Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T09:19:08.577Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Perfectionism-related variations in error processing in a task with increased response selection complexity

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 January 2023

André Mattes*
Affiliation:
Department of Individual Differences and Psychological Assessment, University of Cologne, Cologne, Germany
Markus Mück
Affiliation:
Department of Individual Differences and Psychological Assessment, University of Cologne, Cologne, Germany
Jutta Stahl
Affiliation:
Department of Individual Differences and Psychological Assessment, University of Cologne, Cologne, Germany
*
Author for correspondence: André Mattes, Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Perfectionists strive for a flawless performance because they are intrinsically motivated to set and achieve high goals (personal standards perfectionism; PSP) and/or because they are afraid to be negatively evaluated by others (evaluative concern perfectionism; ECP). We investigated the differential relationships of these perfectionism dimensions with performance, post-response adaptation, error processing (reflected by two components of the event-related potential: error/correct negativity – Ne/c; error/correct positivity – Pe/c) and error detection. In contrast to previous studies, we employed a task with increased response selection complexity providing more room for perfectionistic dispositions to manifest themselves. Although ECP was related to indicators of increased preoccupation with errors, high-EC perfectionists made more errors than low-EC perfectionists. This observation may be explained by insufficient early error processing as indicated by a reduced Ne/c effect and a lack of post-response adaptation. PSP had a moderating effect on the relationship between ECP and early error processing. Our results provide evidence that pure-EC perfectionists may spend many of their cognitive resources on error-related contents and worrying, leaving less capacity for cognitive control and thus producing a structural lack of error processing.

Type
Empirical Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

While human beings share basic psychological functions such as information processing, they differ greatly in the specific expression of these functions. One determinant of such variations in psychological functions is perfectionism, that is the stable disposition to strive for flawlessness (Stoeber, Reference Stoeber and Stoeber2018).

Perfectionism consists of two key dimensions (Frost, Heimberg, Holt, Mattia, & Neubauer, Reference Frost, Heimberg, Holt, Mattia and Neubauer1993; Slade & Owens, Reference Slade and Owens1998; Stoeber & Otto, Reference Stoeber and Otto2006): Evaluative concern perfectionism (ECP) describes the tendency to equate mistakes to failure and to be afraid of being evaluated negatively by others based on one’s performance (Frost et al., Reference Frost, Marten, Lahart and Rosenblate1990) and Personal standards perfectionism (PSP) is characterised by setting very high goals for one’s own performance and excessively evaluating oneself based on these criteria (Frost, Marten, Lahart, & Rosenblate, Reference Frost, Marten, Lahart and Rosenblate1990). While ECP has been associated with higher levels of neuroticism, external motivation, fear of failure and an avoidance orientation, PSP has been linked to higher levels of conscientiousness, internal motivation, hope of success and an approach orientation (Stricker, Buecker, Schneider, & Preckel, Reference Stricker, Buecker, Schneider and Preckel2019; Stoeber, Damian, & Madigan, Reference Stoeber, Damian, Madigan and Stoeber2018). An interactionist approach, the 2×2 model of perfectionism by Gaudreau and Thompson (Reference Gaudreau and Thompson2010), does not only consider the two main effects of ECP and PSP in isolation but also takes the within-person combination of both perfectionism dimensions into account, allowing to identify four subtypes of perfectionism: pure-EC perfectionism (ECP high, PSP low), pure-PS perfectionism (ECP low, PSP high), mixed perfectionism (ECP high, PSP high) and non-perfectionism (ECP low, PSP low). Note that despite the term “subtype,” the model is based on fully continuous measures of ECP and PSP (Gaudreau, Reference Gaudreau2013).Footnote 1 Finally, perfectionism also constitutes an important predisposition for psychological disorders (e.g. depression, anxiety and eating disorders; Flett, Hewitt, Blankstein, & Gray, Reference Flett, Hewitt, Blankstein and Gray1998; Flett, Madorsky, Hewitt, & Heisel, Reference Flett, Madorsky, Hewitt and Heisel2002; Gnilka, Ashby, & Noble, Reference Gnilka, Ashby and Noble2012; Rice & Aldea, Reference Rice and Aldea2006; Shafran, Cooper, & Fairburn, Reference Shafran, Cooper and Fairburn2002) and may even serve as a trans-diagnostic factor (Limburg, Watson, Hagger, & Egan, Reference Limburg, Watson, Hagger and Egan2017).

Considering that – independent from the specific perfectionism motivation – perfectionists strive for flawlessness, making an error poses a challenge to perfectionists because it is not compatible with their strivings and concerns. Yet, error processing or – more broadly – response monitoring is an integral part of human behaviour and cognition. It serves to ensure that the intended action is correctly executed and to improve subsequent behaviour if the action is not executed as intended (Wessel, Reference Wessel2018). The error/correct negativity (Ne/c), a component of the event-related potential (ERP), peaks at around 100 ms at fronto-central areas following a response and is usually larger for errors than for correct responses (first mentioned independently by Falkenstein, Hohnsbein, Hoormann, & Blanke, Reference Falkenstein, Hohnsbein, Hoormann and Blanke1991, in a research article; and by Gehring, Coles, Meyer, & Donchin, Reference Gehring, Coles, Meyer and Donchin1990, in a conference abstract). It is assumed to reflect early error processing, and it is usually independent from error awareness (Wessel, Reference Wessel2012) but may signal the need for more cognitive control (Cavanagh & Frank, Reference Cavanagh and Frank2014; Hester, Foxe, Molholm, Shpaner, & Garavan, Reference Hester, Foxe, Molholm, Shpaner and Garavan2005) to improve behaviour on the subsequent trial (King, Korb, Cramon, & Ullsperger, Reference King, Korb, Cramon and Ullsperger2010; Maier, Yeung, & Steinhauser, Reference Maier, Yeung and Steinhauser2011; Mattes, Porth, & Stahl, Reference Mattes, Porth and Stahl2022; Ridderinkhof, Ullsperger, Crone, & Nieuwenhuis, Reference Ridderinkhof, Ullsperger, Crone and Nieuwenhuis2004). Another error-related ERP component is the error/correct positivity (Pe/c) which peaks at around 300 ms at centro-parietal areas following a response. It has been linked to error awareness (Nieuwenhuis, Ridderinkhof, Blom, Band, & Kok, Reference Nieuwenhuis, Ridderinkhof, Blom, Band and Kok2001) and evidence accumulation (Steinhauser & Yeung, Reference Steinhauser and Yeung2010, Reference Steinhauser and Yeung2012) because it is usually larger for aware errors than for unaware errors or correct responses.

Although response monitoring is a basic psychological function, previous research has shown that perfectionists differ in the extent to which they process errors. For example, PSP has been associated with more error-specific activity in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC; Barke et al., Reference Barke, Bode, Dechent, Schmidt-Samoa, van Heer and Stahl2017), the postulated neural generator of the Ne/c (Ridderinkhof et al., Reference Ridderinkhof, Ullsperger, Crone and Nieuwenhuis2004). Furthermore, pure-PS and mixed perfectionists were found to yield more intense early error processing in terms of the Ne/c amplitude (Stahl, Acharki, Kresimon, Völler, & Gibbons, Reference Stahl, Acharki, Kresimon, Völler and Gibbons2015). The Ne/c, in turn, has consistently been linked to improved future behaviour (e.g. Debener et al., Reference Debener, Ullsperger, Siegel, Fiehler, Cramon and Engel2005; Gehring, Goss, Coles, Meyer, & Donchin, Reference Gehring, Goss, Coles, Meyer and Donchin1993; Mattes et al., Reference Mattes, Porth and Stahl2022). Indeed, high-PS perfectionists were shown to exhibit better behavioural adaptation than low-PS perfectionists (i.e. more post-error slowing and a higher post-error accuracy; Barke et al., Reference Barke, Bode, Dechent, Schmidt-Samoa, van Heer and Stahl2017; Stahl et al., Reference Stahl, Acharki, Kresimon, Völler and Gibbons2015), suggesting that high-PS perfectionists may draw on error processing to optimise their behaviour.

Findings that pure-EC perfectionists show less intense early error processing have previously been interpreted as indicating an attempt to avoid error processing and escape the aversive consequences associated with errors such as negative evaluations by others, being faced with the experience of failure or being confronted with one’s own imperfections (Stahl et al., Reference Stahl, Acharki, Kresimon, Völler and Gibbons2015). Alternatively, in pure-EC perfectionists, an error may not signal the need for cognitive control as much as in the other perfectionism subtypes (Cavanagh & Frank, Reference Cavanagh and Frank2014; Ridderinkhof et al., Reference Ridderinkhof, Ullsperger, Crone and Nieuwenhuis2004). While the former explanation assumes a motivational lack of error processing in pure-EC perfectionists, the latter account suggests more of a structural lack of error processing. Findings linking (pure) ECP to increased late error processing as indicated by the Pe/c amplitude (Drizinsky, Zülch, Gibbons, & Stahl, Reference Drizinsky, Zülch, Gibbons and Stahl2016; Tops, Koole, & Wijers, Reference Tops, Koole and Wijers2013) support the notion of a structural lack of error processing in ECP given that aware error processing or error evidence accumulation seem to work well in EC perfectionists.

Reviewing the current literature on perfectionism and error processing, it is astonishing that almost all studies employed relatively simple two-choice tasks (e.g. Barke et al., Reference Barke, Bode, Dechent, Schmidt-Samoa, van Heer and Stahl2017; Drizinsky et al., Reference Drizinsky, Zülch, Gibbons and Stahl2016; Pieters et al., Reference Pieters, Bruijn, Maas, Hulstijn, Vandereycken, Peuskens and Sabbe2007; Schrijvers, Bruijn, Destoop, Hulstijn, & Sabbe, Reference Schrijvers, Bruijn, Destoop, Hulstijn and Sabbe2010; Stahl et al., Reference Stahl, Acharki, Kresimon, Völler and Gibbons2015; Tops et al., Reference Tops, Koole and Wijers2013), given that perfectionistic tendencies may manifest themselves more clearly in more challenging tasks. Indeed, while meta-analyses suggest that perfectionism is associated with performance in several domains (e.g. academic performance: Madigan, Reference Madigan2019; performance in sports: Hill, Mallinson-Howard, & Jowett, Reference Hill, Mallinson-Howard and Jowett2018), no perfectionism-related differences in the correct response rate were found in any of the studies referenced above. It seems that overly simplified experimental tasks may not be suitable to investigate perfectionism-related performance differences that are well-established in real-world settings. To avoid such a potential ceiling effect, we designed a more complex yet not unrealistically difficult task that produced enough error trials to reliably assess the error-related components (Ne/c and Pe/c) and triggered perfectionistic traits due to its challenging nature (Stahl et al., Reference Stahl, Mattes, Hundrieser, Kummer, Mück, Niessen and Dummel2020). We expected that ECP was associated with less deep processing of errors (avoidance orientation). Regarding the underlying mechanisms of the reduced error processing in ECP, we investigated two competing explanations. It could be that high-EC perfectionists intend to avoid the confrontation with unpleasant consequences of their behaviour, for example errors, and thus avoid error processing altogether (avoidance hypothesis). Alternatively, high-EC perfectionists might have fewer cognitive capacities to process their errors because a substantial part of their cognitive capacity is captured by reiterating thoughts about error-related contents or – more generally – worrying (Eysenck, Derakshan, Santos, & Calvo, Reference Eysenck, Derakshan, Santos and Calvo2007; Moser, Reference Moser2017). Hence, error processing might be less deep for high- compared to low-EC perfectionists due to the reduced amount of available cognitive capacity (capacity hypothesis). Furthermore, we expected that PSP was associated with a deeper processing of errors (approach orientation). This would allow high-PS perfectionists to draw information from their current behaviour to adapt and optimise their future behaviour (optimisation hypothesis). To forward our understanding of error processing in perfectionists in the context of these suggested accounts, we explored a conjoint of behavioural and electrophysiological variables in a task with increased response selection complexity (i.e. eight response options).

1. Method

1.1. Participants

A total of 95 participants were recruited via e-mail lists and social media groups using a computer-aided registration tool for experiments (CORTEX; Elson & Bente, Reference Elson and Bente2009). They received course credit for participation. Four participants had to be excluded from the analyses because of technical problems with the recording of triggers. One participant was excluded because the correct response rate was not significantly higher than chance suggesting that this participant did not participate conscientiously. The remaining 90 participants (74 female, 15 male, 1 other) had a mean age of 25.08 years (SD = 7.58). Post hoc sensitivity analyses showed that this sample size allowed us to detect medium effect sizes of r = .30 with an alpha level of 5% and a power of 84% in a multiple regression model with seven predictors (see Statistical analyses for more details). Participants gave their written informed consent, and the study was approved by the ethics committee of the German Psychological Association.

1.2. Psychometric assessment

PSP and ECP scores (mean across items) were assessed using the respective personal standards (here, Cronbach’s α = .81, range: 1.86–5.71) and concern over mistakes scales (here, Cronbach’s α = .91, range: 1.11–5.22) of the German version (Altstötter-Gleich, & Bergemann, Reference Altstötter-Gleich and Bergemann2006) of the Frost Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale (FMPS; Frost et al., Reference Frost, Marten, Lahart and Rosenblate1990), respectively. The items of both perfectionism subscales were measured on a six-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (“trifft gar nicht zu,” roughly: do not agree at all) to 6 (“trifft sehr zu,” roughly: fully agree). We primarily chose these subscales as measures for PSP and ECP to ensure that our results were comparable to previous studies investigating perfectionism-related variations in error processing (e.g. Barke et al., Reference Barke, Bode, Dechent, Schmidt-Samoa, van Heer and Stahl2017; Drizinsky et al., Reference Drizinsky, Zülch, Gibbons and Stahl2016; Stahl et al., Reference Stahl, Acharki, Kresimon, Völler and Gibbons2015), all of which used the same subscales. Furthermore, researchers have argued that the other FMPS subscales are not as central to the two higher-order dimensions as personal standards and concern over mistakes (Stoeber, Reference Stoeber and Stoeber2018). For example, the parental criticism and expectations subscales are more important in the context of developing perfectionism (Damian, Stoeber, Negru, & Băban, Reference Damian, Stoeber, Negru and Băban2013; Rice, Lopez, & Vergara, Reference Rice, Lopez and Vergara2005) and the organisation subscale is often considered as existing in addition to PSP and ECP (Frost et al., Reference Frost, Marten, Lahart and Rosenblate1990; Kim, Chen, MacCann, Karlov, & Kleitman, Reference Kim, Chen, MacCann, Karlov and Kleitman2015). Participants completed the questionnaire on a computer and were not able to leave items unanswered.

1.3. Procedure and experimental task

We used a task introduced by Stahl et al. (Reference Stahl, Mattes, Hundrieser, Kummer, Mück, Niessen and Dummel2020) termed the 8ART (eight-alternative response task, Figure 1). Participants had to learn the assignment of eight symbols to eight of their fingers (thumbs excluded). They were instructed to respond as fast and as accurately as possible to the symbol that was presented on the screen by pressing the key with the corresponding finger (Figure 1A). Each of the eight fingers rested on a separate key throughout the experiment. The trial course is illustrated in Figure 1B. At the beginning of each trial, eight white boxes were displayed horizontally representing the eight fingers. Above each box, the corresponding symbol was presented. In one of the boxes, the target symbol was presented and participants had to press the key with the finger that was assigned to the symbol and explicitly not to the location where the target symbol was presented. The target display (800 ms duration) was followed by a blank screen (1200 ms duration). The RT limit was 1200 ms after target onset. On trials on which responses exceeded this limit, the feedback “zu langsam” (“too slow”) was presented and the trial was terminated. On trials with a valid response, the blank screen was followed by a rating display presented for 2000 ms. The eight-point rating scale ranged from “sicher richtig” (“certainly right”) to “sicher falsch” (“certainly wrong”) and could be answered with the eight response keys. To prevent motor response preparation prior to the onset of the rating scale, the scale orientation was assigned randomly on each trial, that is the anchor “certainly right” was randomly presented at the left or right end of the scale. A cross indicated the chosen box for the remaining rating display. The inter-trial interval varied from 550 to 850 ms [for more details on 8ART see, Stahl et al. (Reference Stahl, Mattes, Hundrieser, Kummer, Mück, Niessen and Dummel2020)].

Figure 1. (A) Stimulus-response assignment and (B) trial course. In the example, a percentage sign which is assigned to the right middle finger is the target, so pressing the button with the right middle finger would be the correct response.

The participants completed a total of twelve blocks, each consisting of 64 trials so that the eight target symbols appeared at each of the eight locations once in each block. The target symbols were presented in random order. The first block was a practice block and did not contain a response time (RT) criterion, and the target display was visible until a response was given to make sure that participants were able to form the symbol-finger assignment. After each block, participants were given a break of at least two minutes.

After the behavioural task, the participants filled in a questionnaire including the PSP and ECP items (Frost et al., Reference Frost, Marten, Lahart and Rosenblate1990).

1.4. Behavioural data

Behavioural data were recorded using eight force-sensitive keys which registered a response when they were pressed with 40 cN or more. A VarioLab AD converter (Becker-Meditec) digitised the analogous signal at a sampling rate of 512 Hz. The actual keys were embedded in individual plastic forms which were separately adjustable for each hand and each finger (not the thumbs). Thus, the hands and fingers had a constant position throughout the experiment. A height adjustable chin rest was placed 85 cm from the screen to reduce general movement artefacts and for a constant eye-screen distance.

1.5. Electroencephalography recording and data preprocessing

The EEG was recorded from 63 scalp electrode sites using actiCAP slim/snap (Brain Products), which were arranged according to the standard international 10–20 system (Jasper, Reference Jasper1958) (FP1, FP2, AF7, AF3, AF4, AF8, F7, F5, F3, F1, Fz, F2, F4, F6, F8, FT7, FC5, FC3, FC1, FCz, FC2, FC4, FC6, FT8, T7, C5, C3, C3’, C1, Cz, C2, C4, C4’, C6, T8, TP7, CP5, CP3, CP1, CPz, CP2, CP4, CP6, TP8, P7, P5, P3, P1, Pz, P2, P4, P6, P8, PO7, PO3, POz, PO4, PO8, PO9, O1, Oz, O2, PO10). The active Ag/AgCl electrodes provided by Brain Products were online referenced against the left mastoid electrode. The reference on the left mastoid was an additional electrode (not included in the actiCAP) that was physically connected to the same amplifier as the other scalp electrodes. We placed a second reference electrode (the 64th active electrode from the cap) on the right mastoid that was later used for offline re-referencing (see below). The EEG signal was recorded continuously at a sampling rate of 500 Hz by means of a BrainAmp DC (Brain Products) amplifier. An additional amplifier BrainAmp E×G (Brain Products) was used to record the Electrooculography (EOG) signals using passive Ag/AgCl bipolar electrodes above and below the left eye (vertical eye movements) and on the lower part of the left and right temples (horizontal eye movements).

The EEG data preprocessing was performed in the EEGLAB toolbox (Delorme & Makeig, Reference Delorme and Makeig2004) and the ERPLAB plugin (Lopez-Calderon & Luck, Reference Lopez-Calderon and Luck2014) both running under the MATLAB environment (Mathworks). The data were recorded using the BrainVision Recorder (Brain Products) and were imported into EEGLAB using the bva-io plugin version 1.57 (Widmann & Delorme, Reference Widmann and Delorme2004). The EEG signal was re-referenced offline to the average of the left and right mastoid. A 1 Hz high-pass filter and a 50 Hz notch filter were applied, and a moving window (width: 500 ms; steps: 250 ms) identified segments with a signal exceeding ± 1.000 µV, which were removed. An independent component analysis (ICA) was performed on the continuous EEG signal in the experimental blocks, which was high-pass filtered at 1 Hz. The resulting ICA weights were applied to the continuous EEG signal, which was high-pass filtered at 0.1 Hz. Independent components capturing eye movements (blinks and saccades), muscle activity, or channel noise were removed from the data. Response-locked and baseline corrected epochs (baseline: −100 ms to response onset) starting 100 ms before the response and ending 700 ms after the response in the main task were extracted from the continuous signal and epochs in which participants exceeded the RT criterion were discarded, along with epochs in which the EEG signal exceeded ±150 µV. The epochs were averaged separately for each condition, and a current source density transformation was performed.Footnote 2 The Ne/c peak amplitude was defined as the most negative value at FCz from response onset to 150 ms after the response. Similarly, the Pe/c peak amplitude was defined as the most positive value at the Cz site from 150 to 300 ms after the response. The topographical plots confirmed these locations as the local extrema for the Ne/c and Pe/c (Figure 2), and all conditions met the reliability criterion of at least six trials per participant and condition (Olvet & Hajcak, Reference Olvet and Hajcak2009).

Figure 2. Topographical maps for errors and correct responses in an interval of 0–150 ms following the response (left) and 150–300 ms following the response (right) and grand-average waveforms for errors and correct responses. The error/correct negativity was quantified as the peak amplitude in the area marked grey at FCz (left), and the error/correct positivity was quantified as the peak amplitude in the area marked grey at Cz (right).

1.6. Behavioural variables

The RT was measured as the time difference between the stimulus onset and the point in time at which the first key was pressed with a force of at least 40 cN. Behavioural adaptation was assessed by the pre-post-RT difference RTpre-post and the post-response accuracy. The pre-post-RT difference is a robust estimate of post-error or post-correct slowing and is computed for a given trial n as follows (Dutilh et al., Reference Dutilh, van Ravenzwaaij, Nieuwenhuis, van der Maas, Forstmann and Wagenmakers2012):

$$ {\rm RT}_{\rm pre-post}={\rm RT}_{n+1}-{\rm RT}_{n-1} $$

The post-response accuracy was defined as the average correct response rate following correct responses or errors. (For additional exploratory analyses of response force and response certainty, see supplementary material.)

1.7. Signal detection

We computed two variables derived from the signal detection theory (Green & Swets, Reference Green and Swets1966). The bias c and sensitivity d’ parameters are computed as follows:

$$ c=-0.5\left[z\left({\rm hit\ rate}\right)+z\left({\rm false\ alarm\ rate}\right)\right] $$
$$ d'=z\left({\rm hit\ rate}\right)-z\left({\rm false\ alarm\ rate}\right) $$

where the function z(p), p ∈ [0,1], is the inverse of the cumulative Gaussian distribution. A “hit” is an error that was correctly identified as such. A “false alarm” is a correct response that was mistakenly evaluated as an error. Higher values in the bias indicate a tendency to evaluate one’s own responses as correct irrespective of the actual response accuracy. Higher values in the sensitivity indicate a better performance in correctly discriminating between correct and erroneous responses. One participant had a false alarm rate of zero; hence, no signal detection parameters could be computed for that participant, reducing the sample size for these analyses by one.

1.8. Statistical analyses

We analysed variables derived from signal detection theory, behavioural variables, post-response adaptation and electrophysiological variables.Footnote 3 Signal detection variables and the error rate were analysed in a linear regression model with PSP, ECP, and their interaction term as predictors. The other variables were analysed in a mixed-effects model with PSP, ECP, Response Accuracy (error vs. correct), and all possible interaction terms as predictors.Footnote 4 Participants were treated as random effects, and random intercepts were estimated. Since the RT was measured on the single trial level, random slopes were additionally estimated. Note that the only reason for this procedure was that RTs can relatively easily and reliably be assessed on the single trial level, whereas estimates of electrophysiological variables are commonly derived by averaging the trials to increase the signal-to-noise ratio. Signal detection variables are per definition assessed on the participant level. In all analyses, ECP and PSP were centred and used as continuous predictors. Response type was contrast-coded (correct = −0.5, error = 0.5). Standardised regression coefficients (β) were obtained by repeating the analyses with z-standardised variables. In case an interaction reached statistical significance, simple slope analyses were conducted to determine the values of the moderators PSP and ECP for which an effect of Response accuracy was observed. Given that we were primarily interested in error processing (as opposed to unsuccessful error processing, that is error trials that were not signalled as errors), we only included trials in our analyses in which the response accuracy was correctly signalled. This ensured that potentially different mechanisms were not conflated (Stahl et al., Reference Stahl, Mattes, Hundrieser, Kummer, Mück, Niessen and Dummel2020). The variance inflation factors for all predictors in all models ranged from 1.04 to 1.54, indicating that multicollinearity was not an issue in our analyses (Eid, Gollwitzer, & Schmitt, Reference Eid, Gollwitzer and Schmitt2013, p. 687).

The statistical analyses were conducted in R (R Core Team, 2017). The psychometric statistics were computed in the psych package (Revelle, Reference Revelle2020). Mixed-effects models were estimated using the lme4 package (Bates, Mächler, Bolker, & Walker, Reference Bates, Mächler, Bolker and Walker2015), and significance tests were conducted using the lmerTest package (Kuznetsova, Brockhoff, & Christensen, Reference Kuznetsova, Brockhoff and Christensen2017). To run simple slope analyses on two-way interactions and determine the Johnson-Neyman interval, we resorted to the interactions package (Long, Reference Long2019). Plots were produced with the ggplot2 package (Wickham, Reference Wickham2016). The variance inflation factors were computed using the performance package (Lüdecke, Ben-Shachar, Patil, Waggoner, & Makowski, Reference Lüdecke, Ben-Shachar, Patil, Waggoner and Makowski2021).

2. Results

Descriptive statistics are presented in Table 1. For the sake of brevity, we only report the statistically significant results in the text. The full analyses including results of predictors that were not statistically significant are displayed in Table 2 (signal detection variables) and Table 3 (behavioural and electrophysiological variables). The data and analyses scripts are available here: https://osf.io/a2jbq/

Table 1. Descriptive statistics

Mean (M) and standard deviation (SD) for all relevant variables split by response accuracy (error vs. correct).

a The mean of the total post-response accuracy is smaller than the means of the post-response accuracy for errors and correct responses. Nevertheless, for each individual participant, the total post-response accuracy lies between the post-response accuracy for errors and correct responses. See Kievit, Frankenhuis, Waldorp and Borsboom (Reference Kievit, Frankenhuis, Waldorp and Borsboom2013) on how patterns on the group level may differ from patterns on the participant level.

Table 2. Results of the regression analyses for the signal detection and electrophysiological variables

Regression coefficient (b), standard error (SE), standardised regression coefficient (β), and p-value (p) for the predictors in each model.

Table 3. Results of the mixed-effects model analyses for behavioural and electrophysiological variables

Regression coefficient (b), standard error (SE), standardised regression coefficient (β), and p-value (p) for the predictors in each model.

2.1. Signal detection variables

The regression model predicting the error rate by ECP, PSP, and their interaction term yielded a significant effect of ECP, b ± SE = 0.05 ± 0.02, t(86) = 3.48, p = .001. Higher levels of ECP were associated with higher error rates (Figure 3A).

Figure 3. Results regarding perfectionism dimensions. The dots indicate the observed values of PSP and ECP. The grey area in panels A and B represents the 95% confidence interval around the regression line. The grey area in panel C indicates the range of the moderator depicted on the x-axis for which the slope of Response Accuracy is significant. The colours in panel D indicate the value of the simple slope for Response Accuracy (“Acc. Effect”) for each combination of PSP and ECP. White represents insignificant slopes. More information on how panel D was generated is provided in the supplementary material.

For the bias, a significant ECP effect emerged, b = 0.11 ± 0.04, t(85) = 2.93, p = .004. The higher ECP was, the more participants tended to evaluate their responses as errors, independent from the actual response accuracy (Figure 3B).

2.2. Behavioural variables and post-response adaptation

The mixed-effects model for RT only yielded a significant main effect for response accuracy, b = 22.16 ± 7.87, t(81.56) = 2.82, p = .006. Errors (M ± SE: 794 ± 7.08 ms) were significantly slower than correct responses (772 ± 4.95 ms).

For the pre-post-RT difference, that is the more robust estimate of changes in post-RT, we found a significant effect of response accuracy, b = 24.60 ± 5.50, t(86) = 4.47, p < .001, and a response accuracy by ECP interaction, b = −11.88 ± 5.66, t(86) = −2.10, p = .039. The pre-post-RT difference was larger for errors (22.12 ± 4.24 ms) than correct responses (0.14 ± 3.37 ms), indicating more slowing following errors than following correct responses. However, this observation was restricted to participants with low- to-medium scores of ECP as indicated by the Johnson-Neyman interval [0.83, 35.81] (Figure 3C). The difference between errors and correct responses was only significant for ECP values outside this interval.

The regression model predicting post-response accuracy yielded a significant effect for PSP, b = 0.05 ± 0.02, t(86) = 2.23, p = .028 and a significant effect for ECP, b = −0.06 ± 0.02, t(86) = −3.69, p < .001. The higher the PSP was, the higher the probability of giving a correct response on the following trial was. For ECP, the relationship was reversed. Increasing ECP scores were associated with a decreasing probability of a correct response on the subsequent trial. Note, however, that both main effects may be explained by a generally higher response accuracy for PS perfectionists and a generally lower response accuracy for EC perfectionists.

2.3. Electrophysiological variables

The mixed-effects model for the Ne/c amplitude yielded a significant Response Accuracy main effect, b = −0.03 ± 0.01, t(86) = 3.83, p < .001, and a response accuracy by PSP-by-ECP interaction effect, b = 0.02 ± 0.01, t(86) = 2.05, p = .043. On average, the Ne/c amplitude was more pronounced after errors (0.13 ± 0.01 µV/cm2) than after correct responses (0.09 ± 0.01 µV/cm2). The results of a simple slope analysis to further investigate the three-way interaction are illustrated in Figure 3D. The difference in Ne/c amplitude between errors and correct responses was largest for non-perfectionists and mixed-perfectionists, pointing towards more error-specific activity in these perfectionism subtypes. A striking observation is that for pure-EC perfectionists, there was hardly a difference in the Ne/c amplitude, suggesting a lack of error-specific activity. Fitted waveforms for the different perfectionism subtypes are presented in Figure 4.

Figure 4. Fitted grand-average waveforms (derived from mixed-effects models) at FCz for errors and correct responses for pure-EC perfectionists (upper left), mixed perfectionists (upper right), non-perfectionists (lower left), and pure-PS perfectionists (lower right). For illustrative purposes, the waveforms were low-pass filtered at 5 Hz. To obtain the waveforms, a separate mixed-effects model with response accuracy, PSP, ECP, and all possible interactions was computed for each time step. Next, each mixed-effects model was used to predict the amplitude for all possible combinations of response accuracy (error and correct), high PSP or ECP (i.e. mean plus one standard deviation) and low PSP or ECP (mean minus one standard deviation), resulting in the presented waveforms. For more details on the computation of the waveforms and for the corresponding waveforms at the Cz electrode site, see supplementary material.

For the Pe/c amplitude, we only found a significant response accuracy effect, b = 0.08 ± 0.01, t(86) = 6.32, p < .001. Errors showed a higher Pe/c amplitude than correct responses (0.19 ± 0.02 µV/cm2 and 0.11 ± 0.02 µV/cm2, respectively). There were no perfectionism-related effects regarding the Pe/c amplitude. This is surprising because the fitted waveform in Figure 4 suggests that the Pe/c amplitude covaries with the two perfectionism dimensions. However, our analyses are based on the Pe/c amplitude at the electrode site Cz while Figure 4 displays the (fitted) waveforms at the electrode site FCz. For exploratory purposes, we repeated the analyses for the Pe/c amplitude measured at FCz. Note that although most scientific studies use the EEG signal at the Cz electrode to quantify the Pe/c, a notable body of literature investigates the Pe/c at the FCz site (e.g. Endrass, Reuter, & Kathmann, Reference Endrass, Reuter and Kathmann2007; Morris, Yee, & Nuechterlein, Reference Morris, Yee and Nuechterlein2006). The mixed-effects model yielded a significant main effect for response accuracy, b = 0.09 ± 0.01, t(86) = 7.43, p < .001, and PSP, b = −0.04 ± 0.02, t(86) = −2.00, p = .049, and a significant response accuracy by ECP interaction effect, b = −0.04 ± 0.01, t(86) = −3.22, p = .002. Errors showed a higher Pe/c amplitude than correct responses (0.17 ± 0.02 µV/cm2 and 0.08 ± 0.01 µV/cm2, respectively). However, this difference was only significant for participants with low- to-medium scores of ECP as indicated by the Johnson-Neyman interval [1.24, 5.97]. For ECP values outside this interval, the difference in Pe/c amplitude between correct responses and errors was significant. Furthermore, the higher PSP was, the smaller was the Pe/c amplitude regardless of whether the response was correct or erroneous.

In the supplementary material, we report additional analyses on the non-transformed (i.e. not CSD-transformed) data and analyses including the error rate and age as covariates.

3. Discussion

In the framework of the 2×2 model of perfectionism (Gaudreau & Thompson, Reference Gaudreau and Thompson2010), we investigated error processing in a task with increased response selection complexity. The variables of interest captured task performance, post-response adaptation, error detection and electrophysiological error processing indicators, which demonstrated differential processing of errors among within-person combinations of ECP and PSP.

Higher levels of ECP were associated with a higher error rate than lower levels of ECP, which indicates that high ECP is linked to worse task performance. Although ECP has been linked to worse performance in several domains such as worse academic performance (Madigan, Reference Madigan2019) or reduced goal achievement at the work place (Ocampo, Wang, Kiazad, Restubog, & Ashkanasy, Reference Ocampo, Wang, Kiazad, Restubog and Ashkanasy2020), no relationship with the error rate has been found in experimental tasks so far (e.g. Schrijvers et al., Reference Schrijvers, Bruijn, Destoop, Hulstijn and Sabbe2010; Stahl et al., Reference Stahl, Acharki, Kresimon, Völler and Gibbons2015; Tops et al., Reference Tops, Koole and Wijers2013). Unlike previous studies, we employed a task with a more complex response selection to avoid a potential ceiling effect in performance due to the simplicity of the task. It seems that the increased complexity of our task facilitated the detection of perfectionism-related differences in task performance. Further analyses provide several potential reasons for the observed relationship between ECP and the error rate. First, ECP was associated with less post-error slowing, a phenomenon that is linked to post-response adaptation (Laming, Reference Laming1968; Rabbitt, Reference Rabbitt1966). Post-error slowing has been hypothesised to emerge from an increased response caution following errors, allowing participants to collect more information before the response is initiated on the subsequent trial, hence preventing future errors. Other researchers have suggested that post-error slowing might reflect an orienting response aimed at identifying the source of the error (Houtman & Notebaert, Reference Houtman and Notebaert2013; Notebaert et al., Reference Notebaert, Houtman, van Opstal, Gevers, Fias and Verguts2009), which would also allow participants to avoid committing repeated errors (Wessel, Reference Wessel2018). Regardless of the specific mechanism of post-error slowing, it seems that high-EC perfectionists do not implement this post-trial correction mechanism as much as low-EC perfectionists, which could explain their poorer task performance. Second, early error-specific activity as indicated by the difference between the Ne and Nc peak amplitude was low for pure-EC perfectionists. In fact, for high levels of pure-ECP, the regression model predicted no error-specific activity at all, while mixed perfectionists and pure-PS perfectionists did display error-specific activity (Figure 3D). It seems that pure-EC perfectionists process errors less deeply and more similarly to correct responses compared to other perfectionism subtypes and may even lack error-specific processing altogether (Stahl et al., Reference Stahl, Acharki, Kresimon, Völler and Gibbons2015). Perhaps the combination of less error-specific activation in the early stages of error processing and less post-trial correction may have led to the poorer performance that was observable for high ECP.

We expected that high-PS perfectionists compared to low-PS perfectionists processed errors more deeply to optimise their behaviour. On a descriptive level, we found that high-PS perfectionists tended to perform better than low-PS perfectionists in terms of response accuracy. However, this effect was not statistically significant. Furthermore, we found that PSP moderated the relationship between ECP and early response monitoring as indicated by the Ne/c amplitude (for further discussion, see below). It is surprising that we did not find convincing evidence for the optimisation hypothesis of PSP considering numerous reports in the literature that highlight the adaptive aspect of PSP in response to errors. For instance, Stahl et al. (Reference Stahl, Acharki, Kresimon, Völler and Gibbons2015) found an increased post-error accuracy for high-PS perfectionists. Similarly, Barke et al. (Reference Barke, Bode, Dechent, Schmidt-Samoa, van Heer and Stahl2017) reported that PSP was associated with increased activity in brain areas that are related to response monitoring (ACC; Ridderinkhof et al., Reference Ridderinkhof, Ullsperger, Crone and Nieuwenhuis2004) and goal orientation and post-response adaptation (putamen; Hester et al. Reference Hester, Foxe, Molholm, Shpaner and Garavan2009; Linke et al., Reference Linke, Kirsch, King, Gass, Hennerici, Bongers and Wessa2010). Taken together, these findings suggest that high-PS perfectionists process errors more deeply allowing them to draw more information from their errors and consequently successfully adapt their subsequent behaviour. Our study could not contribute to this body of evidence.

Although our results regarding the optimisation hypothesis of PSP were not as convincing as in previous studies, we found clear evidence in favour of our hypothesis that ECP was associated with less deep error processing. However, it is still unclear whether this is due to avoidance of error processing or capacity limitations. Figure 5 illustrates both accounts and their predictions regarding the Ne/c amplitude. The avoidance hypothesis postulates that high-EC perfectionists avoid error processing because of the unpleasant consequences associated with errors. Hence, pure-EC perfectionists should show less error-specific activity in terms of the Ne/c amplitude than non-perfectionists. For both perfectionism subtypes, PSP is low. When PSP is high, the associated intrinsic motivation to perform flawlessly (Nordin-Bates, Raedeke, & Madigan, Reference Nordin-Bates, Raedeke and Madigan2017) might overwrite the fear of negative evaluation to a certain extent. Hence, error-specific activity in mixed perfectionists should be higher than in pure-EC perfectionists, but lower than in pure-PS perfectionists because pure-PS perfectionists have a high intrinsic motivation and no fear of negative evaluation. The capacity hypothesis postulates that high-EC perfectionists have a limited cognitive capacity because much of their capacity is captured by error-related contents or – more generally – worrying (Moser, Moran, Schroder, Donnellan, & Yeung, Reference Moser, Moran, Schroder, Donnellan and Yeung2013; Moser, Reference Moser2017). As a consequence, pure-EC perfectionists process errors less deeply than non-perfectionists, resulting in less error-specific activity in terms of the Ne/c amplitude. Furthermore, the account predicts that because of their high internal motivation, mixed perfectionists may be able to allocate additional resources to error processing, compensating for the reduced capacities. This compensatory effort in mixed perfectionists may produce more error-specific activity than both in pure-EC perfectionists (who lack internal motivation) and in pure-PS perfectionists (who have sufficient processing capacities and thus do not show a compensatory effort) because greater resources need to be allocated to error processing to make up for the capacities that are captured by worrying (Moser, Reference Moser2017). To sum up, both accounts converge in their predictions regarding the pattern of error-specific activity for pure-EC perfectionists. However, they differ in their predictions for mixed perfectionists (intensity of error processing for the avoidance hypothesis: pure-PSP > mixed perfectionism > pure-ECP; for the capacity hypothesis: mixed perfectionism > pure-PSP > pure-ECP). Hence, the comparison of the error-specific activity pattern between mixed and pure-PS perfectionists allows to draw conclusions regarding the underlying mechanism of ECP-related error processing.

Figure 5. Illustration of the avoidance hypothesis (upper panel) and the capacity hypothesis (lower panel). The avoidance hypothesis postulates that when ECP is high and personal standards perfectionism (PSP) is low (i.e. pure-EC perfectionists; upper left panel), error processing is avoided altogether. When PSP is high (i.e. mixed perfectionists; upper right panel), error processing might be attenuated or not impacted at all. The capacity hypothesis claims that when ECP is high and PSP is low (lower left panel), pure-EC perfectionists dispose of less capacities that could be allocated to error processing because parts of the cognitive capacities are captured by worrying. Furthermore, they are not intrinsically motivated to perform well. Hence, error processing is diminished. When ECP is high and PSP is also high (lower right panel), mixed perfectionists also dispose of less capacities. However, because they are intrinsically motivated to perform well, they reallocate resources to error processing in a compensatory effort, resulting in intensive error processing.

As Figure 3D shows, our results regarding the Ne/c amplitude support the capacity hypothesis. The difference in the Ne/c amplitude between errors and correct responses was larger for mixed perfectionists than for pure-PS perfectionists. Furthermore, for pure-EC perfectionists, this accuracy effect was very small or even insignificant. This pattern replicates findings by Stahl et al. (Reference Stahl, Acharki, Kresimon, Völler and Gibbons2015). Apart from the Ne/c amplitude results, our data provide even more evidence in favour of the capacity hypothesis. ECP was associated with a bias to declare the given response as an error. Note that despite this bias, ECP was not associated with a more accurate classification of the given response. Hence, the declaration of the response as an error was independent of the actual response accuracy and might reflect a preoccupation of high-EC perfectionists with errors. This preoccupation may be a marker of the limited cognitive capacities due to error-related contents and worrying as suggested by the capacity hypothesis (Figure 5). The avoidance hypothesis, however, would not predict a preoccupation with errors of EC perfections because they should avoid error processing altogether. Our data demonstrate that EC perfectionists are very much concerned with errors. Finally, we found that the difference in the Pe/c amplitude between errors and correct responses at the FCz electrode site was restricted to low- to-medium levels of ECP and disappeared for high levels of ECP. The Pe/c has been associated with error awareness (Nieuwenhuis et al., Reference Nieuwenhuis, Ridderinkhof, Blom, Band and Kok2001) and error evidence accumulation (Steinhauser & Yeung, Reference Steinhauser and Yeung2010, Reference Steinhauser and Yeung2012). The fact that – for high levels of ECP – the Pe/c peaked just as high for correct responses as for errors (Figure 4) may indicate that high-EC perfectionists tended to interpret correct responses as errors (or accumulate evidence for errors when responses were actually correct). This electrophysiological mechanism may have translated to a bias in high-EC perfectionists to declare correct responses as errors on the behavioural level.

3.1. Limitations and future research

Although our data lend more support for the capacity hypothesis of ECP than for the avoidance hypothesis, our study is not suitable to falsify the avoidance hypothesis. The primary purpose of our study was to explore perfectionism-related variations in error processing in a task with increased response selection complexity. While we were able to interpret our findings in the context of the avoidance, capacity, and optimisation hypotheses, these interpretations were post hoc. Furthermore, we did not employ an adjustment for multiple comparisons due to the exploratory nature of our study. Individual effects should thus be interpreted with caution. While our sample size was large enough to detect medium effects with an adequate probability, small effects may not have been detected. Our study provides standardised effect sizes that may serve as a basis for a priori sample size calculations in future studies. Most importantly, however, future studies could be designed more specifically to test either one of the hypotheses or reconcile them in an overarching account. For example, future studies may employ the presence vs. absence of a detection rating to specifically test the avoidance hypothesis: If high-EC perfectionists avoid error processing, they should do so when no detection rating is required, but they should fail to do so when they are forced to rate the accuracy of their responses (Drizinsky et al., Reference Drizinsky, Zülch, Gibbons and Stahl2016).

It should further be noted that in our study, errors were slower than correct responses, replicating findings by Stahl et al. (Reference Stahl, Mattes, Hundrieser, Kummer, Mück, Niessen and Dummel2020) and Porth, Mattes and Stahl (Reference Porth, Mattes and Stahl2022) who used the same experimental task in different samples. Other studies using a simpler task reported faster errors compared to correct responses (e.g. Stahl et al., Reference Stahl, Acharki, Kresimon, Völler and Gibbons2015). This observation suggests that some of the errors in our task may originate from a different mechanism than the errors in previous studies. For example, fast errors are often assumed to be the result of an impulsive, premature response (Novikov et al., Reference Novikov, Nurislamova, Zhozhikashvili, Kalenkovich, Lapina and Chernyshev2017). Slow errors may emerge on trials on which the correct response is not identified but an arbitrary response is initiated nonetheless to avoid exceeding the time limit (Murphy, Boonstra, & Nieuwenhuis, Reference Murphy, Boonstra and Nieuwenhuis2016; Stahl et al., Reference Stahl, Mattes, Hundrieser, Kummer, Mück, Niessen and Dummel2020). This mechanism is more likely to be relevant in tasks with an increased response selection complexity like in our study (Stahl et al., Reference Stahl, Mattes, Hundrieser, Kummer, Mück, Niessen and Dummel2020). We cannot rule out that our findings are restricted to this type of error, although Stahl et al. (Reference Stahl, Mattes, Hundrieser, Kummer, Mück, Niessen and Dummel2020) showed that this task provokes both fast and slow error types. Future studies may contrast a simpler and a more complex task within-participants to explore whether these potential qualitative differences in errors are linked to different dimensions/subtypes of perfectionism.

Finally, a recent debate has called into question whether variables capturing individual differences are better explained by a small set of basic personality traits like the Big Five (Bainbridge et al., Reference Bainbridge, Ludeke and Smillie2022). While PSP and ECP are related to conscientiousness and neuroticism, respectively (Stoeber et al., Reference Stoeber, Damian, Madigan and Stoeber2018; Stricker et al., Reference Stricker, Buecker, Schneider and Preckel2019), researchers have stressed that the concepts are qualitatively different. Furthermore, perfectionism has been shown to play an important conceptual role in a series of psychological disorders (Limburg et al., Reference Limburg, Watson, Hagger and Egan2017), perhaps above and beyond the Big Five. Hence, studying perfectionism-related variations in cognitive mechanisms has a large potential to contribute to understanding these disorders better and developing adequate treatments for them.

4. Conclusion

Employing a more challenging task compared to simpler tasks has proven helpful in exploring differential mechanisms of response monitoring associated with within-subject PSP-by-ECP combinations. Future studies on perfectionism-related variations in error processing may benefit from increasing the response selection complexity of their experimental tasks to avoid potential ceiling effects. Our study contributes to developing a testable model of error processing in perfectionists.

Supplementary materials

For supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/pen.2022.3

Footnotes

This research was supported by the German Research Foundation (STA 1035/7-1, awarded to Jutta Stahl).

1 The term “subtype” is merely used to describe more easily the location of a person on the two-dimensional plane that results from the within-person combinations of ECP and PSP.

2 Analyses of the non-transformed data are presented in the supplementary material.

3 Results of the analyses of response force and response certainty are provided in the supplementary material.

4 Additional analyses including the error rate and age as covariates are presented in the supplementary material.

References

Altstötter-Gleich, C., & Bergemann, N. (2006). Testgüte einer deutschsprachigen Version der Mehrdimensionalen Perfektionismus Skala von Frost, Marten, Lahart und Rosenblate (MPS-F). Diagnostica, 52, 105118. https://doi.org/10.1026/0012-1924.52.3.105 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bainbridge, T. F., Ludeke, S. G., & Smillie, L. D. (2022). Evaluating the Big Five as an organizing framework for commonly used psychological trait scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 122, 749777. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000395 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barke, A., Bode, S., Dechent, P., Schmidt-Samoa, C., van Heer, C., & Stahl, J. (2017). To err is (perfectly) human: Behavioural and neural correlates of error processing and perfectionism. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12, 16471657. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsx082 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 148. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cavanagh, J. F., & Frank, M. J. (2014). Frontal theta as a mechanism for cognitive control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 414421. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Damian, L. E., Stoeber, J., Negru, O., & Băban, A. (2013). On the development of perfectionism in adolescence: Perceived parental expectations predict longitudinal increases in socially prescribed perfectionism. Personality and Individual Differences, 55, 688693. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.05.021 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Debener, S., Ullsperger, M., Siegel, M., Fiehler, K., Cramon, D. Y.von, & Engel, A. K. (2005). Trial-by-trial coupling of concurrent electroencephalogram and functional magnetic resonance imaging identifies the dynamics of performance monitoring. The Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 1173011737. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3286-05.2005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delorme, A., & Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: An open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 134, 921. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Drizinsky, J., Zülch, J., Gibbons, H., & Stahl, J. (2016). How personal standards perfectionism and evaluative concerns perfectionism affect the error positivity and post-error behavior with varying stimulus visibility. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 16, 876887. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-016-0438-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dutilh, G., van Ravenzwaaij, D., Nieuwenhuis, S., van der Maas, H. L. J., Forstmann, B. U., & Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2012). How to measure post-error slowing: A confound and a simple solution. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 56, 208216. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jmp.2012.04.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eid, M., Gollwitzer, M., & Schmitt, M. (2013). Statistik und Forschungsmethoden (3rd ed.). Weinheim, Germany: Beltz Google Scholar
Elson, M., & Bente, G. (2009). CORTEX - COmputer-aided Registration Tool for EXperiments. University of Cologne. Retrieved from http://cortex.uni-koeln.de/ Google Scholar
Endrass, T., Reuter, B., & Kathmann, N. (2007). Erp correlates of conscious error recognition: Aware and unaware errors in an antisaccade task. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 17141720. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05785.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion, 7, 336353. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.336 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Falkenstein, M., Hohnsbein, J., Hoormann, J., & Blanke, L. (1991). Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components. II. Error processing in choice reaction tasks. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 78, 447455. https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(91)90062-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flett, G. L., Hewitt, P. L., Blankstein, K. R., & Gray, L. (1998). Psychological distress and the frequency of perfectionistic thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 13631381. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.75.5.1363 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flett, G. L., Madorsky, D., Hewitt, P. L., & Heisel, M. J. (2002). Perfectionism cognitions, rumination, and psychological distress. Journal of Rational-Emotive and Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 20, 3347. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1015128904007 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frost, R. O., Heimberg, R. G., Holt, C. S., Mattia, J. I., & Neubauer, A. L. (1993). A comparison of two measures of perfectionism. Personality and Individual Differences, 14, 119126. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(93)90181-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frost, R. O., Marten, P., Lahart, C., & Rosenblate, R. (1990). The dimensions of perfectionism. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14, 449468. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01172967 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaudreau, P. (2013). The 2×2 model of perfectionism: Commenting the critical comments and suggestions of Stoeber (2012). Personality and Individual Differences, 55, 351355. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.03.021 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaudreau, P., & Thompson, A. (2010). Testing a 2×2 model of dispositional perfectionism. Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 532537. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.11.031 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gehring, W. J., Coles, M. G. H., Meyer, D. E., & Donchin, E. (1990). The error-related negativity: An event-related brain potential accompanying errors. Psychophysiology, 27(s4a), 34.Google Scholar
Gehring, W. J., Goss, B., Coles, M. G. H., Meyer, D. E., & Donchin, E. (1993). A neural system for error detection and compensation. Psychological Science, 4, 385390. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1993.tb00586.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gnilka, P. B., Ashby, J. S., & Noble, C. M. (2012). Multidimensional perfectionism and anxiety: Differences among individuals with perfectionism and tests of a coping-mediation model. Journal of Counseling & Development, 90, 427436. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1556-6676.2012.00054.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, D. M., & Swets, J. A. (1966). Signal detection theory and psychophysics. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Hester, R., Foxe, J. J., Molholm, S., Shpaner, M., Garavan, H. (2005). Neural mechanisms involved in error processing: A comparison of errors made with and without awareness. NeuroImage, 27, 602608. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.04.035 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hester, R., Madeley, J., Murphy, K., & Mattingley, J. B. (2009). Learning from errors: Error-related neural activity predicts improvements in future inhibitory control performance. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 71587165. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4337-08.2009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hill, A. P., Mallinson-Howard, S. H., & Jowett, G. E. (2018). Multidimensional perfectionism in sport: A meta-analytical review. Sport, Exercise, and Performance Psychology, 7, 235270. https://doi.org/10.1037/spy0000125 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Houtman, F., & Notebaert, W. (2013). Blinded by an error. Cognition, 128, 228236. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.04.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jasper, H. (1958). The ten-twenty electrode system of the International Federation. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 10, 370375. https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(58)90053-1 Google Scholar
Kievit, R. A., Frankenhuis, W. E., Waldorp, L. J., & Borsboom, D. (2013). Simpson’s paradox in psychological science: A practical guide. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 513. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00513 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, L. E., Chen, L., MacCann, C., Karlov, L., & Kleitman, S. (2015). Evidence for three factors of perfectionism: Perfectionistic strivings, order, and perfectionistic concerns. Personality and Individual Differences, 84, 1622. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.01.033 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
King, J. A., Korb, F. M., Cramon, D. Y.von, & Ullsperger, M. (2010). Post-error behavioral adjustments are facilitated by activation and suppression of task-relevant and task-irrelevant information processing. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 1275912769. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3274-10.2010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2017). lmerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82, 126. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v082.i13 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laming, D. (1968). Information theory of choice reaction times. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Limburg, K., Watson, H. J., Hagger, M. S., & Egan, S. J. (2017). The relationship between perfectionism and psychopathology: A meta-analysis. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 73, 13011326. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.22435 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linke, J., Kirsch, P., King, A. V., Gass, A., Hennerici, M. G., Bongers, A., & Wessa, M. (2010). Motivational orientation modulates the neural response to reward. NeuroImage, 49, 26182625. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.09.013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Long, J. A. (2019). Interactions: Comprehensive, user-friendly toolkit for probing interactions. Retrieved from https://cran.r-project.org/package=interactions Google Scholar
Lopez-Calderon, J., & Luck, S. J. (2014). Erplab: An open-source toolbox for the analysis of event-related potentials. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 213. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00213 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lüdecke, D., Ben-Shachar, M., Patil, I., Waggoner, P., & Makowski, D. (2021). performance: An R package for assessment, comparison and testing of statistical models. Journal of Open Source Software, 6, 3139. https://doi.org/10.21105/joss.03139 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Madigan, D. J. (2019). A meta-analysis of perfectionism and academic achievement. Educational Psychology Review, 28, 239989. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-019-09484-2 Google Scholar
Maier, M. E., Yeung, N., & Steinhauser, M. (2011). Error-related brain activity and adjustments of selective attention following errors. NeuroImage, 56, 23392347. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.03.083 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mattes, A., Porth, E., & Stahl, J. (2022). Linking neurophysiological processes of action monitoring to post-response speed-accuracy adjustments in a neuro-cognitive diffusion model. NeuroImage, 247, Article 118798. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118798 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, S. E., Yee, C. M., & Nuechterlein, K. H. (2006). Electrophysiological analysis of error monitoring in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115, 239250. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.115.2.239 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moser, J. S. (2017). The nature of the relationship between anxiety and the error-related negativity across development. Current Behavioral Neuroscience Reports, 4, 309321. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40473-017-0132-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moser, J. S., Moran, T. P., Schroder, H. S., Donnellan, M. B., & Yeung, N. (2013). On the relationship between anxiety and error monitoring: A meta-analysis and conceptual framework. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 466. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00466 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, P. R., Boonstra, E., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2016). Global gain modulation generates time-dependent urgency during perceptual choice in humans. Nature Communications, 7, 13526. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms13526 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nieuwenhuis, S., Ridderinkhof, K. R., Blom, J., Band, G. P. H., & Kok, A. (2001). Error-related brain potentials are differentially related to awareness of response errors: Evidence from an antisaccade task. Psychophysiology, 38, 752760. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-8986.3850752 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nordin-Bates, S. M., Raedeke, T. D., & Madigan, D. J. (2017). Perfectionism, burnout, and motivation in dance: A replication and test of the 2×2 model of perfectionism. Journal of Dance Medicine & Science, 21, 115122. https://doi.org/10.12678/1089-313X.21.3.115 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Notebaert, W., Houtman, F., van Opstal, F., Gevers, W., Fias, W., & Verguts, T. (2009). Post-error slowing: An orienting account. Cognition, 111, 275279. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.02.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Novikov, N. A., Nurislamova, Y. M., Zhozhikashvili, N. A., Kalenkovich, E. E., Lapina, A. A., & Chernyshev, B. V. (2017). Slow and fast responses: Two mechanisms of trial outcome processing revealed by EEG oscillations. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11, 218. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00218 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ocampo, A. C. G., Wang, L., Kiazad, K., Restubog, S. L. D., & Ashkanasy, N. M. (2020). The relentless pursuit of perfectionism: A review of perfectionism in the workplace and an agenda for future research. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 41, 144168. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.2400 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olvet, D. M., & Hajcak, G. (2009). The stability of error-related brain activity with increasing trials. Psychophysiology, 46, 957961. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00848.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pieters, G. L. M., Bruijn, E. R. A.de, Maas, Y., Hulstijn, W., Vandereycken, W., Peuskens, J., & Sabbe, B. G. (2007). Action monitoring and perfectionism in anorexia nervosa. Brain and Cognition, 63, 4250. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2006.07.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Porth, E., Mattes, A., & Stahl, J. (2022). The influence of error detection and error significance on neural and behavioral correlates of error processing in a complex choice task. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 103, 357. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-022-01028-6 Google Scholar
Rabbitt, P. M. (1966). Errors and error correction in choice-response tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 71, 264272. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0022853 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team. (2017). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from https://www.R-project.org/ Google Scholar
Revelle, W. (2020). psych: Procedures for psychological, psychometric, and personality research. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psych Google Scholar
Rice, K. G., & Aldea, M. A. (2006). State dependence and trait stability of perfectionism: A short-term longitudinal study. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 53, 205213. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.53.2.205 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rice, K. G., Lopez, F. G., & Vergara, D. (2005). Parental/social influences on perfectionism and adult attachment orientations. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 24, 580605. https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2005.24.4.580 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ridderinkhof, K. R., Ullsperger, M., Crone, E. A., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2004). The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science, 306, 443447. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1100301 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schrijvers, D. L., Bruijn, E. R. A.de, Destoop, M., Hulstijn, W., & Sabbe, B. G. C. (2010). The impact of perfectionism and anxiety traits on action monitoring in major depressive disorder. Journal of Neural Transmission, 117, 869880. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00702-010-0419-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shafran, R., Cooper, Z., & Fairburn, C. G. (2002). Clinical perfectionism: A cognitive-behavioural analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 773791. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(01)00059-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slade, P. D., & Owens, R. G. (1998). A dual process model of perfectionism based on reinforcement theory. Behavior Modification, 22, 372390. https://doi.org/10.1177/01454455980223010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stahl, J., Acharki, M., Kresimon, M., Völler, F., & Gibbons, H. (2015). Perfect error processing: Perfectionism-related variations in action monitoring and error processing mechanisms. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 97, 153162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.06.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stahl, J., Mattes, A., Hundrieser, M., Kummer, K., Mück, M., Niessen, E., …Dummel, S. (2020). Neural correlates of error detection during complex response selection: Introduction of a novel eight-alternative response task. Biological Psychology, 156, 107969. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2020.107969 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steinhauser, M., & Yeung, N. (2010). Decision processes in human performance monitoring. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 1564315653. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1899-10.2010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steinhauser, M., & Yeung, N. (2012). Error awareness as evidence accumulation: Effects of speed-accuracy trade-off on error signaling. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 240. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00240 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoeber, J. (2018). The psychology of perfectionism: An introduction. In Stoeber, J. (Ed.), The psychology of perfectionism: Theory, research, applications (pp. 316). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Stoeber, J., Damian, L. E., & Madigan, D. J. (2018). Perfectionism: A motivational perspective. In Stoeber, J. (Ed.), The psychology of perfectionism: Theory, research, applications (pp. 1943). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Stoeber, J., & Otto, K. (2006). Positive conceptions of perfectionism: Approaches, evidence, challenges. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 295319. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr1004_2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stricker, J., Buecker, S., Schneider, M., & Preckel, F. (2019). Multidimensional perfectionism and the Big Five personality traits: A meta-analysis. European Journal of Personality, 33, 176196. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2186 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tops, M., Koole, S. L., & Wijers, A. A. (2013). The Pe of perfectionism. Journal of Psychophysiology, 27, 8494. https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000090 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wessel, J. R. (2012). Error awareness and the error-related negativity: Evaluating the first decade of evidence. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 88. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00088 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wessel, J. R. (2018). An adaptive orienting theory of error processing. Psychophysiology, 55, e13041. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13041 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wickham, H. (2016). ggplot2: Elegant graphics for data analysis. Use R!. New York: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-24277-4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Widmann, A., & Delorme, A. (2004). bva–io. Retrieved from https://github.com/arnodelorme/bva-io Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. (A) Stimulus-response assignment and (B) trial course. In the example, a percentage sign which is assigned to the right middle finger is the target, so pressing the button with the right middle finger would be the correct response.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Topographical maps for errors and correct responses in an interval of 0–150 ms following the response (left) and 150–300 ms following the response (right) and grand-average waveforms for errors and correct responses. The error/correct negativity was quantified as the peak amplitude in the area marked grey at FCz (left), and the error/correct positivity was quantified as the peak amplitude in the area marked grey at Cz (right).

Figure 2

Table 1. Descriptive statistics

Figure 3

Table 2. Results of the regression analyses for the signal detection and electrophysiological variables

Figure 4

Table 3. Results of the mixed-effects model analyses for behavioural and electrophysiological variables

Figure 5

Figure 3. Results regarding perfectionism dimensions. The dots indicate the observed values of PSP and ECP. The grey area in panels A and B represents the 95% confidence interval around the regression line. The grey area in panel C indicates the range of the moderator depicted on the x-axis for which the slope of Response Accuracy is significant. The colours in panel D indicate the value of the simple slope for Response Accuracy (“Acc. Effect”) for each combination of PSP and ECP. White represents insignificant slopes. More information on how panel D was generated is provided in the supplementary material.

Figure 6

Figure 4. Fitted grand-average waveforms (derived from mixed-effects models) at FCz for errors and correct responses for pure-EC perfectionists (upper left), mixed perfectionists (upper right), non-perfectionists (lower left), and pure-PS perfectionists (lower right). For illustrative purposes, the waveforms were low-pass filtered at 5 Hz. To obtain the waveforms, a separate mixed-effects model with response accuracy, PSP, ECP, and all possible interactions was computed for each time step. Next, each mixed-effects model was used to predict the amplitude for all possible combinations of response accuracy (error and correct), high PSP or ECP (i.e. mean plus one standard deviation) and low PSP or ECP (mean minus one standard deviation), resulting in the presented waveforms. For more details on the computation of the waveforms and for the corresponding waveforms at the Cz electrode site, see supplementary material.

Figure 7

Figure 5. Illustration of the avoidance hypothesis (upper panel) and the capacity hypothesis (lower panel). The avoidance hypothesis postulates that when ECP is high and personal standards perfectionism (PSP) is low (i.e. pure-EC perfectionists; upper left panel), error processing is avoided altogether. When PSP is high (i.e. mixed perfectionists; upper right panel), error processing might be attenuated or not impacted at all. The capacity hypothesis claims that when ECP is high and PSP is low (lower left panel), pure-EC perfectionists dispose of less capacities that could be allocated to error processing because parts of the cognitive capacities are captured by worrying. Furthermore, they are not intrinsically motivated to perform well. Hence, error processing is diminished. When ECP is high and PSP is also high (lower right panel), mixed perfectionists also dispose of less capacities. However, because they are intrinsically motivated to perform well, they reallocate resources to error processing in a compensatory effort, resulting in intensive error processing.

Supplementary material: PDF

Mattes et al. supplementary material

Mattes et al. supplementary material

Download Mattes et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 924.9 KB