Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-fbnjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T16:21:04.648Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A Phase Window Framework for Articulatory Timing*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 October 2008

Dani Byrd
Affiliation:
University of California, Los Angeles/Haskins Laboratories

Extract

One of the most significant challenges in the study of speech production is to acquire a theoretical understanding of how speakers coordinate articulatory movements. A variety of work has demonstrated that articulatory, prosodic and extralinguistic factors all influence speech timing in a complex and interactive way. Models such as Articulatory Phonology that stipulate the relative timing of articulatory units must be revised to allow for this variability. Such a revision is outlined below.

The following work should be viewed as a presentation of a new framework for conceptualising articulatory timing. This approach, meant to be programmatic rather than conclusive, is productive if it motivates research that might not otherwise have been undertaken. §1 overviews Articulatory Phonology. The implementation of articulatory timing in terms of phasing relations is discussed. Speech production data bearing on timing variability are discussed in §2. §3 argues for an alternative to Articulatory Phonology's current rule-based approach to intergestural timing that can allow for linguistic and extralinguistic variables to systematically influence phasing relations. §3.2 introduces the PHASE WINDOW framework, which allows the degree of articulatory overlap between linguistic gestures to vary within a constrained range. Finally, §4 concerns the relation of intergestural timing to the postulation of the segment as a primitive unit in phonology. It is hypothesised that certain intergestural timing relations are stable and lexically specified. Gestures whose coordination is constrained by lexical PHASE WINDOWS seem to bear a close relation to those conglomerates of gestures that constitute what is traditionally considered to be a segment.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1996

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Barry, M. (1985). A palatographic study of connected speech process. Cambridge Papers in Phonetics and Experimental Linguistics 4. 116.Google Scholar
Barry, W. J. (1983). Some problems of interarticulator phasing as an index of temporal regularity in speech. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 9. 826828.Google ScholarPubMed
Bell-Berti, F. & Raphael, L. J. (eds.) (1995). Producing speech: contemporary issues for Katherine Safford Harris. New York: AIP Press.Google Scholar
Benoit, C. (1986). Note on the use of correlations in speech timing. JASA 80. 18461849.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Browman, C. (1995). Assimilation as gestural overlap: comments on Hoist and Nolan. In Conneli & Arvaniti (1995). 334342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Browman, C. & Goldstein, L. (1986). Toward an articulatory phonology. Phonology Yearbook 3. 219252.Google Scholar
Browman, C. & Goldstein, L. (1988). Some notes on syllable structure in articulatory phonology. Phonetica 45. 140155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Browman, C. & Goldstein, L. (1989). Articulatory gestures as phonological units. Phonology 6. 201251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Browman, C. & Goldstein, L. (1990a). Gestural specification using dynamically-defined articulatory structures. JPh 18. 299320.Google Scholar
Browman, C. & Goldstein, L. (1990b). Tiers in articulatory phonology, with some implications for casual speech. In Kingston & Beckman (1990). 341376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Browman, C. & Goldstein, L. (1991). Gestural structures: distinctiveness, phonological processes, and historical change. In Mattingly, I. & Studdert-Kennedy, M. (eds.) Modularity and the motor theory of speech perception. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 313338.Google Scholar
Browman, C. & Goldstein, L. (1992a). Articulatory phonology: an overview. Phonetica 49. 155180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Browman, C. & Goldstein, L. (1992b). Response to commentaries. Phonetica 49. 222234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Browman, C. & Goldstein, L. (1995a). Gestural syllable position effects in American English. In Bell-Berti & Raphael (1995). 1933.Google Scholar
Browman, C. & Goldstein, L. (1995b). Dynamics and articulatory phonology. In Port & van Gelder (1995). 175193.Google Scholar
Byrd, D. (1992). Perception of assimilation in consonant clusters: a gestural model. Phonetica 49. 124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrd, D. (1994). Articulatory timing in English consonant sequences. PhD dissertation, UCLA.Google Scholar
Byrd, D. (1996). Influences on articulatory timing in consonant sequences. JPh 24. 209224.Google Scholar
Byrd, D., Kaun, A. R. & Narayanan, S. (1996). Prosodic boundary effects in Tamil: an articulatory study. Paper presented at the 70th Annual Meeting of the Linguistic Society of America, San Diego.Google Scholar
Byrd, D. & Tan, C. C. (1996). Saying consonant clusters quickly. JPh 24. 263282.Google Scholar
Catford, J. C. (1977). Fundamental problems in phonetics. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Chermak, G. D. & Schneiderman, C. R. (1985). Speech timing variability of children and adults. JPh 13. 477480.Google Scholar
Clements, G. N. (1992). Phonological primes: features or gestures. Phonetica 49. 181193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conneli, B. & Arvaniti, A. (eds.) (1995). Phonology and phonetic evidence: papers in laboratory phonology IV. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delattre, P. (1971). Consonant gemination in four languages: an acoustic, perceptual, and radiographic study. Part I. IRAL 9. 3152.Google Scholar
Delattre, P. & Freeman, D. (1968). A dialect study of American R's by x-ray motion picture. Linguistics 44. 2968.Google Scholar
Diehl, R. L. (1991). The role of phonetics within the study of language. Phonetica 48. 120134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Docherty, G. J. (1992). The timing of British English obstruents. Berlin: Foris.Google Scholar
Docherty, G. J. & Ladd, D. R. (eds.) (1992). Papers in laboratory phonology II: gesture, segment, prosody. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elenius, K. & Branderud, P. (eds.) (1995). Proceedings of the 13th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. Vol. 3. Stockholm: Congress Organizers at KTH and Stockholm University.Google Scholar
Flemming, E., Ladefoged, P. & Thomason, S. (1993). The phonetic structures of Montana Salish. UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics 87. 134.Google Scholar
Goldstein, L. (1989). On the domain of the quantal theory. JPh 17. 9197.Google Scholar
Goldstein, L. (1990). On articulatory binding: comments on Kingston's paper. In Kingston & Beckman (1990). 445450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodell, E. W. & Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1993). Acoustic evidence for the development of gestural coordination in the speech of 2-year-olds: a longitudinal study. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 36. 707727.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gracco, V. L. (1988). Timing factors in the coordination of speech movements. Journal of Neuroscience 8. 46284634.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gracco, V. L. (1991). Sensorimotor mechanisms in speech motor control. In Peters, H. F. M., Hulstijn, W. & Starkweather, C. W. (eds.) Speech motor control and stuttering. Amsterdam: Elsevier. 5376.Google Scholar
Gracco, V. L. (1992). Characteristics of speech as a motor control system. Haskins Laboratories Status Report on Speech Research SR-109/110. 1326. To appear in Hammond, G. (ed.) Cerebral control of speech and limb movements. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Gracco, V. L. & Abbs, J. H. (1989). Sensorimotor characteristics of speech motor sequences. Experimental Brain Research 75. 586598.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hardcastle, W. J. (1985). Some phonetic and syntactic constraints on lingual Coarticulation during /kl/ sequences. Speech Communication 4. 247263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hardcastle, W. J. & Marchal, A. (eds.) (1995). Speech production and speech modelling. Kluwer: Dordrecht.Google Scholar
Hardcastle, W. J. & Roach, P. (1979). An instrumental investigation of coarticulation in stop consonant sequences. In Hollien, H. & Hollien, P. (eds.) Current issues in the phonetic sciences. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 531540.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, S. (1992). An introduction to task dynamics. In Docherty & Ladd (1992). 925.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Hoist, T. & Nolan, F. (1995). The influence of syntactic structure on [s] and [J] assimilation. In Connell & Arvaniti (1995). 315333.Google Scholar
Johnson, K., Ladefoged, P. & Lindau, M. (1993). Individual differences in vowel production. JASA 94. 701714.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, D. (1956). An outline of English phonetics. 8th edn. Cambridge: Heffer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jong, K. de (1991). An articulatory study of consonant-induced vowel duration changes in English. Phonetica 48. 117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jordan, M. (1986). Serial order in behaviour: a parallel distributed processing approach. San Diego: Institute for Cognitive Science, University of California.Google Scholar
Katz, W., Kripke, C. & Tallal, P. (1991). Anticipatory coarticulation in the speech of adults and young children: acoustic, perceptual, and video data. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 34. 12221232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kay, B. A., Kelso, J. A. S., Saltzman, E. & Schöner, G. (1987). Space-time behavior of single and bimanual rhythmical movements: data and limit cycle model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 13. 178192.Google ScholarPubMed
Keating, P. A. (1990a). Phonetic representations in a generative grammar. JPh 18. 321334.Google Scholar
Keating, P. A. (1990b). The window model of coarticulation: articulatory evidence. In Kingston & Beckman (1990). 451470.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keating, P. A. (1995). Segmental phonology and non-segmental phonetics. In Elenius & Branderud (1995). 2632.Google Scholar
Keller, E. (1987). The variation of absolute and relative measures of speech activity. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 30. 223229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, E. (1990). Speech motor timing. In Hardcastle & Marchal (1990). 342364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelso, J. A. S., Saltzman, E. & Tuller, B. (1986a). The dynamical perspective on speech production: data and theory. JPh 14. 2959.Google Scholar
Kelso, J. A. S., Saltzman, E. & Tuller, B. (1986b). Intentional contents, communicative context, and task dynamics: a reply to commentators. JPh 14. 171196.Google Scholar
Kelso, J. A. S. & Tuller, B. (1987). Intrinsic time in speech production: theory, methodology, and preliminary observations. In Keller, E. & Gopnik, M. (eds.) Sensory and motor processes in language. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 203222.Google Scholar
Kelso, J. A. S., Vatikiotis-Bateson, E., Saltzman, E. & Kay, B. (1985). A qualitative dynamic analysis of reiterant speech production: phase portraits, kinematics, and dynamic modeling. JASA 77. 266280.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kent, R. D. (1983). The segmental organization of speech. In MacNeilage, P. F. (ed.) The production of speech. New York: Springer. 5789.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kent, R. D. (1986). Is a paradigm change needed? JPh 14. 111115.Google Scholar
Kent, R. D. & Forner, L. L. (1980). Speech segment duration in sentence recitations by children and adults. JPh 8. 157168.Google Scholar
Kewley-Port, D. & Preston, M. S. (1974). Early apical stop production: a voice onset time analysis. JPh 2. 195210.Google Scholar
Kingston, J. & Beckman, M. E. (eds.) (1990). Papers in laboratory phonology I: between the grammar and physics of speech. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kohler, K. J. (1992). Gestural reorganization in connected speech: a functional viewpoint on ‘articulatory phonology’. Phonetica 49. 205211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kosko, B. (1993). Fuzzy thinking. New York: Hyperion.Google Scholar
Kozhevnikov, V. & Chistovich, L. (1965). Speech: articulation and perception. Translated and distributed by Joint Publications Research Service, Washington D.C. Originally published in Russian, 1965.Google Scholar
Krakow, R. A. (1989). The articulatory organization of syllables: a kinematic analysis of labial and velar gestures. PhD dissertation, Yale University.Google Scholar
Kuehn, D. P. & Moll, K. L. (1976). A cineradiographic study of VC and CV articulatory velocities. JPh 4. 303320.Google Scholar
Ladefoged, P., DeClerk, J., Lindau, M. & Papcun, G. (1972). An auditory-motor theory of speech production. UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics 22. 4875.Google Scholar
Lindau, M. (1985). The story of /r/. In Fromkin, V. A. (ed.) Phonetic linguistics: essays in honor of Peter Ladefoged. Orlando, FI.: Academic Press. 157168.Google Scholar
Lindblom, B. (1990). Explaining phonetic variation: a sketch of the H & H theory. In Hardcastle & Marchal (1990). 403439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Löfqvist, A. (1986). Stability and change. JPh 14. 139144.Google Scholar
Lofqvist, A. (1990). Speech as audible gesture. In Hardcastle & Marchal (1990). 289322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lofqvist, A. (1991). Proportional timing in speech motor control. JPh 19. 343350.Google Scholar
Lofqvist, A. & Yoshioka, H. (1981). Interarticulator programming in obstruent production. Phonetica 38. 2134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lofqvist, A. & Yoshioka, H. (1984). Intrasegmental timing: laryngeal-oral coordination in voiceless consonant production. Speech Communication 3. 279289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lubker, J. (1986). Articulatory timing and the concept of phase. JPh 14. 133137.Google Scholar
McLean, M. (1973). Forward coarticulation of velar movement at marked junctural boundaries. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 16. 286296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacNeilage, P. (1970). Motor control of serial ordering of speech. Psychological Review 77. 182196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maddieson, I. (1993). Investigating Ewe articulations with electromagnetic articulography. Forschungsberichte, Institut für Phonetik und Sprachliche Kommunikation 31. 184214.Google Scholar
Maddieson, I. & Ladefoged, P. (1989). Multiply-articulated segments and the feature hierarchy. UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics 72. 116138.Google Scholar
Mattingly, I. G. (1981). Phonetic representation and speech synthesis by rule. In Myers, T., Laver, J. & Anderson, J. (eds.) The cognitive representation of speech. Amsterdam: North-Holland. 415420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mermelstein, P. (1973). Articulatory model for the study of speech production. JASA S3. 10701082.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Munhall, K. G. (1985). An examination of intra-articulatory relative timing. JASA 78. 15481553.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Munhall, K. G., Lofqvist, A. & Kelso, J. A. S. (1986). Laryngeal compensation following sudden oral perturbation. JASA 80. S109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Munhall, K. G., Ostry, D. J. & Parush, A. (1985). Characteristics of velocity profiles of speech movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology 4. 457474.Google Scholar
Nittrouer, S. (1991). Phase relations of jaw and tongue tip movements in the production of VCV utterances. JASA 90. 18061815.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nittrouer, S. (1993). The emergence of mature gestural patterns is not uniform: evidence from an acoustic study. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 36. 959972.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nittrouer, S., Munhall, K. G., Kelso, J. A. S., Tuller, B. & Harris, K. S. (1988). Patterns of interarticulator phasing and their relation to linguistic structure. JASA 84. 16531661.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nittrouer, S., Studdert-Kennedy, M. & McGowan, R. S. (1989). The emergence of phonetic segments: evidence from the spectral structure of fricative—vowel syllables spoken by children and adults. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 32. 120132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nolan, F. (1992). The descriptive role of segments: evidence from assimilation. In Docherty & Ladd (1992). 261289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ohala, J. (1990). The generality of articulatory binding. In Kingston & Beckman (1990). 435444.Google Scholar
Ohala, J., Hiki, S., Hubler, S. & Harshman, R. (1968). Photoelectric methods of transducing lip and jaw movements in speech. UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics 10. 135144.Google Scholar
Ostry, D. J., Keller, E. & Parush, A. (1983). Similarities in the control of the speech articulators and the limbs: kinematics of tongue dorsum movements in speech. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 9. 622636.Google ScholarPubMed
Ostry, D. J. & Munhall, K. G. (1985). Control of rate and duration of speech movements. JASA 77. 640648.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pierrehumbert, J. (1994). Syllable structure and word structure: a study of triconsonantal clusters in English. In Keating, P. (ed.) Phonological structure and phonetic form: papers in laboratory phonology III. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 168187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Port, R. F. & van Gelder, T. (eds.) (1995). Mind as motion: explorations in the dynamics of cognition. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Saltzman, E. (1986). Task dynamic coordination of the speech articulators: a preliminary model. In Heuer, H. & Fromm, C. (eds.) Generation and modulation of action patterns. New York: Springer. 129144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saltzman, E. (1995a). Intergestural timing in speech production: data and modeling. In Elenius & Branderud (1995). 8491.Google Scholar
Saltzman, E. (1995b). Dynamics and coordinate systems in skilled sensorimotor activity. In Port & van Gelder (1995). 149173.Google Scholar
Saltzman, E. & Kelso, J. A. S. (1987). Skilled actions: a task dynamic approach. Psychological Review 94. 84106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saltzman, E., Lofqvist, A., Kay, B., Kinsella-Shaw, J. & Rubin, P. (in preparation). Dynamics of intergestural timing: a perturbation study of lip-larynx coordination.Google Scholar
Saltzman, E., Lofqvist, A., Kinsella-Shaw, J., Kay, B. & Rubin, P. (1995). On the dynamics of temporal patterning in speech. In Bell-Berti & Raphael (1995). 469487.Google Scholar
Saltzman, E. & Munhall, K. G. (1989). A dynamical approach to gestural patterning in speech production. Ecological Psychology 1. 333382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaiman, S., Adams, S. G. & Kimelman, M. D. Z. (1995). Timing relationships of the upper lip and jaw across changes in speaking rate. JPh 23. 119128.Google Scholar
Shaiman, S. & Porter, R. J. Jr (1991). Different phase-stable relationships of the upper lip and jaw for production of vowels and diphthongs. JASA 90. 30003007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharkey, S. G. & Folkins, J. W. (1985) Variability of lip and jaw movements in children and adults: implications for the development of speech motor control. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 28. 815.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sock, R. & Jah, O. (1986). Old wine in new bottles, new wine in old bottles: action theory and (para)metric phonology views in the domain of Wolof. Bulletin de l'Institut de Phonétique de Grenoble 15. 117133.Google Scholar
Sproat, R. & Fujimura, O. (1993). Allophonic variation in English /l/ and its implications for phonetic implementation. JPh 21. 291312.Google Scholar
Stevens, K. N. (1989). On the quantal nature of speech. JPh 17. 345.Google Scholar
Sussman, H. M., MacNeilage, P. F. & Hanson, R. J. (1973). Labial and mandibular dynamics during the production of bilabial consonants: preliminary observations. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 16. 397420.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tingley, B. M. & Allen, G. D. (1975). Development of speech timing control in children. Child Development 46. 186194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tuller, B. & Kelso, J. A. S. (1984). The timing of articulatory gestures: evidence for relational invariants. JASA 76. 10301036.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tuller, B., Kelso, J. A. S. & Harris, K. S. (1982). Interarticulator phasing as an index of temporal regulatory in speech. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 8. 460472.Google Scholar
Tung, T. (1964). A descriptive study of the Tsou language, Formosa. Taipei: Institute of History and Philology.Google Scholar
Turvey, M. T. (1990). Coordination. American Psychologist. August 1990. 938953.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turvey, M. T., Rosenblum, L. D., Kugler, P. N. & Schmidt, R. C. (1986). Fluctuations and phase symmetry in coordinated rhythmic movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 12. 564583.Google ScholarPubMed
Uldall, E. T. (1958). American ‘molar’ r and ‘flapped’ r. Revista do Laboratorio de Fonética Experimental (Coimbra) 4. 103106.Google Scholar
Vatikiotis-Bateson, E. & Kelso, J. A. S. (1993). Rhythm type and articulatory dynamics in English, French and Japanese. JPh 21. 231265.Google Scholar
Westermann, D. & Ward, I. C. (1933). Practical phonetics for students of African languages. Oxford: Oxford University Press, for the International African Institute.Google Scholar