Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-t5tsf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-09T09:12:18.794Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Evolution of Cooperative Behavior and Its Implications for Ethics

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2022

Abstract

While many philosophers agree that evolutionary theory has important implications for the study of ethics, there has been no consensus on what these implications are. I argue that we can better understand these implications by examining two related yet distinct issues in evolutionary theory: the evolution of our moral beliefs and the evolution of cooperative behavior. While the prevailing evolutionary account of morality poses a threat to moral realism, a plausible model of how altruism evolved in human beings provides the grounding for a research program that focuses on achieving some of the more practical goals shared by ethicists.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Philosophy of Science Association

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Batson, C. Daniel (1990), “Affect and Altruism”, in Moore, Bert S. and Isen, Alice M. (eds.), Affect and Social Behavior. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 89125.Google Scholar
Brosnan, Sarah F., and de Waal, Frans B. M. (2003), “Monkeys Reject Unequal Pay”, Monkeys Reject Unequal Pay 425:297299.Google ScholarPubMed
Crandall, James E. (1978), “Effects of Threat and Failure on Concern for Others”, Effects of Threat and Failure on Concern for Others 12:350360.Google Scholar
Darwin, Charles (1871), The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex. London: Murray.Google Scholar
de Waal, Frans (2006), Primates and Philosophers: How Morality Evolved. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ernst, Zachary (2007), “The Liberationists’ Attack on Moral Intuitions”, American Philosophical Quarterly 44 (2): 129142..Google Scholar
Greene, Joshua (2010), The Terrible, Horrible, No Good, Very Bad Truth about Morality and What to Do about It. New York: Penguin, forthcoming.Google Scholar
Haidt, Jonathan (2001), “The Emotional Dog and Its Rational Tail: A Social Intuitionist Approach to Moral Judgment”, The Emotional Dog and Its Rational Tail: A Social Intuitionist Approach to Moral Judgment 108 (4): 814834..Google ScholarPubMed
Isen, Alice M., Horn, N., and Rosenhan, David L. (1973), “Effects of Success and Failure on Children's Generosity”, Effects of Success and Failure on Children's Generosity 27:239247.Google Scholar
Joyce, Richard (2001), The Myth of Morality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lyubomirsky, Sonja, and Ross, Lee (1997), “Hedonic Consequences of Social Comparison: A Contrast of Happy and Unhappy People”, Hedonic Consequences of Social Comparison: A Contrast of Happy and Unhappy People 73:11411157.Google ScholarPubMed
Markey, Sean (2003), “Monkeys Show Sense of Fairness, Study Says”, Natural Geographic News (September 17), http://www.primates.com/monkeys/fairness.html.Google Scholar
Maynard Smith, John (1964), “Group Selection and Kin Selection”, Group Selection and Kin Selection 201:14451447.Google Scholar
Midlarsky, Elizabeth (1971), “Aiding under Stress: The Effects of Competence, Dependence, Visibility, and Fatalism”, Aiding under Stress: The Effects of Competence, Dependence, Visibility, and Fatalism 39:132149.Google Scholar
Moore, Bert S., and Isen, Alice M. (1990), “Affect and Social Behavior”, in Moore, Bert S. and Isen, Alice M. (eds.), Affect and Social Behavior. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 121.Google Scholar
Ruse, Michael (1995a), “Evolution and Ethics: The Sociobiological Approach”, in Pojman, Louis P. (ed.), Ethical Theory: Classical and Contemporary Readings. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth, 91122.Google Scholar
Ruse, Michael (1995b), Evolutionary Naturalism. London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruse, Michael, and Wilson, Edward O. (1984), “Moral Philosophy as Applied Science”, in Sober, Elliott (ed.), Conceptual Issues in Evolutionary Biology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 555574.Google Scholar
Sayre-McCord, Geoffrey (1988), Essays on Moral Realism. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Sober, Elliott, and Wilson, David Sloan (1998), Unto Others. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Weyant, James M. (1978), “Effects of Mood States, Costs, and Benefits on Helping”, Effects of Mood States, Costs, and Benefits on Helping 36:11691176.Google Scholar
Weyment, Heidi, and Taylor, Shelley (1995), “Self-Evaluation Processes: Motives, Information Use, and Self-Esteem”, Self-Evaluation Processes: Motives, Information Use, and Self-Esteem 63 (4): 729757..Google Scholar
Whitfield, John (2003), “Monkeys Strike for Justice”, Nature News (September 18), http://www.bioedonline.org/news/news.cfm?art=524.Google Scholar
Wilson, Edward O. (1975), Sociobiology: The New Synthesis. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar