Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rcrh6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T05:05:47.232Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Mental Simulation and Motor Imagery

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 April 2022

Gregory Currie*
Affiliation:
Department of Philosophy, Flinders University of South Australia
Ian Ravenscroft*
Affiliation:
Department of Philosophy, Flinders University of South Australia
*
Send reprint requests to the authors, Philosophy Department, Flinders University of South Australia, GPO Box 2100, Adelaide, SA, 5001, Australia.

Abstract

Motor imagery typically involves an experience as of moving a body part. Recent studies reveal close parallels between the constraints on motor imagery and those on actual motor performance. How are these parallels to be explained? We advance a simulative theory of motor imagery, modeled on the idea that we predict and explain the decisions of others by simulating their decision-making processes. By proposing that motor imagery is essentially off-line motor action, we explain the tendency of motor imagery to mimic motor performance. We close by arguing that a simulative theory of motor imagery gives (modest) support to and illumination of the simulative theory of decision-prediction.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © 1997 by the Philosophy of Science Association

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

This research was supported by the Australian Research Council (grant no. A59602059). We are indebted to Larry Parsons and Ian Gold for helpful advice. We would also like to thank two anonymous referees for their comments.

References

Adams, L., Guz, A., Innes, J., and Murphy, K. (1987), “The Early Circulatory and Ventilatory Response to Voluntary and Electrically Induced Exercise in Man”, Journal of Physiology 383: 1930.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S. (1995), Mindblindness. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bisiach, E. and Luzzatti, C. (1978), “Unilateral Neglect of Representational Space”, Cortex 14: 129133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bower, J. (forthcoming): “Is the Cerebellum Sensory for Motor's Sake, or Motor for Sensory's Sake? The View From The Whiskers of a Rat”, Progress in Brain Research.Google Scholar
Brooks, L. (1967), “The Suppression of Visualization by Reading”, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 19: 289299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooper, L. and Shepard, R. (1975), “Mental Transformations in the Identification of Left and Right Hands”, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 104: 4856.Google ScholarPubMed
Currie, G. (1995), “Visual Imagery as the Simulation of Vision”, Mind and Language 10: 2544.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Currie, G. (in preparation), “Planning and Predicting Bodily Movements”.Google Scholar
Davidson, R. and Schwartz, G. (1977), “Brain Mechanisms Subserving Self-generated Imagery: Electrophysiological Specificity and Patterning”, Psychophysiology 14: 598602.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davies, M. and Stone, T. (1995a), Folk Psychology. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Davies, M. and Stone, T. (1995b), Mental Simulation. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Decety, J., Jeannerod, M., Germain, M., and Pastene, J. (1991), “Vegetative Response During Imagined Movement is Proportional to Mental Effort”, Behavioral Brain Research 34: 3542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Decety, J., Jeannerod, M., and Preblanc, C. (1989), “The Timing of Mentally Represented Actions”, Behavioral and Brain Research 34: 3542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Decety, J., Sjoholm, H., Ryding, E., Stenberg, G., and Ingvar, D. (1990), “The Cerebellum Participates in Cognitive Activity: Tomographic Measurements of Regional Cerebral Blood Flow”, Brain Research 535: 313317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denis, M. and Cocude, M. (1989), “Scanning Visual Images Generated from Verbal Descriptions”, European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 1: 293307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dominey, P., Decety, J., Brouselle, E., Chazot, G., and Jeannerod, M. (1995), “Motor Imagery of a Lateralized Sequential Task is Asymmetrically Slowed in Hemi-Parkinson's Patients”, Neuropsychologia 33: 727741.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evarts, E. (1968), “Relation of Pyramidal Tract Activity to Force Exerted during Voluntary Movement”, Journal of Neurophysiology 31: 1427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farah, M. (1989), “The Neural Basis of Mental Imagery”, Trends in Neuroscience 12: 395399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farah, M., Soso, M., and Dasheiff, R. (1992), “Visual Angle of the Mind's Eye Before and After Unilateral/Occipital Lobectomy”, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 18: 241246.Google ScholarPubMed
Finke, R. and Kurtzman, H. (1981), “Mapping the Visual Field in Imagery”, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 110: 501517.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, P., Pardo, J., Petersen, S., and Raichle, M. (1987), “Supplementary Motor and Premotor Responses to Actual and Imagined Hand Movements with Positron Emission Tomography”, Society for Neuroscience Abstracts 13: 1433.Google Scholar
Gao, J., Parsons, L., Bower, J., Xiong, J., Li, J., and Fox, P. (1996), “Cerebellum Implicated in Sensory Acquisition and Discrimination Rather than Motor Control”, Science 272: 545547.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Georgopoulos, A. and Grillner, S. (1989), “Visuomotor Coordination in Reaching and Locomotion”, Science 245: 12091210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldenberg, G., Podreka, I., Steiner, M., Willmes, K., Suess, E., and Deecke, L. (1989a), “Regional Cerebral Blood Flow Patterns in Visual Imagery”, Neuropsychologia 27: 641664.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldenberg, G., Podreka, I., Uhl, F., Steiner, M., Willmes, K., and Deecke, L. (1989b), “Cerebral Correlates of Imagining Colours, Faces and a Map”, Neuropsychologia 27: 13151328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldman, A. (1989), “Interpretation Psychologized”, Mind and Language Mind and Language: 4161. Reprinted in Davies and Stone 1995a.Google Scholar
Goldman, A. (1992): “In Defense of the Simulation Theory”, Mind and Language Mind and Language: 7104. Reprinted in Davies and Stone 1995a.Google Scholar
Gordon, R. (1986), “Folk Psychology as Simulation”, Mind and Language Mind and Language: 1158. Reprinted in Davies and Stone 1995a.Google Scholar
Grandy, R. (1973), “Reference, Meaning, and Belief”, Journal of Philosophy 70: 439452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heal, J. (1986), “Replication and Functionalism”, in Butterfield, J. (ed.), Language, Mind and Logic. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 135150. Reprinted in Davies and Stone 1995a.Google Scholar
Jeannerod, M. (1994), “The Representing Brain: Neural Correlates of Motor Intention and Imagery”, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17: 187202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kosslyn, S. (1978), “Measuring the Visual Angle of the Mind's Eye”, Cognitive Psychology 10: 356389.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kosslyn, S., Albert, N., Thompson, W., Maljkovic, V., Weise, S., Chabris, C., Hamilton, S., Rauch, S., and Buananno, F. (1993), “Visual Mental Imagery Activates Topographically Organized Visual Cortex: PET Investigations”, Cognitive Neuroscience 5: 263287.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kosslyn, S., Ball, T., and Reiser, B. (1978), “Visual Images Preserve Metric Spatial Information: Evidence of Studies from Image Scanning”, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 4: 4760.Google ScholarPubMed
Kosslyn, S. and Koenig, O. (1992), Wet Mind. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Leslie, A. (1994), “Pretending and Believing: Issues in the Theory of ToMM”, Cognition 50: 211238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mahoney, M. and Avener, M. (1987), “Psychology of the Elite Athlete. An Explorative Study”, Cognitive Therapy and Research 1: 135141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meltzoff, A. and Gopnik, A. (1993), “The Role of Imitation in Understanding Persons and Developing Theories of Mind”, in Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., and Cohen, D., (ed.), S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, and D. Cohen, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 335366.Google Scholar
Metzler, J. and Shepard, R. (1971), “Mental Rotation of Three-dimensional Objects”, Science 171: 701703.Google Scholar
Morton, A. (1994), “Motor Simulation”, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17: 215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nichols, S., Stich, S., Leslie, A., and Klein, D. (1996), “Varieties of Off-Line Simulation”, in P. Carruthers and P. Smith (ed.) Theories of Theories of Mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 3947.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parsons, L. (1987a), “Imagined Spatial Transformations of One's Body”, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 116: 172191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parsons, L. (1987b), “Imagined Spatial Transformations of One's Hands and Feet”, Cognitive Psychology 19: 178241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parsons, L. (1994), “Temporal and Kinematic Properties of Motor Behavior Reflected in Mentally Simulated Action”, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 20: 709730.Google ScholarPubMed
Parsons, L., Fox, P., Downs, J., Glass, T., Hirsch, T., Martin, C., Jerabek, P., and Lancaster, J. (1995a), “Use of Implicit Motor Imagery for Visual Shape Discrimination as Revealed by PET”, Nature 375: 5458.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parsons, L., Gabrieli, J., and Gazzaniga, M. (1995b), “Cerebrally-Lateralized Mental Representations of Hand Shape and Movement”, presented at the Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Nov. 1995, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Richardson, A. (1967), “Mental Practice: A Review and Discussion”, parts I and II, Research Quarterly 38: 95–107; 262273.Google ScholarPubMed
Roland, P. and Friberg, L. (1985), “Localization of Cortical Areas Activated by Thinking”, Journal of Neurophysiology 53: 12191243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roland, P., Larsen, B., Lassen, N., and Skinhoj, E. (1980), “Supplementary Motor Area and Other Cortical Areas in Organization of Voluntary Movements in Man”, Journal of Neurophysiology 43: 118136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, F. and Baron-Cohen, S. (forthcoming), “Imagining Real and Unreal Things: Evidence of a Dissociation in Autism”, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.Google Scholar
Segal, G., (1996), “The Modularity of Theory of Mind”, in P. Carruthers and P. Smith (ed.) Theories of Theories of Mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 141157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sekiyama, K. (1982), “Kinesthetic Aspects of Mental Representation in the Identification of Left and Right Hands”, Perception and Psychophysics 32: 8995.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sirigu, A., Cohen, L., Duhamel, J., Pillon, B., Dubois, B., Agid, Y., and Pierrot-Deseilligny, C. (1995a), “Congruent Unilateral Impairments for Real and Imagined Hand Movements”, NeuroReport 6: 9971001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sirigu, A., Duhamel, J., Pillon, B., Cohen, L., Dubois, B., and Agid, Y. (1995b), “Mental Simulation of Movement in Patients with Posterior Parietal Cortex Lesions”, Abstract, Society of Neurosciences Meeting, Nov. 1995.Google Scholar
Stich, S. and Nichols, S. (1992), “Folk Psychology: Simulation or Tacit Theory?”, Mind and Language Mind and Language: 735. Reprinted in Davies and Stone 1995a.Google Scholar
Yue, G. and Cole, K. (1992), “Strength Increases from the Motor Program: Comparison of Training with Maximal Voluntary and Imagined Muscle Contractions”, Journal of Neurophysiology 67: 11141123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed