Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T00:48:51.128Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Relativity and Representativeness

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 March 2022

Kenneth R. Hammond*
Affiliation:
University of Colorado

Extract

Certain suggestions recently made by Brunswik (2) concerning the design of experiments in psychology seem to have far reaching implications. Indeed, Brunswik's suggestions appear to the writer to be congruent with Einstein's General Theory of Relativity. Congruences between such diverse disciplines as psychology and physics bear watching if for no other reason than the fact that psychologists frequently point to the physicist as the ideal scientist. Unfortunately, in the writer's opinion, the ideal which the psychologist still admires is the classical, or Newtonian, physicist rather than the modern, or Einsteinian, physicist—the psychologist's outlook being concomitantly distorted. Brunswik's theories of experimental design in psychology, however, are appealing for the very reason that they do not fit the mold of the classical physical experiment, but do seem to be congruent with Einstein's physical theories—with concomitant expansion of scope.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © 1951, The Williams & Wilkins Company

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

(1) Brillouin, L. Life, thermodynamics, and cybernetics. Amer. Scientist, 1949, 37, 554568.Google ScholarPubMed
(2) Brunswik, E. Systematic and representative design of psychological experiments. Univ. of Calif. Press, 1947.Google Scholar
(3) Einstein, A. The meaning of relativity. Princeton Univ. Press, 1946.Google Scholar
(4) Einstein, A., and Infeld, L. The evolution of modern physics. New York: Simon and Schuster, 1938.Google Scholar
(5) Flanagan, J. C. (Ed.). The Aviation Psychology Program in the Army Air Forces. Army Air Forces Aviation Psychology Program Research Report No. 1, Washington: U. S. Government Printing Office, 1948.Google Scholar
(6) Frank, P. Modern science and its philosophy. Harvard Univ. Press, 1949.Google Scholar
(7) Spence, K. Cognitive vs. stimulus-response theories of learning. Psychol. Rev., 1950, 57, 159171.10.1037/h0058250CrossRefGoogle Scholar