Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-09T16:49:14.776Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Unlimited Associative Learning as a Null Hypothesis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 May 2022

Marta Halina*
Affiliation:
Department of History and Philosophy of Science, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK

Abstract

A common strategy in comparative cognition is to require that one reject associative learning as an explanation for behavior before concluding that an organism is capable of causal reasoning. In this paper, I argue that standard causal-reasoning tasks can be explained by a powerful form of associative learning: unlimited associative learning (UAL). The lesson, however, is not that researchers should conduct more studies to reject UAL, but that they should instead focus on 1) enriching the cognitive hypothesis space and 2) testing a broader range of information processing patterns—errors, biases and limits, rather than successful problem solving alone.

Type
Symposia Paper
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the Philosophy of Science Association

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bastos, Amalia P. M., and Taylor, Alex H.. 2020. “Macphail’s Null Hypothesis of Vertebrate Intelligence: Insights from Avian Cognition.” Frontiers in Psychology 11 (1):18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bausman, William C. 2018. “Modeling: Neutral, Null, and Baseline.” Philosophy of Science 85 (4):594616.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bausman, William, and Halina, Marta. 2018. “Not Null Enough: Pseudo-Null Hypotheses in Community Ecology and Comparative Psychology.” Biology & Philosophy 33 (3–4):120.10.1007/s10539-018-9640-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bender, Andrea. 2020. “What Is Causal Cognition?Frontiers in Psychology 11:16. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Birch, Jonathan, Ginsburg, Simona, and Jablonka, Eva. 2020. “Unlimited Associative Learning and the Origins of Consciousness: A Primer and Some Predictions.” Biology & Philosophy 35 (6):123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bronfman, Zohar Z., Ginsburg, Simona, and Jablonka, Eva. 2016. “The Transition to Minimal Consciousness Through the Evolution of Associative Learning.” Frontiers in Psychology 7:116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buckner, Cameron. 2018. “Empiricism Without Magic: Transformational Abstraction in Deep Convolutional Neural Networks.” Synthese 195 (12):5339–72.10.1007/s11229-018-01949-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, Andy. 2016. Surfing Uncertainty: Prediction, Action, and the Embodied Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Currie, Adrian. 2021. Comparative Thinking in Biology. 1st ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/9781108616683CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dacey, Mike. 2021. “Evidence in Default: Rejecting Default Models of Animal Minds.” The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science:142. doi: 10.1086/714799 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennett, Daniel C. 1996. Kinds of Minds: Towards an Understanding of Consciousness. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Douglas, Heather. 2013. “The Value of Cognitive Values.” Philosophy of Science 80 (5):796806.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fitzpatrick, Simon. 2008. “Doing Away with Morgan’s Canon.” Mind & Language 23 (2):224–46.10.1111/j.1468-0017.2007.00338.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ginsburg, Simona, and Jablonka, Eva. 2019. The Evolution of The Sensitive Soul: Learning and the Origins of Consciousness. Cambridge: MIT Press.10.7551/mitpress/11006.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ginsburg, Simona, and Jablonka, Eva. 2021. “Evolutionary Transitions in Learning and Cognition.” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 376 (1821):19.10.1098/rstb.2019.0766CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Girndt, Antje, Meier, T., and Call, J.. 2008. “Task Constraints Mask Great Apes’ Ability to Solve the Trap-Table Task.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 34 (1):5462.Google ScholarPubMed
Godfrey-Smith, Peter. 2018. “Towers and Trees in Cognitive Evolution.” In The Philosophy of Daniel Dennett, edited by Huebner, Bryce, 225–49. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hanus, Daniel. 2016. “Causal Reasoning Versus Associative Learning: A Useful Dichotomy or a Strawman Battle in Comparative Psychology?Journal of Comparative Psychology 130 (3):241–48.10.1037/a0040235CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herrmann, E., Call, J., Hernandez-Lloreda, M. V., Hare, B., and Tomasello, M.. 2007. “Humans Have Evolved Specialized Skills of Social Cognition: The Cultural Intelligence Hypothesis.” Science 317 (5843):1360–66.10.1126/science.1146282CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hohwy, Jakob. 2020. “New Directions in Predictive Processing.” Mind & Language 35 (2):209–23.10.1111/mila.12281CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenward, Ben, Christian Schloegl, Christian Rutz, Weir, Alexander A. S., Bugnyar, Thomas, and Kacelnik, Alex. 2011. “On the Evolutionary and Ontogenetic Origins of Tool-Oriented Behaviour in New Caledonian Crows (Corvus Moneduloides).” Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 102 (4):870–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mulcahy, Nicholas J., and Call, Josep. 2006. “How Great Apes Perform on a Modified Trap-Tube Task.” Animal Cognition 9 (3):193–99.10.1007/s10071-006-0019-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penn, Derek C., and Povinelli, Daniel J.. 2007. “Causal Cognition in Human and Nonhuman Animals: A Comparative, Critical Review.” Annual Review of Psychology 58 (1):97118.10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085555CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Povinelli, Daniel J. 2012. World Without Weight: Perspectives on an Alien Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Seed, Amanda, Hanus, Daniel, and Call, Josep. 2011. “Causal Knowledge in Corvids, Primates, and Children.” In Tool Use and Causal Cognition, edited by McCormack, Teresa, Hoerl, Christoph, and Butterfill, Stephen, 89110. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seed, Amanda M., Tebbich, Sabine, Emery, Nathan J., and Clayton, Nicola S.. 2006. “Investigating Physical Cognition in Rooks, Corvus Frugilegus.” Current Biology 16 (7):697701.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sober, Elliott. 2012. “Anthropomorphism, Parsimony, and Common Ancestry.” Mind & Language 27 (3):229–38.10.1111/j.1468-0017.2012.01442.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sober, Elliott. 2015. Ockham’s Razors: A User’s Manual. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spelke, Elizabeth. 1994. “Early Knowledge of Object Motion: Continuity and Inertia.” Cognition 51:131176.10.1016/0010-0277(94)90013-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Starzak, Tobias Benjamin, and David Gray, Russell. 2021. “Towards Ending the Animal Cognition War: A Three-Dimensional Model of Causal Cognition.” Biology & Philosophy 36 (2):124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Voudouris, Konstantinos. 2020. “An Interdisciplinary Approach to the Associative-Cognitive Distinction.” MPhil diss. University of Cambridge.Google Scholar
Williams, Daniel. 2018. “Predictive Processing and the Representation Wars.” Minds and Machines 28 (1):141–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodward, James. 2011. “Psychological Studies of Causal and Counterfactual Reasoning.” In Understanding Counterfactuals, Understanding Causation: Issues in Philosophy and Psychology, edited by Hoerl, Christoph, McCormack, Teresa, and Sarah, R. Beck, 1653. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar