Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rcrh6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T21:53:07.413Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Place assimilation and phonetic grounding: a cross-linguistic perceptual study*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 November 2007

Alexei Kochetov
Affiliation:
University of Toronto
Connie K. So
Affiliation:
University of Western Sydney

Abstract

This paper investigates predictions made by the ‘phonetic knowledge hypothesis’ (Jun 1995, 2004, Hayes & Steriade 2004) about the relation between perceptibility of stops and common patterns of major place assimilation. In two perceptual experiments, stimuli with Russian released and unreleased voiceless stops in clusters were presented for identification of 56 listeners, native speakers of Russian, Canadian English, Korean and Taiwanese Mandarin. Percentages of correct responses and reaction time data were used to determine scales of perceptual salience. Results reveal considerable perceptual differences between places of articulation, consistent across four language groups. Perceptual salience of place of articulation was strongly affected by presence or absence of stop releases. While the salience scale for released stops closely corresponded to cross-linguistic patterns of assimilation, the scale for unreleased stops did not. The results provide partial support for the hypothesis, while suggesting a less direct relation between scales of phonetic difficulty and phonological markedness.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Abramson, Arthur S. & Tingsabadh, Kalaya (1999). Thai final stops: cross-language perception. Phonetica 56. 111122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, Stephen R. (1985). Phonology in the twentieth century. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Archangeli, Diana & Pulleyblank, Douglas (1994). Grounded phonology. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bladon, Anthony (1986). Phonetics of hearers. In McGregor, Graham (ed.) Language for hearers. Oxford: Pergamon. 124.Google Scholar
Blevins, Juliette (2004). Evolutionary Phonology: the emergence of sound patterns. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blevins, Juliette (2007). Interpreting misperception: beauty is in the ear of the beholder. In Solé, Maria-Josep, Beddor, Patrice & Ohala, Manjari (eds.) Experimental approaches to phonology. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 144154.Google Scholar
Bondarko, L. V. (1977). Zvukovoj stroj sovremennogo russkogo jazyka. [A sound pattern of Modern Russian.] Moscow: Prosveshchenie.Google Scholar
Bregman, Albert S. (1990). Auditory scene analysis: the perceptual organization of sound. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Browman, Catherine P. & Goldstein, Louis (1989). Articulatory gestures as phonological units. Phonology 6. 201251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Browman, Catherine P. & Goldstein, Louis (1992). Articulatory phonology: an overview. Phonetica 49. 155180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Browman, Catherine P. & Goldstein, Louis (1995). Gestural syllable position effects in American English. In Bell-Berti, Fredericka & Raphael, Lawrence (eds.) Producing speech: contemporary issues. For Katherine Safford Harris. Woodbury, NY: American Institute of Physics Press. 1933.Google Scholar
Byrd, Dani (1992). Perception of assimilation in consonant clusters: a gestural model. Phonetica 49. 124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrd, Dani (1996). Influences on articulatory timing in consonant sequences. JPh 24. 209244.Google Scholar
ChitoranIoana, Louis Goldstein Ioana, Louis Goldstein & Byrd, Dani (2002). Gestural overlap and recoverability: articulatory evidence from Georgian. In Gussenhoven, Carlos & Warner, Natasha (eds.) Laboratory Phonology 7. Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter. 419447.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cho, Taehong & Ladefoged, Peter (1999). Variation and universals in VOT: evidence from 18 languages. JPh 27. 207229.Google Scholar
de Lacy, Paul (2002). The formal expression of markedness. PhD dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst.Google Scholar
Duanmu, San (2002). The phonology of standard Chinese. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ellis, Lucy & William, J. Hardcastle (2002). Categorical and gradient properties of assimilation in alveolar to velar sequences: evidence from EPG and EMA data. JPh 30. 373396.Google Scholar
Flege, James Emil & Chipin, Wang (1989). Native-language phonotactic constraints affect how well Chinese subjects perceive the word-final English /t/–/d/ contrast. JPh 17. 299315.Google Scholar
Fujimura, O., Macchi, M. J. & Streeter, L. A. (1978). Perception of stop consonants with conflicting transitional cues: a cross-linguistic study. Language and Speech 21. 337346.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gao, Man (2005). Identifying native Cantonese stops: implication for native speakers perception. JASA 117. 2460.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harnad, Stevan (1990). The symbol grounding problem. Physica D 42. 335346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, Bruce, Kirchner, Robert & Steriade, Donca (eds.) (2004). Phonetically based phonology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, Bruce & Steriade, Donca (2004). Introduction: the phonetic bases of phonological markedness. In Hayes, et al. . (2004). 133.Google Scholar
Henderson, Janette, B. & Repp, Bruno H. (1982). Is a stop consonant released when followed by another stop consonant? Phonetica 39. 7182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Householder, Fred W. Jr (1956). Unreleased ptk in American English. In Halle, Morris, Lunt, Horace, McLean, Hugh & van Schooneveld, Cornelis (eds.) For Roman Jakobson: essays on the occasion of his sixtieth birthday. The Hague: Mouton. 235244.Google Scholar
Hume, Elizabeth (2003). Language specific markedness: the case of place of articulation. Studies in Phonetics, Phonology and Morphology 9. 295310.Google Scholar
Hume, Elizabeth & Johnson, Keith (2001a). A model of the interplay of speech perception and phonology. In , Hume & , Johnson (2001b). 326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hume, Elizabeth & Johnson, Keith (eds.) (2001b) The role of speech perception in phonology. San Diego: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hume, Elizabeth, Johnson, Keith, Seo, Misun, Tserdanelis, Georgios & Winters, Stephen (1999). A cross-linguistic study of stop place perception. In Ohala, John, Hasegawa, Yoko, Ohala, Manjari, Granville, Daniel & Bailey, Ashlee (eds.) Proceedings of the 14th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. Berkeley: Department of Linguistics, University of California, Berkeley. 20692072.Google Scholar
Hura, Susan L., Lindblom, Björn & Diehl, Randy L. (1992). On the role of perception in shaping phonological assimilation rules. Language and Speech 35. 5972.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jun, Jongho (1995). Perceptual and articulatory factors in place assimilation: an Optimality-theoretic approach. PhD dissertation, University of California, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Jun, Jongho (1996). Place assimilation is not the result of gestural overlap: evidence from Korean and English. Phonology 13. 377407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jun, Jongho (2004). Place assimilation. In Hayes, et al. (2004). 5886.Google Scholar
Kessler, Brett & Treiman, Rebecca (1997). Syllable structure and the distribution of phonemes in English syllables. Journal of Memory and Language 37. 295311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim-Renaud, Young-Key (1991). Korean consonantal phonology. Seoul: Hanshin.Google Scholar
Kingston, John & Shinya, Takahito (2003). Markedness asymmetries in place perception in consonant clusters. In Solé, M. J., Recasens, D. & Romero, J. (eds.) Proceedings of the 15th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. Barcelona: Causal Productions. 399402.Google Scholar
Kochetov, Alexei & Connie, K. So (2005). Investigating the relation between place of articulation markedness and perceptual salience. In Gurski, Claire (ed.) Proceedings of the 2005 Canadian Linguistics Association Annual Conference. http://ling.uwo.ca/publications/cla-acl/cla-acl2005.htm.Google Scholar
Kohler, Klaus (1990). Segmental reduction in connected speech in German: phonological facts and phonetic explanation. In Hardcastle, W. J. & Marchal, A. (eds.) Speech production and speech modelling. Dordrecht: Kluwer. 6992.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krakow, Rena A. (1999). Physiological organization of syllables: a review. JPh 27. 2354.Google Scholar
Kučera, Henry & George, K. Monroe (1968). A comparative quantitative phonology of Russian, Czech, and German. New York: American Elsevier.Google Scholar
Lindblom, Björn, Guion, Susan, Hura, Susan, Moon, Seung-Jae & Willerman, Raquel (1995). Is sound change adaptive? Rivista di Linguistica 7. 537.Google Scholar
Lisker, Leigh (1999). Perceiving final voiceless stops without release: effects of preceding monophthongs versus nonmonophthongs. Phonetica 56. 4455.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malécot, André (1958). The role of releases in the identification of released final stops: a series of tape-cutting experiments. Lg 34. 370380.Google Scholar
Nolan, Francis (1992). The descriptive role of segments: evidence from assimilation. In Docherty, Gerard & Ladd, D. (eds.) Papers in laboratory phonology II: gesture, segment, prosody. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 261280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norman, Jerry (1988). Chinese. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Oh, Mira (2002). Place perception in Korean consonants. Korean Journal of Speech Sciences 9. 131142.Google Scholar
Ohala, John J. (1981). The listener as a source of sound change. In Masek, C. S., Hendrick, R. A. & Miller, M. F. (eds.) Papers from the parasession on language and behavior. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society. 178203.Google Scholar
Ohala, John J. (1990). The phonetics and phonology of aspects of assimilation. In Kingston, John & Beckman, Mary (eds.) Papers in laboratory phonology I: between the grammar and physics of speech. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 258275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, Carole & Prunet, Jean-François (eds.) (1991). The special status of coronals: internal and external evidence. San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Perkell, Joseph S., Cohen, Marc H., Svirsky, Mario A., Matthies, Melanie L., Garabieta, Iñaki & Jackson, Michael T. T. (1992). Electromagnetic midsagittal articulometer systems for transducing speech articulatory movements. JASA 92. 30783096.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perkell, Joseph S., Guenther, Frank H., Lane, Harlan, Marrone, Nicole, Matthies, Melanie L., Stockmann, Ellen, Tiede, Mark & Zandipour, Majid (2006). Production and perception of phoneme contrasts covary across speakers. In Harrington, Jonathan & Tabain, Marija (eds.) Speech production models, phonetic processes, and techniques. New York & London: Routledge. 6984.Google Scholar
Pierrehumbert, Janet, Beckman, Mary E. & Ladd, D. R. (2000). Conceptual foundations of phonology as a laboratory science. In Burton-Roberts, Noel, Carr, Philip & Docherty, Gerard (eds.) Phonological knowledge: conceptual and empirical issues. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 273303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Port, Robert F. & Adam, P. Leary (2005). Against formal phonology. Lg 81. 927964.Google Scholar
Pouplier, Marianne & Louis, Goldstein (2005). Asymmetries in the perception of speech production errors. JPh 33. 4775.Google Scholar
Prince, Alan & Paul, Smolensky (1993). Optimality Theory: constraint interaction in generative grammar. Ms, Rutgers University & University of Colorado, Boulder. Published 2004, Malden, Mass. & Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Repp, Bruno H. (1983a). Coarticualtion in sequences of two nonhomorganic stop consonants: perceptual and acoustic evidence. JASA 74. 420427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Repp, Bruno H. (1983b). Bidirectional context effects in the perception of VC-CV sequences. Perception and Psychophysics 33. 147155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rice, Keren (1996). Default variability: the coronal–velar relationship. NLLT 14. 493543.Google Scholar
Rice, Keren (1999). Featural markedness in phonology: variation. Glot International 4:7. 36. 4:8. 37.Google Scholar
Son, Minjung, Kochetov, Alexei & Pouplier, Marianne (2007). The role of gestural overlap in perceptual place assimilation: evidence from Korean. In Cole, Jennifer & Hualde, José (eds.) Laboratory Phonology 9. Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Steriade, Donca (2001). Directional asymmetries in place assimilation: a perceptual account. In , Hume & , Johnson (2001b). 219250.Google Scholar
Stevens, Kenneth N. (1989). On the quantal nature of speech. JPh 17. 345.Google Scholar
Stevens, Kenneth N. (1998). Acoustic phonetics. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Surprenant, Aimée M. & Goldstein, Louis (1998). The perception of speech gestures. JASA 104. 518529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winitz, Harris, Scheib, M. E. & Reeds, James A. (1972). Identification of stops and vowels for the burst portion of /p, t, k/ isolated from conversational speech. JASA 51. 13091317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winters, Stephen (2003). Emprirical investigations into the perceptual and articulatory origins of cross-linguistic asymmetries in place assimilation. PhD dissertation, Ohio State University.Google Scholar
Wright, Richard (2001). Perceptual cues in contrast maintenance. In , Hume & , Johnson (2001b). 251277.Google Scholar
Wright, Richard (2004). A review of perceptual cues and cue robustness. In Hayes, et al. . (2004). 3457.Google Scholar
Zsiga, Elizabeth C. (1994). Acoustic evidence for gestural overlap in consonant sequences. JPh 22. 121140.Google Scholar
Zsiga, Elizabeth C. (2000). Phonetic alignment constraints: consonant overlap and palatalization in English and Russian. JPh 28. 69102.Google Scholar