Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dzt6s Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T00:40:47.318Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Phonetic Segments and the Organization of Speech

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2022

Abstract

According to mainstream linguistic phonetics, speech can be modeled as a string of discrete sound segments or “phones” drawn from a universal phonetic inventory. Recent work has argued that a mature phonetics should refrain from theorizing about speech and speech processing using sound segments and that the phone concept should be eliminated from linguistic theory. The article lays out the tenets of the phone methodology and evaluates its prospects in light of the eliminativist arguments. I claim that the eliminativist arguments fail to show that the phone concept should be eliminated from linguistic theory.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The Philosophy of Science Association

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

I am indebted to Claire Beyssade, Paul Egré, Mikhail Kissine, Salvador Mascarenhas, and François Recanati for discussion and comments on ancestors of this article and to two anonymous reviewers for detailed and very helpful feedback on the submitted manuscript. All errors and misunderstandings are my own. The research that led to this article was supported by an individual fellowship from the École des Hautes Études en Sciences Sociales and by grants ANR-10-LABX-0087 IEC and ANR-10-IDEX-0001-02 PSL*.

References

Appelbaum, Irene. 1999. “The Dogma of Isomorphism: A Case Study from Speech Perception.” Philosophy of Science 66:S250S259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Archangeli, Diana, and Pulleyblank, Douglas. 1994. Grounded Phonology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Azzouni, Jody. 2010. Talking about Nothing: Numbers, Hallucinations, and Fictions. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beckman, Mary E., and Edwards, Jan. 2010. “Generalizing over Lexicons to Predict Consonant Mastery.” Laboratory Phonology 1:319–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bond, Zinny S. 2005. “Slips of the Ear.” In The Handbook of Speech Perception, ed. Pisoni, David B. and Remez, Robert E., 290310. Oxford: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradlow, Ann R. 1995. “A Comparative Acoustic Study of English and Spanish Vowels.” Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 97:1916–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brady, Susan A., and Shankweiler, Donald P., eds. 2001. Phonological Processes in Literacy: A Tribute to Isabelle Y. Liberman. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bromberger, Sylvain, and Halle, Morris. 2000. “The Ontology of Phonology (Revised).” In Phonological Knowledge: Conceptual and Empirical Issues, ed. Burton-Roberts, Noel, Carr, Philip, and Docherty, Gerard, 1938. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Browman, Catherine P., and Goldstein, Louis. 1992. “Articulatory Phonology: An Overview.” Phonetica 49:155–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bybee, Joan. 2001. Phonology and Language Use. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caramazza, Alfonso, and Yeni-Komshian, Grace H.. 1974. “Voice Onset Time in Two French Dialects.” Journal of Phonetics 2:239–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carr, Philip. 2000. “Scientific Realism, Sociophonetic Variation and Innate Endowments in Phonology.” In Phonological Knowledge: Conceptual and Empirical Issues, ed. Burton-Roberts, Noel, Carr, Philip, and Docherty, Gerard. 67104. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Carr, Philip. 2012. “The Philosophy of Phonology.” In Philosophy of Linguistics, ed. Kempson, Ruth, Fernando, Tim, and Asher, Nicholas, 403–44. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam, and Halle, Morris. 1968. The Sound Pattern of English. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Clark, Andy. 1997. Being There: Putting Brain, Body, and World Together Again. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cowley, Stephen J. 2011. “Taking a Language Stance.” Ecological Psychology 23:185209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davenport, Mike, and Hannahs, Stephen J.. 2013. Introducing Phonetics and Phonology. 3rd ed. London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dretske, Fred. 1981. Knowledge and the Flow of Information. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dunbar, Ewan, and Idsardi, William J.. 2010. Review of Critical Introduction to Phonology: Of Sound, Mind, and Body, by D. Silverman. Phonology 27:325–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dupoux, Emmanuel, Parlato, Erika, Frota, Sonia, Hirose, Yuki, and Peperkamp, Sharon. 2011. “Where Do Illusory Vowels Come From?Journal of Memory and Language 64:199210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elgin, Catherine Z. 2017. True Enough. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ereshefsky, Marc. 1998. “Species Pluralism and Anti-realism.” Philosophy of Science 65:103–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Faber, Alice. 1993. “Phonemic Segmentation as Epiphenomenon: Evidence from the History of Alphabetic Writing.” In The Linguistics of Literacy, ed. Downing, Pamela A., Lima, Susan D., and Noonan, Michael, 111–34. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Flege, James E., and Hillenbrand, James E.. 1986. “Differential Use of Temporal Cues to the /s/-/z/ Contrast by Native and Non-native Speakers of English.” Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 79:508–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, Jerry A., Bever, Thomas G., and Garrett, Merrill F.. 1972. The Psychology of Language. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Foss, Donald J., and Gernsbacher, Morton A.. 1983. “Cracking the Dual Code: Towards a Unitary Model of Phoneme Identification.” Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 22:609–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fowler, Carol A. 2010. “The Reality of Phonological Forms: A Reply to Port.” Language Sciences 32:5659.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fowler, Carol A.. 2015. “The Segment in Articulatory Phonology.” In The Segment in Phonetics and Phonology, ed. Raimy, Eric and Cairns, Charles E., 2343. London: Wiley.Google Scholar
Fox, Robert A., and Terbeek, Dale. 1977. “Dental Flaps, Vowel Duration, and Rule Ordering in American English.” Journal of Phonetics 5:2734.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fromkin, Victoria A., ed. 1980. Errors in Linguistic Performance. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Gafos, Adamantios I. 2002. “A Grammar of Gestural Coordination.” Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 20:269337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gillon, Gail T. 2004. Phonological Awareness: From Research to Practice. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Goldinger, Stephen D. 1996. “Words and Voices: Episodic Traces in Spoken Word Identification and Recognition Memory.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 22:1166–83.Google ScholarPubMed
Hale, Mark, and Reiss, Charles. 2008. The Phonological Enterprise. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hall, Kathleen C. 2015. “Categorical Segments, Probabilistic Models.” In The Segment in Phonetics and Phonology, ed. Raimy, Eric and Cairns, Charles E., 129–45. London: Wiley.Google Scholar
Halle, Morris. 1954. “The Strategy of Phonemics.” Word 10:197209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harrington, Jonathan. 2010. “Acoustic Phonetics.” In The Handbook of the Phonetic Sciences, 2nd ed., ed. William J. Hardcastle, John Laver, and Fiona E. Gibbon, 81–129. Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Hockett, Charles F. 1955. A Manual of Phonology. Indiana University Publications in Anthropology and Linguistics. Baltimore: Waverly.Google Scholar
Hockett, Charles F.. 1960. “The Origin of Speech.” Scientific American 203:8997.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
IPA (International Phonetic Association). 1999. Handbook of the International Phonetic Association: A Guide to the Use of the International Phonetic Alphabet. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Jakobson, Roman, Fant, Gunnar M., and Halle, Morris. 1952. Preliminaries to Speech Analysis: The Distinctive Features and Their Correlates. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kelly, John, and Local, John K.. 1986. “Long-Domain Resonance Patterns in English.” In Proceedings of the IEE Conference on Speech Input/Output, 304–8. London: Institution of Electrical Engineers.Google Scholar
Kelso, J. A. Scott. 1995. Dynamic Patterns: The Self-Organization of Brain and Behavior. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kenstowicz, Michael. 1994. Phonology in Generative Grammar. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Kingston, John, and Beckman, Mary E., eds. 1990. Papers in Laboratory Phonology. Vol. 1, Between the Grammar and Physics of Speech. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klatt, Dennis H. 1979. “Speech Perception: A Model of Acoustic-Phonetic Analysis and Lexical Access.” Journal of Phonetics 7:279312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kleber, Felicitas, John, Tina, and Harrington, Jonathan. 2010. “The Implications for Speech Perception of Incomplete Neutralization of Final Devoicing in German.” Journal of Phonetics 38:185–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ladd, D. Robert. 2011. “Phonetics in Phonology.” In The Handbook of Phonological Theory, 2nd ed., ed. John Goldsmith, Jason Riggle, and Alan C. L. Yu, 348–73. London: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Laver, John. 1994. Principles of Phonetics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liberman, Alvin M. 1996. Speech: A Special Code. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Liberman, Alvin M., Cooper, Franklin S., Shankweiler, Donald P., and Studdert-Kennedy, Michael. 1967. “Perception of the Speech Code.” Psychological Review 74:431–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liberman, Alvin M., Delattre, Pierre C., and Cooper, Franklin S.. 1952. “The Role of Selected Stimulus-Variables in the Perception of the Unvoiced Stop Consonants.” American Journal of Psychology 65:497516.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieberman, Philip, and Blumstein, Sheila E.. 1988. Speech Physiology, Speech Perception, and Acoustic Phonetics. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lodge, Ken. 2009. Fundamental Concepts in Phonology: Sameness and Difference. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Lombardi, Linda, ed. 2001. Segmental Phonology in Optimality Theory: Constraints and Representations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luce, Paul A., and McLennan, Conor T.. 2005. “Spoken Word Recognition: The Challenge of Variation.” In The Handbook of Speech Perception, ed. Pisoni, David B. and Remez, Robert E., 591609. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Markman, Arthur B., and Dietrich, Eric. 2000. “In Defense of Representation.” Cognitive Psychology 40:138–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morais, José, Alegria, Jesus, and Content, Alain. 1987. “The Relationships between Segmental Analysis and Alphabetic Literacy: An Interactive View.” Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive 7:415–38.Google Scholar
Ohala, John J. 1992. “The Segment: Primitive or Derived?” In Papers in Laboratory Phonology, Vol. 2, Gesture, Segment, Prosody, ed. Docherty, Gerard J. and Ladd, D. Robert, 166–83. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Pierrehumbert, Janet B. 2003. “Probabilistic Phonology: Discrimination and Robustness.” In Probabilistic Linguistics, ed. Bod, Rens, Hay, Jennifer, and Jannedy, Stefanie, 177228. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Pisoni, David B., and Levi, Susannah V.. 2007. “Some Observations on Representations and Representational Specificity in Speech Perception and Spoken Word Recognition.” In The Oxford Handbook of Psycholinguistics, ed. Gaskell, Gareth, 318. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Port, Robert F. 2007a. “The Graphical Basis of Phones and Phonemes.” In Language Experience in Second Language Speech Learning: In Honor of James Emil Flege, ed. Bohn, Ocke-Schwen and Munro, Murray J., 349–65. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Port, Robert F.. 2007b. “How Are Words Stored in Memory? Beyond Phones and Phonemes.” New Ideas in Psychology 25:143–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Port, Robert F.. 2010. “Rich Memory and Distributed Phonology.” Language Sciences 32:4355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Port, Robert F., and Leary, Adam P.. 2005. “Against Formal Phonology.” Language 85:927–64.Google Scholar
Potochnik, Angela. 2017. Idealization and the Aims of Science. Chicago: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prince, Alan, and Smolensky, Paul. 2004. Optimality Theory: Constraint Interaction in Generative Grammar. London: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Qu, Qingqing, Damian, Markus, and Kazanina, Nina. 2012. “Sound-Sized Segments Are Significant for Mandarin Speakers.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 109:14265–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raimy, Eric, and Cairns, Charles E.. 2015. “Introduction.” In The Segment in Phonetics and Phonology, ed. Raimy, Eric and Cairns, Charles E., 121. London: Wiley.Google Scholar
Read, Charles, Yun-Fei, Zhang, Hong-Yin, Nie, and Bao-Qing, Ding. 1986. “The Ability to Manipulate Speech Sounds Depends on Knowing Alphabetic Writing.” Cognition 24:3144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rozin, Paul, and Gleitman, Lila R.. 1977. “The Structure and Acquisition of Reading.” Pt. 2, “The Reading Process and the Acquisition of the Alphabetic Principle.” In Toward a Psychology of Reading, ed. Reber, Arthur S. and Scarborough, Don L., 55141. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Scholz, Barbara C., Pelletier, Francis J., and Pullum, Geoffrey K.. 2016. “Philosophy of Linguistics.” In The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy, ed. Zalta, Edward N.. Stanford, CA: Stanford University. https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/win2016/entries/linguistics/.Google Scholar
Shockey, Linda. 2003. Sound Patterns of Spoken English. Oxford: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silverman, Daniel. 2006. A Critical Introduction to Phonology: Of Sound, Mind, and Body. London: Continuum.Google Scholar
Smolensky, Paul, Goldrick, Matthew, and Mathis, Donald. 2014. “Optimization and Quantization in Gradient Symbol Systems: A Framework for Integrating the Continuous and the Discrete in Cognition.” Cognitive Science 38:1102–38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevens, Kenneth N. 1989. “On the Quantal Nature of Speech.” Journal of Phonetics 17:346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Studdert-Kennedy, Michael. 1998. “The Particulate Origins of Language Generativity: From Syllable to Gesture.” In Approaches to the Evolution of Language, ed. Hurford, James R., Studdert-Kennedy, Michael, and Knight, Chris, 202–21. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
van der Hulst, Harry. 2003. “Cognitive Phonology.” In Germania et Alia: A Linguistic Webschrift for Hans den Besten on the Occasion of His 55th Birthday, ed. Jan Koster and Henk Van Riemsdijk. http://www.let.rug.nl/~koster/DenBesten/contents.htm.Google Scholar
van Gelder, Timothy. 1995. “What Might Cognition Be, If Not Computation?Journal of Philosophy 92:345–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Gelder, Timothy, and Port, Robert F.. 1995. “It’s about Time: An Overview of the Dynamical Approach to Cognition.” In Mind as Motion: Explorations in the Dynamics of Cognition, ed. Port, Robert F. and Gelder, Timothy van, 144. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Varzi, Achille C. 2011. “Boundaries, Conventions, and Realism.” In Carving Nature at Its Joints: Natural Kinds in Metaphysics and Science, ed. Campbell, Joseph K., O’Rourke, Michael, and Slater, Matthew H., 129–53. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Vaux, Bert. 2011. “Language Games.” In The Handbook of Phonological Theory, 2nd ed., ed. John Goldsmith, Jason Riggle, and Alan C. L. Yu, 722–50. London: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Wan, I-Ping, and Jaeger, Jeri. 1998. “Speech Errors and the Representation of Tone in Mandarin Chinese.” Phonology 15:417–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Warren, Richard M. 1970. “Perceptual Restoration of Missing Speech Sounds.” Science 176:392–93.Google Scholar
Weisberg, Michael. 2013. Simulation and Similarity: Using Models to Understand the World. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
West, Paula. 1999. “Perception of Distributed Coarticulatory Properties of English /l/ and /r/.” Journal of Phonetics 27:405–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wetzel, Linda. 2009. Types and Tokens: On Abstract Objects. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiese, Richard. 2000. The Phonology of German. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar