argue that the attack is based on false information, s/he should quickly do so because there is a fairly good chance that the offending campaign will be penalized by voters for its actions (Johnson Reference Johnson2007; Johnson-Cartee and Copeland Reference Johnson-Cartee and Copeland1991).Footnote 8
Communications scholars have identified a number of other possible responses, four of which are considered (along with denials) in our analysis:
-
• counterattacks, the content of which may have little to do with the original attack;
-
• mudslinging, a type of counterattack that ignores the original charges and criticizes the opponent for waging a negative campaign, especially if s/he has promised not to do so;
-
• counterimaging, a positive message that flips the attack on its head by “laying out for the voter a counterproposition to the content of the opponent's negative ad,” (e.g., police officers praising the record of someone accused of being soft on crime; see Johnson-Cartee and Copeland Reference Johnson-Cartee and Copeland1991, 244);Footnote 9 and
-
• justifications, which acknowledge the behavior and accept responsibility but attempt to downplay its negative consequences.Footnote 10
Consultants would probably agree with Republican consultant David Hill that in most instances “the best response is to make a counter-charge” (Ansolabehere and Iyengar Reference Ansolabehere and Iyengar1995, 15; Craig and Hill Reference Craig and Hill2011, 141), though our research (Craig, Rippere, and Grayson Reference Craig, Rippere and Grayson2012, 2014) indicates that all of these responses except for accusations of mudslinging are more or less equally effective at mitigating the initial damage done by an attack.
Is this as true for women candidates as it is for men? The literature, which focuses far more on attacks than on responses, provides almost no guidance regarding what gender differences we might expect to find. The same logic that has been applied to attacks might also hold true for counterattacks (including accusations of mudslinging); that is, because of the gender stereotypes held by many voters, there may be a disproportionate risk of backlash for female candidates who respond to negative ads in kind. As noted earlier, however, empirical support for this argument on the attack side is mixed at best. Further, according to Krupnikov and Bauer (Reference Krupnikov and Bauer2014), opposing partisans often react differently to negativity depending on which candidate is perceived as having cast the first stone. Because the attacker in our simulation is, by definition, the “instigator,” there is no reason to anticipate that counterattacks will be less effective for women than for men. Absent any prior studies that might suggest otherwise, we employ the null hypothesis as our benchmark:
H4:
No response type is significantly more (or less) effective for male candidates than for females.
As with attacks ( H 1 ), we expect partisanship to have a strong influence on the effectiveness of any response ad regardless of the candidate's gender:
H5:
Any response, whether delivered by a man or a woman, is more effective among voters who share that candidate's party affiliation.
However, following H 3 , we also predict that shared gender will have an impact:
H6:
Any response, whether delivered by a man or a woman, is more effective among voters who share that candidate's gender.
STUDY DESIGN
The current study, which is part of a larger project designed to assess the impact of both attacks and responses in political campaigns (Craig, Rippere, and Grayson Reference Craig, Rippere and Grayson2012, 2014; Craig and Rippere Reference Craig and Rippere2014), examines 662 voters who completed an Internet survey conducted June 21–24, 2012. Data were provided by qSample (see www.qSample.com), a market research firm that has recruited more than five million individuals to participate in research projects related to video gaming, home building/contracting, home ownership, issues of particular interest to college students and Baby Boomers, as well as politics. Respondents for our survey were drawn from a national panel of registered voters, geographically balanced by region, whose members engage in ad testing, focus groups, and in-depth interviewing on a range of politically relevant topics.Footnote 11
For our survey experiment, respondents were randomly assigned to one of twenty treatment groups and asked to complete a background questionnaire that measured basic demographics, political knowledge, party identification, and a number of other political orientations.Footnote 12 They were then told to imagine that it was the fall of 2012, and one of the races on their ballot involved a congressional matchup between an incumbent seeking a third term and an experienced challenger who had served in both local office and the state legislature. After reading short biographies of the candidates,Footnote 13 participants were asked to indicate a vote preference (“Based on the information you currently have, which candidate would you vote for if the election were held today?”) and to rate each candidate on a 7-point scale ranging from “very unfavorable” (1) to “very favorable” (7); answers to these three questions served as the dependent variables for our analysis. Participants subsequently read what was described as a direct-mail attackFootnote 14 by the challenger and once again registered their vote choice and candidate assessments. Finally, they were given the incumbent's response and answered the vote and candidate questions a third time.Footnote 15
In crafting the attacks, we opted not to employ policy appeals that would likely be viewed through a partisan/ideological lens by many voters (Iyengar, Jackman, and Hahn Reference Iyengar, Jackman and Hahn2008; also see Druckman, Peterson, and Slothuus Reference Druckman, Peterson and Slothuus2013; Lavine, Johnson, and Steenbergen Reference Lavine, Johnston and Steenbergen2012; Stevens et al. Reference Stevens, Allen, Sullivan and Lawrence2015). Our focus was instead on performance-based attacks wherein a challenger alleges that “the incumbent has lost his touch with the people back home, doesn't work hard, doesn't stand on principle and changes his mind to please different people, has used the office for personal gain, [and] will say just about anything to get reelected” (Johnson Reference Johnson2007, 65). This seems to be an especially good basis for evaluation when the target is an incumbent, as is the case in our experimental manipulations.Footnote 16
The attack employed in this study (taking advantage) charged the incumbent with “helping himself to other people's money” in a number of ways: voting for pay raises, using party money to finance a family vacation, renting office space from his brother at inflated prices, overbilling clients for professional services, and channeling no-bid government contracts to campaign donors.Footnote 17 These allegations, regarding matters that are clearly relevant to the target's performance as an elected representative, were made in language that might not be considered “uncivil” but comes very close to that line by using such emotionally charged terms as “corrupt,” “immoral,” and “deserves to be in jail more than in Congress.”Footnote 18 Each participant read the text for taking advantage plus one of the five responses described earlier (see online Appendix B for more information). The impact of candidate gender was tested using a within-subjects design with the following parameters:
1 (attack type) × 5 (response type) × 4 (party affiliation + gender combinations) = a total of 20 treatment groups, to which participants were randomly assigned.
Specifically, for each attack-response pair, four treatment conditions were created: male Republican incumbent attacked by female Democrat, female Republican incumbent attacked by male Democrat, male Democratic incumbent attacked by female Republican, and female Democratic incumbent attacked by male Republican.Footnote 19
RESULTS
Effectiveness of the Attack
Do negative ads work? This one clearly did, at least in terms of its initial impact. Looking at all respondents, the top row in Table 1 indicates that support for the incumbent dropped 18.1 points (from 50.3 to 32.2%),Footnote 20 while his/her favorability score fell by more than a point and a half on the 7-point scale (from 4.624 to 3.000) following the attack; both of these changes are statistically significant (p < .001)Footnote 21 and substantively large. While some backlash is evident in the fact that sponsor favorability declined by 0.512 points (from 4.631 to 4.119, p < .001), there appears to be a sizeable net advantage in these instances to going negative.
Note: For vote choice, difference is calculated as proportion (post-attack vote) minus proportion (baseline vote); values have been converted to percentages. For favorability, difference is calculated as mean (post-attack) minus mean (baseline). Significance tests are 2-tailed. For the effect of the attack, *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001. For significant differences across candidate gender, italics p < .05; bold p < .01
According to H 1 , a well-crafted attack made by any candidate should be more effective with voters who share that candidate's party affiliation. As shown in rows 2–4 of Table 1, however, the attack significantly altered the vote intentions of the target's co-partisans (a decline of 29.9 points) and independents (−22.4 points), while the preferences of those identifying with the attacker's party remained largely unchanged (−4.0 points, p = 0.122). While this result might seem counterintuitive, it undoubtedly reflects the fact that few of the latter group (just 11.1%) planned to vote for the target in the first place, based solely on the candidates’ biosketches; in other words, there was considerably more potential for movement among the target's co-partisans (and, to a lesser degree, among independents, 56.4% of whom initially said they would vote to reelect).
Changes in candidate favorability were more evenly balanced across the three groups, with incumbent ratings dropping about 1.7 points (on a 7-point scale) among both fellow and opposing partisans and nearly 1.3 points among independents. Similarly, backlash against the attacking challenger was greatest among the incumbent's co-partisans (−0.643 points) but evident among the attacker's co-partisans and independents (−0.415 and −0.467, respectively) as well. Such backlash aside, however, our results indicate that shared partisanship does not always shield candidates from the impact of hard-hitting criticism by their opponents. This is not to say that party plays no role in determining how people react to campaign ads; it merely suggests that when voters encounter negative information about a candidate whom they are otherwise inclined to support (especially absent a response from that candidate), shared partisanship may not be enough to prevent a loss of support.
The results in the bottom half of Table 1 more directly address our central research question of whether voters are either more or less receptive to negative ads by male or female candidates in mixed-gender races. For the full sample, we can see that our hypothetical female challenger fared less well than her male opponent in every category: her attack produced a smaller decline in both incumbent vote share (−14.7 percentage points versus −21.7 for the male) and incumbent favorability (−1.524 points versus −1.730), while generating greater backlash in terms of lower favorability scores for herself (−0.568 points vs. −0.453). While this pattern suggests that going negative may be slightly less effective for women than for men, the effects of the attack, when delivered by our female candidate, were not significantly different from the effects of the attack when it was made by her male counterpart.Footnote 22
Following Krupnikov and Bauer (Reference Krupnikov and Bauer2014), H 2 posits that attacks by women will be less effective than those made by men among the incumbent's fellow partisans. We find some evidence of this but not in every instance and not always in the manner anticipated. Among the incumbent's co-partisans, where H 2 predicts that women will be at a disadvantage, the only significant gender difference is for challenger favorability: there was a larger backlash against our female attacker (−0.897 points) compared with the male (−0.373 points, p < .01). Also, contrary to H 2 , we see in Table 1 that women challengers had less success than men in reducing the incumbent's vote share among their own fellow partisans (+2.4 points versus −10 for the male, p < .05). Interestingly, among independents there was less backlash against the female attacker in terms of diminished favorability (−0.253 points versus −0.730 for the male, p < .01); although the same result is not evident for vote preference and incumbent favorability, this suggests that independents may in some instances be even less likely than partisans to invoke traditional gender stereotypes.
Overall, the findings here provide little support for H 2 . Whereas an attack by the woman may have been somewhat less effective than one by the man, differences between the two were small, inconsistent, and evident at times among fellow and opposing partisans alike (with independents exhibiting a modest tendency in the other direction).
What, then, about shared gender? According to H 3 , the attack should have had a greater effect among voters who share the attacker's gender. Rows 5–6 of Table 1 show that the attack caused the incumbent/target to lose ground in terms of both vote share and favorability—and, for the most part, this was equally true (statistically speaking) among those who shared the target's gender and those who did not. There are, however, three exceptions, all involving significantly greater movementFootnote 23 in favorability ratings among female respondents (see the bottom half of Table 1): When a female candidate attacked a male incumbent, the effect on incumbent favorability was more pronounced among women (−1.690 versus −1.349 for men, p < .05), as was backlash against the attacker (−0.707 versus −0.422 for men, p < 0.05). While we predicted that voters would react differently to criticism depending on whether it is directed at in-group (same-sex) or out-group candidates, at least for this sample and this particular campaign attack, no such bias is evident.Footnote 24
Examining differences across male and female candidates, we again find the impact of shared gender to be inconsistent. An attack by the male candidate is significantly more effective than one by the female in two instances: (a) target favorability drops more sharply among women voters when a male challenger attacks a female incumbent (−1.864 points) than it does among men when a female challenger attacks a male incumbent (−1.349, p < .01); and (b) backlash in the form of lower challenger favorability is less pronounced among men voters when a male candidate attacks (−0.379) than it is among women when a female candidate attacks (−0.707, p < .05). Shared gender does not appear to matter much in other areas, including the all-important category of vote intention.
Effectiveness of Responses
As reported elsewhere (Craig, Rippere, and Grayson Reference Craig, Rippere and Grayson2014), four of the five active responses examined here—all except accusations of mudslinging—were more or less equally effective at mitigating the damage done by an attack. With little theoretical reason to believe otherwise, we hypothesized ( H 4 ) that no response type would be more or less effective for women than for men. The results in Table 2 bear out that expectation, with two limited exceptions noted:
-
• Denials seemed to work better for male incumbents, especially in terms of recovered vote share (+23.1 points from post-attack to post-response versus +12.9 for women).
-
• Female incumbents fared better when justifying their actions to voters (+32.2 percentage points vote share versus +17.5 for men; +1.220 points incumbent favorability versus +0.857, and −0.898 points challenger favorability versus −0.508, for men).
Note: For vote choice, difference is calculated as proportion (post-response vote) minus proportion (post-attack vote); values have been converted to percentages. For favorability, difference is calculated as mean (post-response) minus mean (post-attack). Significance tests are 2-tailed. For the effect of the response, *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001. For significant differences across candidate gender, italics p < .05; bold p < .01.
However, none of the gender differences shown in the top portion of Table 2 are statistically significant at p < .05 or better; thus, for the most part, H 4 is clearly confirmed for each of our dependent variables.Footnote 25
Our next hypothesis ( H 4 ) states that any response, whether delivered by a man or a woman, will be most effective among the incumbent/responder's fellow partisans. Looking at the bottom half of Table 2, we find this to be generally true. With regard to vote preference (columns 1 and 2), denials, counterattacks, and justifications produced significant (p < .05) gains among co-partisans for women and men alike—but not, in the majority of cases, among opposing partisans or independents. In contrast, counterimaging and accusations of mudslinging failed to generate significant change in any partisan category.
The pattern for incumbent favorability is different, at least with respect to the effects of shared partisanship. First, four of the five responses (counterattack being the exception) were associated with improved ratings (p < .05) among fellow partisans for male and female incumbents alike. Second, however, significant or near-significant gains were also registered for some responses among opposing partisans (denials, counterimaging, and justifications for incumbents regardless of gender). Nevertheless, the results are generally consistent with H 5 : response messages of all types resonated more fully—though not exclusively—and to an approximately equal degree for men and women among the responding candidate's fellow partisans.
Challenger favorability tells a more complex story, with some new variations on the broader theme. For the sample as a whole, four of the five responses (counterimaging being the exception) produced a significant decline in challenger favorability. As it happens, this decline was not always greatest among the incumbent/responder's fellow partisans. Counterattacks, for example, worked about as well among those who identified with the attacker's party and independents as they did among the incumbent's co-partisans—something that was true for both male and female candidates. The most interesting gender anomaly is that charges of mudslinging were more effective (p < .05) for women candidates than for men among their co-partisans (see Table 2, row 10, last two columns). Still, post-response shifts in challenger favorability appear to be mostly in line with our earlier conclusions. Overall, our findings suggest that shared partisanship does play a positive, if not always a strong or consistent, role in shaping voters’ reactions to response messages in a campaign, and these reactions do not vary systematically for participants according to whether the responding candidate is a man or a woman.
Finally, Table 3 examines the impact of shared gender on the effectiveness of our five responses. Contrary to H 6 , there is little here to suggest that any type of response was more (or less) effective among voters who were of the same gender as the incumbent/target. Rather, each response had similar effects on vote choice and candidate ratings for both men and women, regardless of the incumbent's gender. The only instance where a significant (p < .05) difference is observed between respondents who share the gender of the incumbent and those who do not is when the male incumbent employs a counterimaging response—and in this case, incumbent favorability increased less for in-group (+0.500 points among men) than for out-group voters (+1.273 points among women). No other cross-group differences were significant. Likewise, in no instance do we observe a difference between the performance of female and male incumbents that is significant at p < .05. Overall, these results appear to affirm our earlier conclusion that the gender of respondents and candidates has little influence on a political campaign.
Note: For vote choice, difference is calculated as proportion (post-response vote) minus proportion (post-attack vote); values have been converted to percentages. For favorability, difference is calculated as mean (post-response) minus mean (post-attack). Significance tests are 2-tailed. *p < .10; **p < .05; ***p < .01. For the effect of the response, *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001. For significant differences across candidate gender, italics p < .05; bold p < .01.
To be sure that an interaction effect between respondents’ party and gender is not obscuring some relationships between gender and the effectiveness of our ads, we regressed shared gender and shared partisanship on variables measuring the effect of the attack (or response—that is, the values measuring the difference in vote for incumbent, incumbent favorability, and challenger favorability from t1 to t2 and from t2 to t3). We ran these regressions for each ad individually and across candidate genders (testing the results for male and female candidates separately). The results of these analyses are consistent with those for our t-tests.
DISCUSSION
Although there is evidence that negative advertising “works” at least some of the time, it has been suggested that going negative poses a special risk for female candidates because it violates expectations about appropriate behavior that are rooted in traditional gender stereotypes held by many voters. If this is true, then women could be at a similar disadvantage when they answer a negative with a negative—that is, when they respond in kind to an opponent's attack—which is precisely the strategy that is preferred by many (or perhaps even most) campaign professionals. Our study has addressed these possibilities using data from an Internet-based survey experiment with a national sample of registered voters. Specifically, we examined gender differences in the effectiveness of (1) a performance-based negative attack made by a challenger against an incumbent of the opposite sex in a hypothetical race for the U.S. House; and (2) five distinct responses to that attack, some of them (counterattack, accusations of mudslinging) more negative in tone than others.
Looking at our sample as a whole, we learned that party mattered more than candidate gender in shaping voters’ reactions to both attack and response, though neither shared partisanship nor shared gender had a strong or consistent impact overall. As for the question of whether negative ads are noticeably less effective for women than for men, the answer appears to be no: among all respondents, the attack moved fewer votes and had a marginally weaker impact on favorability ratings when it was made by our female candidate than when it was made by her male counterpart; however, these differences were not statistically significant at conventional levels. Taking our cue from Krupnikov and Bauer (Reference Krupnikov and Bauer2014), we posited that women would incur a greater penalty for going negative but only among the incumbent/target's fellow partisans. For the most part, the attack worked about as well for one candidate as it did for the other among voters of all partisan stripes and both genders.
With little guidance from the literature, we hypothesized that the effectiveness of our five response types would be similar for women and men. While there were hints that denials may work better for men and justifications better for women, the mudslinging charge yielded unimpressive results for both male and female incumbents. In general, however, gender differences in our study were once again both small and inconsistent. We also expected to find that any response, whether made by a man or a woman, would be most effective among the incumbent/responder's fellow partisans ( H 5 ). Although this was true in most cases, there were a number of exceptions, and the overall pattern is consistent with the expectations laid out in our hypothesis. Finally, as with the attacks, we found little to no evidence that shared gender matters in the effectiveness of a response.
For this particular attack and these five responses most voters (regardless of their gender) reacted in a similar fashion to both male and female candidates. Most importantly, there is only slight evidence to indicate that women are held to a different standard when they choose to go negative proactively or when they answer an attack by criticizing their attacker. Our analysis suggests that some types of responses may work better for either men or women with certain groups of voters, and the same thing is almost certainly true for attacks as well. Given the inability of scholars to faithfully re-create a political campaign in the laboratory (Gadarian and Lau Reference Gadarian, Lau, Druckman, Green, Kuklinski and Lupia2011), questions such as these can only be answered through an accumulation of research that employs a healthy mix of observational and experimental (lab, field, and survey) designs (see Arceneaux Reference Arceneaux2010).
Indeed, there are several potentially important campaign elements that we were unable to examine in this study (see Craig, Rippere, and Grayson Reference Craig, Rippere and Grayson2014 for a more complete discussion). One that may have different implications for men and women is the role of repetition. Our participants viewed a single attack followed by a single response, which represents a severely truncated version of the communication that occurs over the course of a typical campaign. Judging from the barrage of advertising to which voters are exposed in virtually any high-visibility, high-expenditure contest, political professionals are strong believers in the value of repetition (see Craig and Hill Reference Craig and Hill2011, 135–38). At least one recent study confirms that belief while also indicating that too much repetition of a negative ad over too short a period of time can backfire on the sponsor candidate (Fernandes Reference Fernandes2013). In addition, repeated exposure to negative ads may create a backlash against the sponsor among women voters (but not necessarily men; see King and McConnell Reference King and McConnell2003). We encourage future scholars to determine whether this still (or typically) is the case today and whether the reactions of men and women voters to message repetition vary with the gender of the candidate doing the attacking. While our study is one bit of good news for those who would like to see a level playing field for all politicians regardless of gender, there remains much work to be done before we can say with confidence that this has been achieved in the area of campaign communication.
SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL
To view supplementary material for this article, please visit http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1743923X15000550.