Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-28T06:48:53.629Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Cognitive Revolution and the Political Psychology of Elite Decision Making

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 May 2013

Emilie M. Hafner-Burton
Affiliation:
Laboratory on International Law and Regulation (ILAR) and School of International Relations and Pacific Studies (IR/PS) at the University of California–San Diego (UCSD)
D. Alex Hughes
Affiliation:
Laboratory on International Law and Regulation (ILAR) and Department of Political Science at the University of California–San Diego (UCSD)
David G. Victor
Affiliation:
Laboratory on International Law and Regulation (ILAR) and School of International Relations and Pacific Studies (IR/PS) at the University of California–San Diego (UCSD)

Abstract

Experimental evidence in cognitive psychology and behavioral economics is transforming the way political science scholars think about how humans make decisions in areas of high complexity, uncertainty, and risk. Nearly all those studies utilize convenience samples of university students, but in the real world political elites actually make most pivotal political decisions such as threatening war or changing the course of economic policy. Highly experienced elites are more likely to exhibit the attributes of rational decision-making; and over the last fifteen years a wealth of studies suggest that such elites are likely to be more skilled in strategic bargaining than samples with less germane experience. However, elites are also more likely to suffer overconfidence, which degrades decision-making skills. We illustrate implications for political science with a case study of crisis bargaining between the US and North Korea. Variations in the experience of US elite decision-makers between 2002 and 2006 plausibly explain the large shift in US crisis signaling better than other rival hypotheses such as “Iraq fatigue.” Beyond crisis bargaining other major political science theories might benefit from attention to the attributes of individual decision-makers.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © American Political Science Association 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ackerman, Phillip L., and Schneider, Walter. 1985. “Individual Differences in Automatic and Controlled Information Processing.” In Individual Differences in Cognition, vol. 2, ed. Dillon, R. F.. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Adelson, Beth. 1984. “When Novices Surpass Experts: The Difficulty of a Task May Increase with Expertise.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 10(3): 483–95.Google Scholar
Alevy, Jonathan E., Haigh, Michael S., and List, John A.. 2007. “Information Cascades: Evidence from a Field Experiment with Financial Market Professionals.” Journal of Finance 62(1): 151–80.Google Scholar
Alvarez, R. Michael, and Brehm, John. 2002. Hard Choices, Easy Answers: Values, Information, and American Public Opinion. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Andreoni, James, and Sprenger, Charles. 2010. “Certain and Uncertain Utility: The Allais Paradox and Five Decision Theory Phenomena.” Unpublished Manuscript. Available at Levine's Working Paper Archive.Google Scholar
Axelrod, Robert, and Hamilton, William D.. 1981. “The Evolution of Cooperation.” Science 211(4489): 1390–96.Google Scholar
Bargh, John A. 1984. “Automatic and Conscious Processing of Social Information.” In Handbook of Social Cognition, vol. 3, eds. Wyer, R.S. and Srull, T.K.. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Beer, Francis A., Sinclair, Grant P., Healy, Alice F., and Bourne, Lyle E. Jr. 1995. “Peace Agreement, Intractable Conflict, Escalation Trajectory: A Psychological Laboratory Experiment.” International Studies Quarterly 39(3): 297312.Google Scholar
Bernhard, Helen, Fischbacher, Urs, and Fehr, Ernst. 2006. “Parochial Altruism in Humans.” Nature 442(7105): 912–15.Google Scholar
Bénabou, Roland, and Tirole, Jean. 2002. “Self-Confidence and Personal Motivation.” Quarterly Journal of Economics 117(3): 871915.Google Scholar
Besley, Timothy, and Reynal-Querol, Marta. 2011. “Do Democracies Select More Educated Leaders?American Political Science Review 105(3): 552–66.Google Scholar
Bhatt, Meghana, and Camerer, Colin F.. 2005. “Self-Referential Thinking and Equilibrium as States of Mind in Games: fMRI Evidence.” Games and Economic Behavior 52(2): 424–59.Google Scholar
Binmore, Ken, McCarthy, John, Ponti, Giovanni, Samuelson, Larry, and Shaked, Avner. 2002. “A Backward Induction Experiment.” Journal of Economic Theory 104(1): 4888.Google Scholar
Blavatskyy, Pavlo. 2009. “Betting on Own Knowledge: Experimental Test of Overconfidence.” Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 38(1): 3949.Google Scholar
Bolton, John. 2007. Surrender Is Not an Option: Defending America at the United Nations and Abroad. New York: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Bullock, John G. 2011. “Elite Influence on Public Opinion in an Informed Electorate.” American Political Science Review 105(3): 496515.Google Scholar
Burdein, Inna, Lodge, Milton, and Taber, Charles. 2006. “Experiments on the Automaticity of Political Beliefs and Attitudes. Political Psychology 27(3): 359–71.Google Scholar
Bush, George W. 2002. Remarks by the President in State of the Union Address. The White House, Office of the Press Secretary, Washington, DC. Retrieved from: http://georgewbush-whitehouse.archives.gov/news/releases/2002/01/20020129-11.htmlGoogle Scholar
Bush, George W. 2010. Decision Points. New York: Broadway.Google Scholar
Cacioppo, John T., Petty, Richard E., Feinstein, Jeffrey A., and Jarvis, W. Blair G.. 1996. “Dispositional Differences in Cognitive Motivation: The Life and Times of Individuals Varying in Need for Cognition.” Psychological Bulletin 119(2): 197253.Google Scholar
Camerer, Colin F. 1997. “Progress in Behavioral Game Theory.” Journal of Economic Perspectives 11(4): 167–88.Google Scholar
Camerer, Colin F., Ho, Teck-Hua, and Chong, Kuan. 2003. “Models of Thinking, Learning, and Teaching in Games.” American Economic Review 93(2): 192–95.Google Scholar
Camerer, Colin F., Loewenstein, George, and Prelec, Drazen. 2005. “Neuroeconomics: How Neuroscience Can Inform Economics.” Journal of Economic Literature 43: 964.Google Scholar
Camerer, Colin F., and Lovallo, Dan. 1999. “Overconfidence and Excess Entry: An Experimental Approach.” American Economic Review 89(1): 306–18.Google Scholar
Chase, William G., and Simon, Herbert A.. 1973. “Perception in Chess.” Cognitive Psychology 4(1): 5581.Google Scholar
Chen, Serena, and Chaiken, Shelly. 1999. “The Heuristic-Systematic Model in its Broader Context.” In Dual-Process Theories in Social Psychology, ed. Chaiken, Shelly and Trope, Yaacov. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Cheney, Dick. 2011. In My Time: A Personal and Political Memoir. New York: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Cheng, Li, and White, Lynn. 1990. “Elite Transformation and Modern Change in Mainland China and Taiwan: Empirical Data and the Theory of Technocracy.” China Quarterly 121: 135.Google Scholar
Chi, Michelene T.H., and Glaser, Robert. 1978. “Knowledge Structures and Memory Development.” In Children's Thinking: What Develops? ed. Siegler, R.. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Chiesi, Harry L., Spilich, George J., and Voss, James F.. 1979. “Acquisition of Domain-Related Information in Relation to High and Low Domain Knowledge.” Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 18(3): 257–73.Google Scholar
Chin, Michelle L., Bond, Jon R., and Geva, Nehemia. 2000. “A Foot in the Door: An Experimental Study of PAC and Constituency Effects on Access.” Journal of Politics 62(2): 534–49.Google Scholar
Cokely, Edward T., Parpart, Paula, and Schooler, Lael J.. 2009. “On the Link between Cognitive Control and Heuristic Processes.” In Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, ed. Taatgen, N.A. and van Rijn, H.. Austin: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Cooper, David J., Kagel, John H., Lo, Wei, and Gu, Qing Liang. 1999. “Gaming against Managers in Incentive Systems: Experimental Results with Chinese Students and Chinese Managers.” American Economic Review 89(4): 781804.Google Scholar
Coricelli, Giorgio, and Nagel, Rosemarie. 2009. “Neural Correlates of Depth of Strategic Reasoning in Medial Prefrontal Cortex.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 106(23): 9163–68.Google Scholar
Costa-Gomes, Miguel A., and Crawford, Vincent P.. 2006. “Cognition and Behavior in Two-Person Guessing Games: An Experimental Study.” American Economic Review 96(5): 1737–68.Google Scholar
Costa-Gomes, Miguel A., Crawford, Vincent P., and Broseta, Bruno. 2001. “Cognition and Behavior in Normal-Form Games: An Experimental Study.” Econometrica 69(5): 1193–235.Google Scholar
Crawford, Vincent P. 2008. “Modeling Behavior in Novel Strategic Situations via Level-k Thinking.” Presented at GAMES 2008, Third World Congress of the Game Theory Society, Evanston, IL July 14.Google Scholar
De Groot, Adriann D. 1965. Thought and Choice in Chess. The Hague: Mouton De Gruyter.Google Scholar
DeNardo, James. 1995. The Amateur Strategist: Intuitive Deterrence Theories and the Politics of the Nuclear Arms Race. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Druckman, James N. 2001. “The Implications of Framing Effects for Citizen Competence.” Political Behavior 23(2): 225–56.Google Scholar
Duffy, John, and Nagel, Rosemarie. 1997. “On the Robustness of Behaviour in Experimental ‘Beauty Contest’ Games.” Economic Journal 107: 16842000.Google Scholar
Egan, Dennis E., and Schwartz, Barry J.. 1979. “Chunking in Recall of Symbolic Drawings.” Memory & Cognition 7(2): 149–58.Google Scholar
Eisenstadt, Marc, and Kareev, Yaakov. 1975. “Aspects of Human Problem Solving: The Use of Internal Representations.” In Explorations in Cognition, ed. Norman, D. A. and Rumelhart, D. E.. San Francisco: W.H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Engelmann, Dirk, and Hollard, Guillaume. 2010. “Reconsidering the Effect of Market Experience on the ‘Endowment Effect’.” Econometrica 78(6): 2005–19.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. Anders. 2006. “The Influence of Experience and Deliberate Practice on the Development of Superior Expert Performance.” In The Cambridge Handbook of Expertise and Expert Performance, ed. Ericsson, K. Anders, Charness, Neil, Feltovich, Paul J. and Hoffman, Robert R.. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Evans, Jonathan St. B.T. 1989. “Concepts and Inference.” Mind & Language 4(1-2): 2934.Google Scholar
Farnham, Barbara. 1992. “Roosevelt and the Munich Crisis: Insights from Prospect Theory.” Political Psychology 13(2): 205–35.Google Scholar
Farnham, Barbara. 2004. “Impact of the Political Context on Foreign Policy Decision-Making.” Political Psychology 25(3): 441–63.Google Scholar
Fearon, James D. 1995. “Rationalist Explanations for War.” International Organization 49(3): 379414.Google Scholar
Feltovich, Paul J., Prietula, Michael J., and Ericsson, K. Anders. 2006. “Studies of Expertise from Psychological Perspectives.” In The Cambridge Handbook of Expertise and Expert Performance, ed. Ericsson, K. Anders, Charness, Neil, Feltovich, Paul J., and Hoffman, Robert R.. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University.Google Scholar
Fitts, Paul M., and Posner, Michael I.. 1967. Human Performance. Oxford, UK: Brooks/Cole.Google Scholar
Fowler, James, and Schreiber, Darren. 2008. “Biology, Politics, and the Emerging Science of Human Nature.” Science 322(5903): 912–14.Google Scholar
Galasso, Vincenzo, and Nannicini, Tommaso. 2011. “Competing on Good Politicians.” American Political Science Review 105(1): 7999.Google Scholar
Gerber, Alan S., Gimpel, James G., Green, Donald P., and Shaw, Daron R.. 2011. “How Large and Long-lasting Are the Persuasive Effects of Televised Campaign Ads? Results from a Randomized Field Experiment.” American Political Science Review 105(1): 135–50.Google Scholar
Gervais, Simon, and Goldstein, Itay. 2003. “Overconfidence and Team Coordination.” The Rodney L. White Center for Financial Research working paper, Wharton School, University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Gick, Mary L., and Holyoak, Keith J.. 1980. “Analogical Problem Solving.” Cognitive Psychology 12(3): 306–55.Google Scholar
Gick, Mary L., and Holyoak, Keith J.. 1983. “Schema Induction and Analogical Transfer.” Cognitive Psychology 15(1): 138.Google Scholar
Glaser, R., and Chi, M.T.H.. 1988. “Overview.” In The Nature of Expertise, ed. Chi, Michelene T.H., Glaser, R., and Farr, Marshall J.. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gneezy, Uri, and List, John A.. 2006. “Putting Behavioral Economics to Work: Testing for Give Exchange in the Labor Market Using Field Experiments.” Econometrica 74(5): 1365–84.Google Scholar
Goette, L., Huffman, D., and Meier, S.. 2006. “The Impact of Group Membership on Cooperation and Norm Enforcement: Evidence Using Random Assignment to Real Social Groups.” American Economic Review 96(2): 212–16.Google Scholar
Graham, Loren R. 1994. Science in Russia and the Soviet Union: A Short History. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hafner-Burton, Emilie M., LeVeck, Brad L., Victor, David G., and Fowler, James H.. 2012. “A Behavioral Approach to International Legal Cooperation.” Available at: http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=1969905Google Scholar
Hafner-Burton, Emilie M., LeVeck, Brad L., and Victor, David G.. 2012. “Strategic Enforcement: Results from an Elite Survey Experiment on International Trade Agreements.” In Proceedings of the 2012 Annual Meeting of the American Association of Political Science. Available at: http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=2132948Google Scholar
Haggard, Stephan, and Noland, Marcus. 2011. Engaging North Korea: The Role of Economic Statecraft. Honolulu: East-West Center Monograph #59.Google Scholar
Haigh, Michael S., and List, John A.. 2005. “Do Professional Traders Exhibit Myopic Loss Aversion? An Experimental Analysis.” Journal of Finance 60(1): 523–34.Google Scholar
Harbaugh, William T., Krause, Kate, and Vesterlund, Lise. 2001. “Are Adults Better Behaved Than Children? Age, Experience, and the Endowment Effect.” Economic Letters 70: 175–81.Google Scholar
Hardin, Curtis D., and Rothman, Alexander J.. 1997. “Rendering Accessible Information Relevant: The Applicability of Everyday Life.” In The Automaticity of Everyday Life, ed. Wyer, Robert S. Jr. and Bargh, John A.. New York: Routledge, Taylor and Francis Group.Google Scholar
Hedinger, Beat, and Goette, Lorenz. 2006. “Cooperation in the Cockpit: Evidence of Reciprocity and Trust Among Swiss Air Force Pilots.” Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Herrmann, Richard K., and Shannon, Vaughn P.. 2001. “Defending International Norms: The Role of Obligation, Material Interest, and Perception in Decision Making.” International Organization 55(3): 621–54.Google Scholar
Herrmann, Richard K., Tetlock, Philip E., and Diascro, M.. 2001. “How Americans Think about Trade: Reconciling Conflicts among Money, Power, and Principles.” International Studies Quarterly 45(2): 191218.Google Scholar
HerrnsteinRichard J,. Richard J,., and Murray, Charles A.. 1996. The Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Hodges, Nicola, Starkes, Janet, and MacMahon, Clare. 2006. “Expert Performance in Sports: A Cognitive Perspective.” In The Cambridge Handbook of Expertise and Expert Performance, ed. Ericsson, K. Anders, Charness, Neil, Feltovich, Paul, and Hoffman, Robert R.. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Holsti, Ole R., and Rosenau, James N.. 1984. American Leadership in World Affairs: Vietnam and the Breakdown of Consensus. Winchester, MA: Allen & Unwin.Google Scholar
Holsti, Ole R., and Rosenau, James N.. 1993. “The Structure of Foreign Policy Beliefs among American Opinion Leaders after the Cold War.” Millennium-Journal of International Studies 22(2): 235.Google Scholar
Hoppe, Hans-Hermann. 2001. Democracy—The God That Failed: The Economics and Politics of Monarchy, Democracy, and Natural Order. Piscataway: Transaction Publishers.Google Scholar
Isaac, Jeffrey C. 1987. “Beyond the Three Faces of Power: A Realist Critique.” Polity 21(1): 431.Google Scholar
Jervis, Robert. 1968. “Hypotheses on Misperception.” World Politics 20(3): 454–79.Google Scholar
Jervis, Robert. 1978. “Cooperation Under the Security Dilemma.” World Politics 30(2): 167214.Google Scholar
Johnson, Dominic D.P. 2004. Overconfidence and War: The Havoc and Glory of Positive Illusions. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, Dominic D.P., and Fowler, James H.. 2011. “The Evolution of Overconfidence.” Nature 477: 317–20.Google Scholar
Johnson, Eric J., Camerer, Colin, Sen, Sankar, and Rymon, Talia. 2002. “Detecting Failures of Backward Induction: Monitoring Information Search in Sequential Bargaining.” Journal of Economic Theory 104(1): 1647.Google Scholar
Johnson, Jonathan L., Daily, Catherine M., and Ellstrand, Allen E.. 1996. “Boards of Directors: A Review and Research Agenda.” Journal of Management 22(3): 409–38.Google Scholar
Jouini, Elyes, Napp, Clotilde, and Viossat, Yannick. 2013. “Evolutionary Strategic Beliefs and Financial Markets.” Review of Finance 17(2): 727–66.Google Scholar
Kahneman, Daniel. 2011. Thinking, Fast and Slow. New York: Farrar Straus & Giroux.Google Scholar
Kahneman, Daniel, and Tversky, Amos. 1979. “Prospect Theory: An Analysis of Decision under Risk.” Econometrica 47(2): 263–91.Google Scholar
Kahneman, Daniel, Knetsch, Jack L., and Thaler, Richard H.. 1991. “Anomalies: The Endowment Effect, Loss Aversion, and Status Quo Bias.” Journal of Economic Perspectives 5(1): 193206.Google Scholar
Keynes, John M. 1936. The General Theory of Employment, Interest, and Money. London: Macmillan Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
King, Erika G., and Wells, Robert A.. 2009. Framing the Iraq War Endgame. New York: Palgrave Macmillian.Google Scholar
Knetsch, Jack L. 1989. “The Endowment Effect and Evidence of Nonreversible Indifference Curves.” American Economic Review 79(5): 1277–84.Google Scholar
Koopman, Cheryl, McDermott, Rose, Jervis, Robert, Snyder, Jack, and Dioso, Joe. 1995. “Stability and Change in American Elite Beliefs about International Relations.” Peace and Conflict: Journal of Peace Psychology 1(4): 365–82.Google Scholar
Laibson, David. 1997. “Golden Eggs and Hyperbolic Discounting.” Quarterly Journal of Economics 112(2): 443–77.Google Scholar
La Porta, Rafael, Lopez-de-Silanes, Florencio, Shleifer, Andrei, and Vishny, Robert W.. 1997. “Trust in Large Organizations.” American Economic Review 87(2): 333–38.Google Scholar
Larkin, Jill, McDermott, John, Simon, Dorothea P., and Simon, Herbert A.. 1980. “Expert and Novice Performance in Solving Physics Problems.” Science 208(4450): 1335–42.Google Scholar
Lehmann, Andreas C., and Gruber, Hans. 2006. “Music.” In The Cambridge Handbook of Expertise and Expert Performance, ed. Ericsson, K. Anders, Charness, Neil, Feltovich, Paul J., and Hoffman, Robert R.. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lesgold, Alan, Rubinson, Harriet, Feltovich, Paul, Glaser, Robert, Klopfer, Dale, and Wang, Yen. 1988. “Expertise in a Complex Skill: Diagnosing X-Ray Pictures.” In The Nature of Expertise, ed. Chi, Michelene T.H., Glaser, Robert and Farr, Marshall J.. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
LeVeck, Brad L. 2012. “Coordination Incentives Can Explain Diverse and Simplistic Partisan Worldviews: An Experimental Test.” In Proceedings of the 2011 Annual Meeting of the American Association of Political Science. Available at: http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=1902654Google Scholar
Levitt, Steven D., List, John A., and Sadoff, Sally E.. 2011. “Checkmate: Exploring Backward Induction among Chess Players.” American Economic Review 101(2): 975–90.Google Scholar
Levy, Jack S. 1997. “Prospect Theory, Rational Choice, and International Relations.” International Studies Quarterly 41(1): 87112.Google Scholar
Lieberman, Matthew D., Schreiber, Darren, and Ochsner, Kevin N.. 2003. “Is Political Cognition Like Riding a Bicycle? How Cognitive Neuroscience Can Inform Research on Political Thinking.” Political Psychology 24(4): 681704.Google Scholar
Lieberman, Matthew D., Jarcho, Johanna M., and Satpute, Ajay B.. 2004. “Evidence-Based and Intuition-Based Self-Knowledge: An fMRI Study.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 87(4): 421–35.Google Scholar
Lin, Shi-Woei, and Bier, Vicki M.. 2008. “A Study of Expert Overconfidence.” Reliability Engineering and System Safety 93(5): 711–21.Google Scholar
List, John A. 2003. “Does Market Experience Eliminate Market Anomalies?Quarterly Journal of Economics 118(1): 4171.Google Scholar
List, John A., and Mason, Charles F.. 2009. “Are CEOs Expected Utility Maximizers?Journal of Econometrics 162(1): 114–23.Google Scholar
Lopez, Anthony, McDermott, Rose, and Peterson, Michael Bang. 2011. “States in Mind: Evolution, Coalitional Psychology, and International Politics.” International Security 36(2): 4883.Google Scholar
Lukes, Steven. 1974. Power: A Radical View. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
March, James G. 1978. “Bounded Rationality, Ambiguity, and the Engineering of Choice.” Bell Journal of Economics 9(2): 587608.Google Scholar
Massey, Cade, and Thaler, Richard H.. 2010. “The Loser's Curse: Overconfidence vs. Market Efficiency in the National Football League Draft.” Available at: http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/Papers.cfm?abstract_id=697121Google Scholar
McDermott, Rose. 1992. “Prospect Theory in International Relations: The Iranian Hostage Rescue Mission.” Political Psychology 13(2): 237–63.Google Scholar
McDermott, Rose. 2004a. “The Feeling of Rationality: The Meaning of Neuroscientific Advances for Political Science.” Perspectives on Politics 2(4): 691706.Google Scholar
McDermott, Rose. 2004b. “Prospect Theory in Political Science: Gains and Losses from the First Decade.” Political Psychology 25(2): 289312.Google Scholar
McDermott, Rose. 2011. “New Directions for Experimental Work in International Relations.” International Studies Quarterly 55(2): 503–20.Google Scholar
McDermott, Rose, Fowler, James H., and Smirnov, Oleg. 2008. “On the Evolutionary Origin of Prospect Theory Preferences.” Journal of Politics 70(2): 335–50.Google Scholar
McDermott, Rose, Cowden, Jonathan, and Koopman, Cheryl. 2002. “Framing, Uncertainty, and Hostile Communications in a Crisis Experiment.” Political Psychology 23(1): 133–49.Google Scholar
McKay, Ryan T., and Dennett, Daniel C.. 2009. “The Evolution of Misbelief.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32(6): 493510.Google Scholar
McKelvey, Richard D., and Palfrey, Thomas R.. 1992. “An Experimental Study of the Centipede Game.” Econometrica 60(4): 803–36.Google Scholar
Mills, Charles W. 2000. The Power Elite. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mintz, Alex. 2004. “Foreign Policy Decision Making in Familiar and Unfamiliar Settings: An Experimental Study of High-Ranking Military Officers.” Journal of Conflict Resolution 48(1): 91104.Google Scholar
Nagel, Rosemarie. 1993. “Experimental Results on Interactive Competitive Guessing.” Discussion paper serie b, University of Bonn, Germany.Google Scholar
Neale, Magaret A., and Bazerman, Max H.. 1985. “The Effects of Framing and Negotiator Overconfidence on Bargaining Behaviors and Outcomes.” Academy of Management Journal 28(1): 3449.Google Scholar
Neustadt, Richard E., and May, Ernest R.. 1988. Thinking in Time: The Uses of History for Decision-Makers. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Oxley, Douglas R., Smith, K.B., Alford, J.R., Hibbing, M.V., Miller, J.L., Scalora, M., Hatemi, P.K., and Hibbing, J.R.. 2008. “Political Attitudes Vary with Physiological Traits.” Science 321(5896): 1667–70.Google Scholar
Pakulski, Jan. 2008. “Elite Theory.” In International Encyclopedia of the Social Sciences, Ed. Darity, William A. Jr. vol. 2, 2d ed. Detroit: Macmillan Reference USA.Google Scholar
Pareto, Vilfredo. 1935. The Mind and Society, ed. Livingstone, Arthur., 4 vol.New York: Harcourt Brace.Google Scholar
Pašeta, Senia. 1999. Before the Revolution: Nationalism, Social Change and Ireland's Catholic Elite 1879–1922. Cork, Ireland: Cork University Press.Google Scholar
Patel, Vimla L., Kaufman, David R., and Arocha, Jose F.. 1995. “Steering through the Murky Waters of a Scientific Conflict: Situated and Symbolic Models of Clinical Cognition.” Artificial Intelligence in Medicine 7(5): 413–38.Google Scholar
Petrusa, Emil R. 2002. “Clinical Performance Assessments.” In International Handbook of Research in Medical Education, vol. 7, ed. Norman, Geoffrey R., van der Vleuten, Cees P.M., and Dordrecht, D.I. Newble., The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.Google Scholar
Plott, Charles R., and Zeiler, Kathryn. 2005. “The Willingness to Pay-Willingness to Accept Gap, the ‘Endowment Effect,’ Subject Misconceptions, and Experimental Procedures for Eliciting Valuations.” American Economic Review 95(3): 530–45.Google Scholar
Popkin, Samuel L., and Dimock, Michael A.. 2000. “Knowledge, Trust, and International Reasoning.” In Elements of Reason: Cognition, Choice, and the Bounds of Rationality, ed. Lupia, Arthur, McCubbins, Matthew D., and Popkin, Samuel L.. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Pritchard, Charles L. 2007. Failed Diplomacy: The Tragic Story of How North Korea Got the Bomb. Washington, DC: Brookings Institution Press.Google Scholar
Reder, Lynne M., and Schunn, Christian D.. 1996. “Metacognition Does Not Imply Awareness: Strategy Choice Is Governed by Implicit Learning and Memory.” In Implicit Memory and Metacognition, ed. Reder, Lynne M.. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Reny, Phillip J. 1988. “Common Knowledge and Games with Perfect Information.” In PSA: Proceedings of the Biennial Meeting of the Philosophy of Science Association, Vol. 2: Symposia and Invited Papers. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Rice, Condoleezza. 2011. No Higher Honor: A Memoir of My Years in Washington. New York: Crown Publishers.Google Scholar
Robbins, Alexandra. 2002. Secrets of the Tomb. New York: Little, Brown, and Company.Google Scholar
Rothman, Alexander J., and Hardin, Curtis D.. 1997. “Differential Use of the Availability Heuristic in Social Judgment.” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 23(2): 123–38.Google Scholar
Rubin, Aviad. 2009. “Political-Elite Formation and Transition to Democracy in Pre-State Conditions: Comparing Israel and the Palestinian Authority.” Government and Opposition 44(43): 262–84.Google Scholar
Rubinstein, Ariel. 1982. “Perfect Equilibrium in a Bargaining Model.” Econometrica 50(1): 97109.Google Scholar
Rubinstein, Ariel. 2003. “‘Economics and Psychology’? The Case of Hyperbolic Discounting.” International Economic Review 44(4): 1207–16.Google Scholar
Ruffle, Bradley J., and Sosis, Richard. 2006. “Cooperation and the In-Group-Out-Group Bias: A Field Test on Israeli Kibbutz Members and City Residents.” Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 60(2): 147–63.Google Scholar
Rumsfeld, Donald. 2011. Known and Unknown: A Memoir. New York: Sentinel.Google Scholar
Schelling, Thomas C. 1960. The Strategy of Conflict. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Schelling, Thomas C. 1966. Arms and influence. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Schneider, W., and Shiffrin, R.M.. 1977. “Controlled and Automatic Human Information Processing: I. Detection, Search, and Attention.” Psychological Review 84(1): 166.Google Scholar
Schreiber, Darren. 2007. “Political Cognition as Social Cognition: Are We All Political Sophisticates?” In The Affect Effect: Dynamics of Emotion in Political Thinking and Behavior, ed. Neuman, W. Russell, Marcus, George E., Crigler, Ann N., and MacKuen, Michael. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Sears, David O. 1986. “College Sophomores in the Laboratory: Influences of a Narrow Data Base on Social Psychology's View of Human Nature.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 51(3): 515–30.Google Scholar
Shiffrin, R.M., and Schneider, W.. 1977. “Controlled and Automatic Human Information Processing: II. Perceptual Learning, Automatic Attending, and a General Theory.” Psychological Review 84(2): 127–90.Google Scholar
Shih, Victor. 2008. Factions and Finance in China: Elite Conflict and Inflation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Simon, Herbert A. 1955. “A Behavioral Model of Rational Choice.” Quarterly Journal of Economics 69(1): 99118.Google Scholar
Simon, Herbert A. 1956. “Rational Choice and the Structure of the Environment.” Psychological Review 63(2): 129–38.Google Scholar
Simon, Herbert A. 1985. “Human Nature in Politics: The Dialogue of Psychology with Political Science.” American Political Science Review 79(2): 293304.Google Scholar
Sloboda, J.A. 1976. “Visual Perception of Musical Notation: Registering Pitch Symbols in Memory.” Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 28(1): 116.Google Scholar
Stahl, Dale O., and Wilson, Paul W.. 1994. “Experimental Evidence on Players' Models of Other Players.” Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 25(3): 309–27.Google Scholar
Stahl, Dale O., and Wilson, Paul W.. 1995. “On Players' Models of Other Players: Theory and Experimental Evidence.” Games and Economic Behavior 10(1): 218–54.Google Scholar
Suedfeld, Peter, and Tetlock, Philip E., eds. 1991. Psychology and Social Policy. New York: Hemisphere Publishing.Google Scholar
Suskind, Ron. 2006. The one percent doctrine: Deep inside America's pursuit of its enemies since 9/11. New York: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Tetlock, Philip E. 2005. Expert Political Judgment: How Good Is It? How Can We Know? Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Tetlock, Philip E., and Goldgeier, J.M.. 2000. “Human Nature and World Politics: Cognition, Identity, and Influence.” International Journal of Psychology 35(2): 8796.Google Scholar
Thaler, Richard H., and Sunstein, Cass R.. 2008. Nudge: Improving Decisions about Health, Wealth and Happiness. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Tingley, Dustin H. 2011. “The Dark Side of the Future: An Experimental Test of Commitment Problems in Bargaining.” International Studies Quarterly 55(2): 521–44.Google Scholar
Tingley, Dustin H., and Walter, Barbara F.. 2011. “The Effect of Repeated Play on Reputation Building: An Experimental Approach.” International Organization 65(2): 343–65.Google Scholar
Tomz, Michael. 2007. “Domestic Audience Costs in International Relations: An Experimental Approach.” International Organization 61(4): 821–40.Google Scholar
Trivers, Robert. 2011. Deceit and Self-Deception: Fooling Yourself the Better to Fool Others. Allen Lane.Google Scholar
Voss, James F., Vesonder, Gregg T., and Spilich, George J.. 1980. “Text Generation and Recall by High-Knowledge and Low-Knowledge Individuals.” Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 19(6): 651–67.Google Scholar
Voss, James F., Tyler, S.W., and Yengo, L.A.. 1983. “Individual Differences in the Solving of Social Science Problems.” In Individual Differences in Cognition, vol. 1, ed. Dillon, R.F.Sneck, R. R.. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Weyland, Kurt. 1996. “Risk Taking in Latin American Economic Restructuring: Lessons from Prospect Theory.” International Studies Quarterly 40(2): 185208.Google Scholar
Wilson, Rick K., Stevenson, Randolph, and Potts, Geoffrey. 2006. “Brain Activity in the Play of Dominant Strategy and Mixed Strategy Games.” Political Psychology 27(3): 459478.Google Scholar
Wit, Joel S., Poneman, Daniel B., and Gallucci, Robert L.. 2004. Going Critical: The First North Korean Nuclear Crisis. Washington, DC: Brookings Institution Press.Google Scholar
Withers, Michael C., Hillman, Amy J., and Cannella, Albert A. Jr. 2012. “A Multidisciplinary Review of the Director Selection Literature.” Journal of Management 38(1): 243–77.Google Scholar
Zahra, Shaker A., and Pearce, John A. II. 1989. “Boards of Directors and Corporate Financial Performance: A Review and Integrative Model.” Journal of Management 15(2): 291334.Google Scholar
Zhang, Qi, and Liu, Mingxing. 2010. “Local Political Elite, Partial Reform Symptoms, and the Business and Market Environment in Rural China.” Business and Politics 12(1): 141.Google Scholar
Zimmerman, Barry, and Campillo, Magda. 2003. “Motivating Self-Regulated Problem Solvers.” In The Psychology of Problem Solving, ed. Davidson, Janet E. and Sternberg, Robert J.. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Hafner-Burton Supplementary Material

Hafner-Burton Supplementary Material

Download Hafner-Burton Supplementary Material(File)
File 58.9 KB