We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
An abstract is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)
References
REFERENCES
Anttila, Arto (2002). Variation and phonological theory. In Chambers, J. K., Trudgill, Peter & Schilling-Estes, Natalie (eds.) The handbook of language variation and change. Oxford: Blackwell. 206–243.Google Scholar
Blevins, Juliette (2004). Evolutionary Phonology: the emergence of sound patterns. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boersma, Paul (forthcoming). Modelling phonological category learning. In Cohn, Abigail C., Fougeron, Cécile & Huffman, Marie K. (eds.) Handbook of laboratory phonology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bresnan, Joan & Deo, Ashwini (2001). Grammatical constraints on variation: ‘be’ in the Survey of English Dialects and (stochastic) Optimality Theory. Ms, Stanford University.Google Scholar
Bresnan, Joan, Deo, Ashwini & Sharma, Devyani (2007). Typology in variation: a probabilistic approach to be and n't in the Survey of English Dialects. English Language and Linguistics11. 301–346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, Alexander & Lappin, Shalom (2011). Linguistic nativism and the poverty of the stimulus. Malden, Mass. & Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coetzee, Andries W. & Pater, Joe (forthcoming). The place of variation in phonological theory. In Goldsmith, John A., Riggle, Jason & Yu, Alan (eds.) The handbook of phonological theory. 2nd edn.Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
de Lacy, Paul (2009). Review of Hale & Reiss (2008). JL45. 719–724.Google Scholar
Fikkert, Paula (1995). From phonetic categories to phonological feature specification: acquiring the European Portuguese vowel system. Lingue e Linguaggio4. 263–280.Google Scholar
Fikkert, Paula & Levelt, Clara (2008). How does Place fall into place? The lexicon and emergent constraints in children's developing phonological grammar. In Avery, Peter, Dresher, B. Elan & Rice, Keren (eds.) Contrast in phonology: theory, perception, acquisition. Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter. 231–268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flemming, Edward (2001). Scalar and categorical phenomena in a unified model of phonetics and phonology. Phonology18. 7–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hale, Mark & Reiss, Charles (2008). The phonological enterprise. Oxford & New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hay, Jennifer (2000). Causes and consequences of word structure. PhD dissertation, Northwestern University.Google Scholar
Frans, Hinskens, van Hout, Roeland & Wetzels, W. Leo (eds.) (1997). Variation, change and phonological theory. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Johnson, Keith (1997). Speech perception without speaker normalization: an exemplar model. In Johnson, Keith & Mullenix, John W. (eds.) Talker variability in speech processing. San Diego: Academic Press. 145–165.Google Scholar
Kapp, Reginald O. (1958). Ockam's Razor and the unification of physical science. The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science8. 265–280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krämer, Martin (2010). Review of Hale & Reiss (2008). Lingua120. 176–182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labov, William (1994). Principles of linguistic change. Vol. 1: Internal factors. Oxford & Cambridge, Mass.: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Marcus, Gary F. (1993). Negative evidence in language acquisition. Cognition46. 53–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ohala, John J. (1983). The origin of sound patterns in vocal tract constraints. In MacNeilage, Peter F. (ed.) The production of speech. New York: Springer. 189–216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petrova, Olga, Plapp, Rosemary, Ringen, Catherine & Szentgyörgyi, Szilárd (2006). Voice and aspiration: evidence from Russian, Hungarian, German, Swedish, and Turkish. The Linguistic Review23. 1–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pierrehumbert, Janet B. (2001). Exemplar dynamics: word frequency, lenition and contrast. In Bybee, Joan & Hopper, Paul (eds.) Frequency and the emergence of linguistic structure. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: Benjamins. 137–157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Polka, Linda & Werker, Janet F. (1994). Developmental changes in perception of nonnative vowel contrasts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance20. 421–435.Google ScholarPubMed
Smolensky, Paul (1996). On the comprehension/production dilemma in child language. LI27. 720–731.Google Scholar
Werker, Janet F. & Tees, Richard C. (1984). Cross-language speech perception: evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behaviour and Development7. 49–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar