Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T16:57:56.526Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A Tribute to Jaak Panksepp (1943–2017)

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 August 2018

Kenneth L. Davis
Affiliation:
Pegasus International, Greensboro, NC, USA
Christian Montag*
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychology and Education, Ulm University, Ulm, Germany Key Laboratory for NeuroInformation/Center for Information in Medicine, School of Life Science and Technology, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China, Chengdu, China
*
Author for correspondence: Christian Montag, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This article gives a short overview on the life and achievements of Jaak Panksepp. Jaak Panksepp dedicated his life to the study of mammalian emotions. By means of electrical stimulation of the brain and psychopharmacological challenges he carved out seven primary emotional systems being highly conserved across different species of mammals including homo sapiens. The primary emotional systems are called SEEKING, CARE, LUST, PLAY (positive emotions), and FEAR, RAGE, SADNESS (negative emotions). While his early career was characterized by the direct study of these primary emotions in mammals, in his late career he invested more and more time in applying his knowledge to different fields of psychology including personality neuroscience and psychiatry.

Type
Short Communication
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCND
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-ncnd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is unaltered and is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained for commercial re-use or in order to create a derivative work.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2018

On the life of Jaak Panksepp

Jaak Panksepp, a founding member of the Editorial Board of Personality Neuroscience, died on April 18, 2017. Born in Estonia on June 5, 1943, his family escaped the ravages of post-WWII Russian occupation by moving to the United States when Jaak was still very young. Much later in 2011, Jaak was honored by the President of Estonia personally presenting him with the Order of the White Star.

Jaak started his college education as an engineering student at the University of Pittsburgh but soon switched to clinical psychology. To help finance his college education, he took a job on a psychiatric hospital ward where he had the opportunity to study patients first hand by interacting with them as well as reviewing their clinical files. It was such experience that gave this gifted student the insight that the key to understanding psychopathology and human behavior was first to understand emotions, an insight more likely to be made by an idealistic young mind given the behavioristic and learning theory zeitgeist prevailing in American psychology at that time. His insight led him to pursue a graduate career in (what today would be called) the neuroscience of emotions at the University of Massachusetts. However, Jaak’s further insight was that the way to understand human emotions was to begin with animal experimental brain research. He realized that brain science techniques had developed the tools to support “preclinical” research that could help clinical psychology escape the orbit of traditional speculation-based clinical theory into a new sphere in which psychiatric treatments could be based on brain research—this was very much in the tradition of Ivan Pavlov.

This was the mindset that the young Jaak took into graduate school where Jay Trowill, his major professor, gave him the space to explore his insights through experimental brain research. In this setting, Jaak began building on the work of pioneers like Hess (Reference Hubel and Wiesel1957) to understand what emotional insights could be revealed by probing the rat hypothalamus. Among other things, he discovered that two kinds of aggression could be elicited through the electrical stimulation of the brain to discrete regions of the rat hypothalamus: the first type was an anger-type attack (RAGE) provoked in nature by bodily restraint (like when captured by a predator), or the protection of resources necessary for living (such as food); the second type would later be called a quiet-biting attack that was a function of the SEEKING system and in this case an extension of dopaminergic foraging behavior manifested as predation (Panksepp & Trowill, Reference Panksepp, Wright, Döbrössy, Schlaepfer and Coenen1971). Electrical stimulation of the brain, which Jaak would use as a tool throughout his professional career, provided a compelling demonstration of the evolved instinctual emotional roots embedded in the subcortical brain. Specifically, by introducing a crude electrical stimulation to particular subcortical brain regions, such as the hypothalamus, one could elicit coherent complex emotional behavioral displays from the animal.

After a brief interlude studying energy balance and hunger, which he would later distinguish as a homeostatic rather than emotional affect, Jaak accepted a position in a burgeoning physiological psychology program at Bowling Green State University (BGSU). There he began pursuing his “opioid hypothesis,” which closely followed the discovery of opioid receptors as well as endogenous opioids in the brain. Jaak capitalized on Scott’s (Elliot & Scott, Reference Florio, Albert, Taverna, Namba, Brandl, Lewitus and Huttner1961; Davis, Gurski, & Scott, Reference Davis, Gurski and Scott1977) ongoing work in social attachment and separation distress at BGSU, and he guided many students within a cross-species research program that delineated the anatomical and pharmacological dynamics of the separation distress experience. This research included an early demonstration of separation distress alleviation using low-dose morphine in canine puppies (Panksepp, Herman, Conner, Bishop, & Scott, Reference Panksepp, Jalowiec, DeEskinazi and Bishop1978)—one of many publications in this area, which fit in with his youthful goal of understanding emotions as the route to understanding psychopathology. Jaak’s work with opioids eventually led to his contributions to the treatment of autistic children with naltrexone (an opioid blocker; e.g., see Sahley & Panksepp, Reference Sharma, Angelucci and Sur1987).

Later, Jaak became known as the “rat tickler”. For Jaak, this meant the prototypical “rough-and-tumble” mammalian PLAY system. Among his many contributions in this area was the demonstration that neonatal decortication did not diminish play in juvenile rats (Panksepp, Normansell, Cox, & Siviy, Reference Panksepp and Trowill1994). Indeed, he provided evidence that play in these neonatally decorticated rats could not be reliably distinguished from sham-operated control rats. Moreover, Jaak discovered “rat laughter” underlining his efforts to dedicate his late research life to understand the molecular underpinnings of joy as a route to find new targets to treat psychiatric disorders (e.g., Panksepp & Burgdorf, Reference Panksepp, Herman, Conner, Bishop and Scott2003). Jaak also switched to exclusively study the science of joy and positive affect, because he was convinced that animals feel raw affects in many of the same ways as humans, with neural circuitries underlying these emotions being highly conserved evolutionarily across mammals (see also his interesting works on consciousness of animals and his interest in studying the human soul: Panksepp, Reference Panksepp1998, Reference Panksepp and Panksepp2005a, Reference Panksepp2007; Alcaro, Carta, & Panksepp, Reference Alcaro, Carta and Panksepp2017).

Jaak carved “Mother Nature at her joints” and delineated seven primary emotions deeply rooted in subcortical areas of the human brain. Always written in capital letters to designate their specific scientific meaning, he labeled them SEEKING, LUST, CARE, PLAY (positive emotions) and RAGE, FEAR, SADNESS (negative emotions), which also have been described in detail in his classic textbook “Affective Neuroscience” originally published in 1998. Of note, Jaak, in 2012, published an update of this influential book called “The Archaeology of Mind” together with Lucy Biven (Panksepp & Biven, Reference Panksepp and Biven2012). His last book called “The Emotional Foundations of Personality” will be posthumously published in 2018 (Davis & Panksepp, Reference Davis and Panksepp2018).

Figure 1 Jaak Panksepp dedicated his professional career to the study of mammalian emotions (picture taken by Henry Moore Jr., CVM/BCU, Washington State University).

Jaak strongly believed that the neocortex was a “tabula rasa” brain region. While many subcortical brain regions were genetically programmed, the neocortex had to be programmed by experience. Further, as demonstrated in the decorticated rat pups, the neocortex was not necessary for the expression of primary emotions. Merker (Reference Montag and Panksepp2007) had shown that anencephalic human children likewise displayed a full complement of primary emotions. Jaak also frequently cited Mriganka Sur’s work showing that auditory cortex could process visual stimuli when thalamic inputs were neonatally rerouted (e.g., Sur, Garraghty, & Roe, Reference Yovell, Bar, Mashiah, Baruch, Briskman, Asherov and Panksepp1988; Sharma, Angelucci, & Sur, Reference Sindermann, Luo, Zhao, Li, Li, Kendrick and Montag2000). Jaak felt that Hubel and Wiesel’s (Reference Merker1959) work with feline vision was strong evidence that sensory processing had to be “programmed” into the cortex confirming that cortical processes were not genetically determined. A capstone on this debate was the Florio et al. (Reference Hess2015) Science paper reporting on the evolutionary expansion of the human neocortex by a single gene (shared by Neanderthals and Denisovans but not by other primates) that greatly expanded the human neocortex through increased basal progenitor proliferation. Altogether, Jaak was deeply skeptical about finding innate neural modules with distinct evolved functions in the recently expanded human neocortex with cortical plasticity being the norm and the spectacular representational and re-representational capacities of the human neocortex being acquired through experience likely guided by subcortical processes.

Since moving to Washington State University in 2006, Jaak seemed to be devoting more effort toward fulfilling his original dream of utilizing knowledge about mammalian emotions to develop psychiatric treatments. In this realm he invested much energy into the International Neuropsychoanalysis Society. In conjunction with the Falk Institute, he focused on gene expression in playing rats, which eventually resulted in GLYX-13, a rapidly acting safe antidepressant that is currently in phase III trials at the Food and Drug Administration (see also Moskal, Burgdorf, Kroes, Brudzynski, & Panksepp, Reference Özkarar-Gradwohl, Panksepp, İçöz, Çetinkaya, Köksal, Davis and Scherler2011). He also helped coordinate work on deep brain stimulation to the medial forebrain bundle of treatment-resistant depression patients (Panksepp, Wright, Döbrössy, Schlaepfer, & Coenen, Reference Pascazio, Bembich, Nardone, Vecchiet, Guarino and Clarici2014). Jaak’s efforts with Yovell et al. (Reference Elliot and Scott2016) demonstrated the successful treatment of suicidal ideation with low-dose buprenorphine, a “safe” opioid agonist–antagonist with a “ceiling effect” for respiratory depression.

Since 2003 until early 2017, Jaak invested substantial time to bring his affective neuroscience theory (ANT) to other research areas including (neuroscientific-oriented) personality psychology (Davis & Panksepp, Reference Davis and Panksepp2011; Davis, Panksepp, & Normansell, Reference Davis, Panksepp and Normansell2003; Montag & Panksepp, Reference Montag and Panksepp2017a, Reference Montag and Pankseppb). In particular, the construction of the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales (ANPS; Davis & Panksepp, Reference Davis and Panksepp2011; Davis, Panksepp, & Normansell, Reference Davis, Panksepp and Normansell2003) together with Ken Davis made it possible to apply Jaak’s theory to many areas of psychology—this scale is available in many languages including English (Davis, Panksepp, & Normansell, Reference Davis, Panksepp and Normansell2003), German (Reuter, Panksepp, Davis, & Montag, Reference Sahley and Panksepp2017), Italian (Pascazio et al., Reference Reuter, Panksepp, Davis and Montag2015), French (Pahlavan, Mouchiroud, Zenasni, & Panksepp, Reference Panksepp2008), Spanish (Abella, Panksepp, Manga, Bárcena, & Iglesias, Reference Abella, Panksepp, Manga, Bárcena and Iglesias2011), Turkish (Özkarar-Gradwohl et al., Reference Pahlavan, Mouchiroud, Zenasni and Panksepp2014), Chinese (Sindermann et al., Reference Sur, Garraghty and Roe2018), and a Japanese version from Keichi Narita’s group that will be published soon.

Why is Panksepp’s theory of tremendous importance to study personality? As pointed out in earlier works (e.g., Davis, Panksepp, & Normansell, Reference Davis, Panksepp and Normansell2003; Montag & Panksepp, Reference Montag and Panksepp2017) individual differences in primary emotional systems could represent the phylogenetically oldest part of human personality. This idea has not only been derived by stable associations across cultures (see references above) between individual differences in primary emotional systems as assessed with the ANPS and the Big Five personality model (e.g., as assessed with the NEO-Five Factor Inventory), but also from the fact that primary emotions are seated in ancient subcortical brain regions driving our behavior in a bottom-up fashion. In particular, for neuroscientific-oriented personality psychologists, Panksepp’s ANT provides researchers with abundant biological candidates to be tested for linkages to human personality. Thus, Panksepp’s ANT is a roadmap to study the neuroscientific basis of personality. To give an example: Montag and Panksepp (Reference Montag and Panksepp2017) demonstrated that PLAY might be the emotional foundation of extraversion. As Panksepp, Jalowiec, DeEskinazi, and Bishop (Reference Panksepp, Lane, Solms and Smith1985) provided evidence that PLAY behavior in rats is influenced by opioids, individual differences in opioid levels or genetic variations of opioid-related genes might be interesting candidates to understand individual differences in extraversion. An overview on candidates to be tested both on neurotransmitter/neuropeptide but also brain anatomical level in the context of personality neuroscience can be found in Montag and Panksepp (Reference Moskal, Burgdorf, Kroes, Brudzynski and Panksepp2017b) and in Montag and Davis (Reference Montag and Panksepp2018).

Jaak put tremendous efforts into building bridges with other emotion theories such as Ekman’s facial theory of emotion (Montag & Panksepp, Reference Montag and Panksepp2016). In the light of the late criticism by constructivists in emotion research he pointed out that “A primary-process/basic emotion view may prevail in many subcortical regions, and constructivist/dimensional approaches may effectively parse higher emotional concepts as processed by the neocortex In other words, such debates may simply reflect investigators working at different levels of control” (Panksepp, Reference Panksepp and Biven2010, p. 536). See also his last efforts to find consensus between more affective and cognitive driven neuroscientists (Panksepp et al., Reference Panksepp, Lane, Solms and Smith2017).

As fate would have it, the young student who dreamed about understanding psychopathology by first understanding emotions was himself to experience an intense bout with strong negative emotion. Namely, in what he would describe as “the most painful time of my life” (Panksepp, Reference Panksepp1998, cited from the preface), his 16-year-old daughter, along with three friends, was killed by a drunk driver, which precipitated an acute depressive episode in his life that significantly stalled the writing of his “Affective Neuroscience” (Panksepp, Reference Panksepp1998). As Jaak described it, after two and one-half demoralized years, “Through the magic of friends and modern psychiatric drugs, my spirits were partially restored” (again cited from the preface; Panksepp, Reference Panksepp1998), and he was able to restart and eventually complete his massive writing project.

On a more joyful note, Jaak was able to watch his younger son, Jules, complete a PhD in neuroscience at BGSU and continue on to gain his own independent research footing. Jules is currently in the Oregon Health and Science University Department of Behavioral Neuroscience, and in addition to his personal work, he and Jaak have published several joint articles, the most recent being a chapter on empathy in the APA Handbook of Comparative Psychology (Panksepp & Panksepp, 2017)

Jaak was a wonderful and warm-hearted colleague whose interest in the study of the human mind never ceased. Until his last days he was an enormously productive scientist fostering research across disciplines. He is deeply missed. If he had one last message to leave us, it would likely be to continue to focus on expanding the knowledge of primary-process mammalian emotions as understanding our subcortical brain may prove to be the limiting factor as we attempt to comprehend our human mind.

Acknowledgment

The position of C.M. is funded by a Heisenberg grant awarded to him by the German Research Foundation (DFG MO2363/3-2).

Conflicts of Interest. The authors have nothing to disclose.

References

Abella, V., Panksepp, J., Manga, D., Bárcena, C. Iglesias, J. A. (2011). Spanish validation of the affective neuroscience personality scales. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 14, 926935. https://doi.org/10.5209/rev_SJOP.2011.v14.n2.38.Google Scholar
Alcaro, A., Carta, S. Panksepp, J. (2017). The affective core of the self: A neuro-archetypical perspective on the foundations of human (and animal) subjectivity. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1424. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01424.Google Scholar
Davis, K. L., Gurski, J. C. Scott, J. P. (1977). Interaction of separation distress with fear in infant dogs. Developmental Psychobiology, 10, 203212. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.420100304.Google Scholar
Davis, K. L. Panksepp, J. (2011). The brain’s emotional foundations of human personality and the affective neuroscience personality scales. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 19461958. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.04.004.Google Scholar
Davis, K. L., Panksepp, J. Normansell, L. (2003). The affective neuroscience personality scales: Normative data and implications. Neuropsychoanalysis, 5, 5769. https://doi.org/10.1080/15294145.2003.10773410.Google Scholar
Davis, K. L. Panksepp, J. (2018). The emotional foundations of personality: A neurobiological and evolutionary approach. New York, NY: WW Norton & Company.Google Scholar
Elliot, O. Scott, J. P. (1961). The development of emotional distress reactions to separation, in puppies. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 99, 322. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221325.1961.10534386.Google Scholar
Florio, M., Albert, M., Taverna, E., Namba, T., Brandl, H., Lewitus, E., Huttner, W. B. (2015). Human-specific gene ARHGAP11B promotes basal progenitor amplification and neocortex expansion. Science, 347, 14651470. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aaa1975.Google Scholar
Hess, W. R. (1957). The functional organization of the diencephalon. In J. R. Hughes (Ed.) (pp. 121–125). New York, NY: Grune & Stratton.Google Scholar
Hubel, D. H. Wiesel, T. N. (1959). Receptive fields of single neurones in the cat’s striate cortex. The Journal of Physiology, 148, 574591. https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1959.sp006308.Google Scholar
Merker, B. (2007). Consciousness without a cerebral cortex: A challenge for neuroscience and medicine. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 6381. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X07000891.Google Scholar
Montag, C. Davis, K. (2018). Affective neuroscience theory and personality: An update. Personality Neuroscience, 1, 112. https://doi.org/10.1017/pen.2018.10.Google Scholar
Montag, C. Panksepp, J. (2016). Primal emotional-affective expressive foundations of human facial expression. Motivation and Emotion, 40, 760766. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-016-9570-x.Google Scholar
Montag, C. Panksepp, J. (2017a). Primary emotional systems and personality: An evolutionary perspective. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 464. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00464.Google Scholar
Montag, C. Panksepp, J. (2017b). Personality neuroscience: Why it is of importance to include primary emotional systems! In H. Zeigler & T. Shackelford (Eds.), Encyclopedia of personality and individual differences. Springer. (Published online.)Google Scholar
Moskal, J. R., Burgdorf, J., Kroes, R. A., Brudzynski, S. M. Panksepp, J. (2011). A novel NMDA receptor glycine-site partial agonist, GLYX-13, has therapeutic potential for the treatment of autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 19821988. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.06.006.Google Scholar
Özkarar-Gradwohl, F. G., Panksepp, J., İçöz, F. J., Çetinkaya, H., Köksal, F., Davis, K. L. Scherler, N. (2014). The influence of culture on basic affective systems: The comparison of Turkish and American norms on the affective neuroscience personality scales. Culture and Brain, 2, 173192. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40167-014-0021-9.Google Scholar
Pahlavan, F., Mouchiroud, C., Zenasni, F. Panksepp, J. (2008). Validation de l’adaptation française de l’échelle neuro-affective de personnalité. Revue européenne de psychologie appliquée [European Review of Applied Psychology], 58, 155163. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.erap.2007.08.004.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (2005). Affective consciousness: Core emotional feelings in animals and humans. Consciousness and Cognition, 14, 3080. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2004.10.004.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (2007). Emotional feelings originate below the neocortex: Toward a neurobiology of the soul. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 101103. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X07001094.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (2010). Affective neuroscience of the emotional brainmind: Evolutionary perspectives and implications for understanding depression. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 12, 533545.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. Biven, L. (2012). The archaeology of mind: Neuroevolutionary origins of human emotions. The Norton series on interpersonal neurobiology (1st ed.). New York, NY: Norton.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. Burgdorf, J. (2003). “Laughing” rats and the evolutionary antecedents of human joy? Physiology & Behavior, 79, 533547. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0031-9384(03)00159-8.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J., Herman, B., Conner, R., Bishop, P. Scott, J. P. (1978). The biology of social attachments: Opiates alleviate separation distress. Biological Psychiatry, 13, 607618.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J., Jalowiec, J., DeEskinazi, F. G. Bishop, P. (1985). Opiates and play dominance in juvenile rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 99, 441453. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.99.3.441.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J., Lane, R. D., Solms, M. Smith, R. (2017). Reconciling cognitive and affective neuroscience perspectives on the brain basis of emotional experience. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 76, 187215. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.09.010.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J., Normansell, L., Cox, J. F. Siviy, S. M. (1994). Effects of neonatal decortication on the social play of juvenile rats. Physiology & Behavior, 56, 429443. https://doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(94)90285-2.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. Panksepp, J. (2017). Empathy through the ages: A comparative perspective on rodent models of shared emotion. In J. Call, G. M. Burghardt, I. M. Pepperberg, C. T. Snowdon, & T. Zentall (Eds.), APA handbook of comparative psychology: perception, learning, and cognition (Vol. 2, pp. 765–792). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. Trowill, J. A. (1971). Positive and negative contrast in licking with shifts in sucrose concentration as a function of food deprivation. Learning and Motivation, 2, 4957. https://doi.org/10.1016/0023-9690(71)90047-6.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J., Wright, J. S., Döbrössy, M. D., Schlaepfer, T. E. Coenen, V. A. (2014). Affective neuroscience strategies for understanding and treating depression. Clinical Psychological Science, 2, 472494. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702614535913.Google Scholar
Pascazio, L., Bembich, S., Nardone, I. B., Vecchiet, C., Guarino, G. Clarici, A. (2015). Validation of the Italian translation of the affective neuroscience personality scales. Psychological Reports, 116, 97115. https://doi.org/10.2466/08.09.PR0.116k13w4.Google Scholar
Reuter, M., Panksepp, J., Davis, K. Montag, C. (2017). Affective neuroscience personality scales (ANPS) – Deutsche Version. Göttingen: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Sahley, T. L. Panksepp, J. (1987). Brain opioids and autism: An updated analysis of possible linkages. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17, 201216. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01495056.Google Scholar
Sharma, J., Angelucci, A. Sur, M. (2000). Induction of visual orientation modules in auditory cortex. Nature, 404, 841847. https://doi.org/10.1038/35009043.Google Scholar
Sindermann, C., Luo, R., Zhao, Z., Li, Q., Li, M., Kendrick, K. M., Montag, C. (2018). High ANGER and low agreeableness predict vengefulness in German and Chinese participants. Personality and Individual Differences, 121, 184192. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.09.004.Google Scholar
Sur, M., Garraghty, P. Roe, A. (1988). Experimentally induced visual projections into auditory thalamus and cortex. Science, 242, 14371441. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.2462279.Google Scholar
Yovell, Y., Bar, G., Mashiah, M., Baruch, Y., Briskman, I., Asherov, J., Panksepp, J. (2016). Ultra-low-dose buprenorphine as a time-limited treatment for severe suicidal ideation: A randomized controlled trial. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 173, 491498. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.15040535.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1 Jaak Panksepp dedicated his professional career to the study of mammalian emotions (picture taken by Henry Moore Jr., CVM/BCU, Washington State University).