Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T05:05:29.599Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Bereavement guilt among young adults impacted by caregivers’ cancer: Associations with attachment style, experiential avoidance, and psychological flexibility

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 September 2024

Madeleine Stone*
Affiliation:
Centre for Emotional Health, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW, Australia Canteen, Sydney, NSW, Australia
Fiona E. J. McDonald
Affiliation:
Canteen, Sydney, NSW, Australia Faculty of Medicine and Health, The University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia
Maria Kangas
Affiliation:
Centre for Emotional Health, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW, Australia
Kerry Sherman
Affiliation:
Centre for Emotional Health, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW, Australia
Kimberley R. Allison
Affiliation:
Canteen, Sydney, NSW, Australia
*
Corresponding author: Fiona E. J. McDonald; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objectives

This paper focuses on the experiences of bereavement guilt among young adults bereaved by a caregiver’s cancer, examining associations with attachment style, experiential avoidance, and psychological flexibility with the aim of informing psychosocial interventions for this population.

Methods

Ninety-seven young adults (18–25 years) bereaved by a parent/guardian’s cancer completed an online survey, including measures of bereavement guilt, attachment style, experiential avoidance, and psychological flexibility. Mediation analyses explored the associations between attachment style (anxious, avoidant) and bereavement guilt, and if these associations were mediated by experiential avoidance or psychological flexibility.

Results

Bereavement guilt was significantly positively associated with anxious, but not avoidant, attachment to the deceased; the relationship between anxious attachment and bereavement guilt was partially mediated by experiential avoidance. Bereavement guilt was also negatively associated with psychological flexibility and engagement with bereavement counseling.

Significance of results

Given the limited literature on cancer-related bereavement in young adulthood, this study offers important theoretical and clinical insights into factors associated with more complex aspects of grief in this population. Specifically, this work identified that anxious attachment is associated with ongoing bereavement complications in the years following the death of a caregiver to cancer, with experiential avoidance partially mediating this relationship. While further research is needed to better understand the interaction between these factors and other related constructs, such as psychological flexibility, these findings may be helpful in selecting therapeutic approaches to use with this population.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - SA
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the same Creative Commons licence is used to distribute the re-used or adapted article and the original article is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained prior to any commercial use.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press.

Introduction

Cancer is a leading cause of death in developed countries (Sung et al. Reference Sung, Ferlay and Siegel2021), with annual cancer deaths projected to reach 17 million worldwide by 2030 (Boyle and Levin Reference Boyle and Levin2008). Bereaved relatives typically experience physical, emotional, and cognitive responses to the death, known as grief (Zisook and Shear Reference Zisook and Shear2009). This often involves distress (Kim et al. Reference Kim, Lucette and Loscalzo2013; Oechsle et al. Reference Oechsle, Ullrich and Marx2020) and guilt (bereavement guilt; Joa and Newberg Reference Joa and Newberg2023; Li et al. Reference Li, Stroebe and Chan2014); some bereaved relatives also experience prolonged and complicated grief reactions (Zordan et al. Reference Zordan, Bell and Price2019). For young adults (18–25 years) bereaved by a caregiver’s cancer, the co-occurrence of bereavement with developmental shifts (e.g. moving out, educational/career progression) present unique challenges, particularly if coping skills and flexibility are still evolving (Asselmann et al. Reference Asselmann, Wittchen and Lieb2015; Ing et al. Reference Ing, Patterson and Szabo2019; Karlsson et al. Reference Karlsson, Andersson and Ahlström2013). However, the experiences and needs of this population remain relatively understudied (Herberman Mash et al. Reference Herberman Mash, Fullerton and Shear2014; Kim et al. Reference Kim, Lucette and Loscalzo2013), and potential intervention strategies and programs are similarly lacking (Ing et al. Reference Ing, Patterson and Szabo2019). This paper focuses on the experiences of bereavement guilt among young adults bereaved by caregivers’ cancer, examining associations with attachment style, experiential avoidance, and psychological flexibility with the aim of informing psychosocial interventions for this population.

Young people’s experiences of bereavement

The death of a close relative is one of the most traumatic events a young person can experience (Stoppelbein and Greening Reference Stoppelbein and Greening2000); cancer-related deaths may carry additional psychosocial burden from witnessing a potentially protracted and traumatic period of illness and decline, and associated anticipatory grief (Hoffmann et al. Reference Hoffmann, Kaiser and Kersting2018; Zordan et al. Reference Zordan, Bell and Price2019). This is complicated by disrupted family communication and dynamics before and after bereavement, with families reporting uncertainty about if and how to discuss impending death, leaving young people unaware of or not expecting the death and still reconstructing the narrative of illness and loss years later (Lundberg et al. Reference Lundberg, Forinder and Olsson2018; Semple et al. Reference Semple, McCaughan and Beck2021; Varathakeyan et al. Reference Varathakeyan, McDonald and Patterson2017). While some studies evidence that young people bereaved by parental cancer are resilient and adjust to their loss (Angelhoff et al. Reference Angelhoff, Sveen and Alvariza2021; Hoffmann et al. Reference Hoffmann, Kaiser and Kersting2018), other work has indicated poor family cohesion (Birgisdóttir et al. Reference Birgisdóttir, Bylund-Grenklo and Kreicbergs2019), higher rates of self-injury (Bylund-Grenklo et al. Reference Bylund-Grenklo, Kreicbergs and Hauksdóttir2013), poorer self-esteem, depression, anxiety, and persistent unresolved grief (Bylund-Grenklo et al. Reference Bylund-Grenklo, Fürst and Nyberg2016; Lundberg et al. Reference Lundberg, Forinder and Olsson2018) following bereavement. Bereaved offspring report unmet needs for support and understanding from friends, family, educators, and those with similar experiences; help coping with feelings; time out or distraction; space and time to grieve; and help managing changing household responsibilities (McDonald et al. Reference McDonald, Patterson and Tindle2020; Patterson and Rangganadhan Reference Patterson and Rangganadhan2010). These unmet needs are associated with greater distress (McDonald et al. Reference McDonald, Patterson and Tindle2020). The psychosocial vulnerabilities of bereaved young people suggest a need for further research on factors associated with adjustment to bereavement to inform support services.

Grief and bereavement

The dual process model proposes that following bereavement, individuals oscillate between loss-oriented and restoration-oriented coping strategies (Stroebe and Schut Reference Stroebe and Schut1999). Loss-oriented strategies focus on thoughts and feelings about the loss, while restoration-oriented coping describes the process of adaptation to the death, including stressors such as role and identity changes (Fiore Reference Fiore2021). Oscillation between loss- and restoration-oriented states is thought to facilitate a more adaptive grieving experience; by contrast, complicated grief symptoms (including prolonged grief disorder and post-traumatic stress disorder) arise when individuals do not oscillate between states, or when this oscillation is disturbed (Burton et al. Reference Burton, Yan and Pat-Horenczyk2012; Stroebe et al. Reference Stroebe, Schut and Stroebe2005). The ability to oscillate between approaches can be understood in terms of psychological flexibility, which allows individuals to adjust their cognitions and behaviors to adapt to situational demands (Kashdan and Rottenberg Reference Kashdan and Rottenberg2010; Landstra et al. Reference Landstra, Ciarrochi and Deane2013). It may also be affected by factors relating to the bereaved person’s attachment and coping responses.

Bereavement guilt is defined as “a remorseful emotional reaction in grieving, with the recognition of having failed to live up to one’s own inner standards and expectations in relationship to the deceased and/or the death” (Li et al. Reference Li, Stroebe and Chan2014, p. 166). Guilt may center on the inability to prevent the death (responsibility guilt), hurting the deceased or their relationship, or living or enjoying life post-bereavement (survivor guilt; (Li et al. Reference Li, Tendeiro and Stroebe2019). Bereavement guilt has been estimated to affect anywhere between 7 and 90% of bereaved individuals; most studies estimating prevalence between 30 and 60% (Stroebe et al. Reference Stroebe, Stroebe and van de Shoot2014). These feelings are hypothesized to interfere with the grieving process by distracting the bereaved individual from confronting the reality of their loss (i.e. experiential avoidance; Camacho et al. Reference Camacho, Pérez-Nieto and Gordillo2018; Eisma et al. Reference Eisma, Stroebe and Schut2013), or being difficult to resolve, particularly if guilt relates to a transgression against the deceased (Camacho et al. Reference Camacho, Pérez-Nieto and Gordillo2018; LeBlanc et al. Reference LeBlanc, Toner and O’Day2020).

Accordingly, bereavement guilt is associated with complicated grief and poorer adjustment to loss (LeBlanc et al. Reference LeBlanc, Toner and O’Day2020; Li et al. Reference Li, Stroebe and Chan2014, Reference Li, Tendeiro and Stroebe2019; Stroebe et al. Reference Stroebe, Stroebe and van de Shoot2014). While previous research was not specific to young people or cancer-related bereavements, psychosocial workers supporting young people bereaved by parental cancer at Canteen Australia (a community support organization for young people impacted by cancer) have observed that bereavement guilt appeared associated with more complicated grief responses, inhibited processing of the loss, and slower therapeutic progress. It is therefore important to examine factors related to bereavement guilt to better understand this response and guide therapeutic interventions for this population.

Attachment

Bowlby (Reference Bowlby1969) and Ainsworth (Reference Ainsworth1989) define attachment as the deep, long-term connection between a child and their parent/guardian, which guides their beliefs and subsequent relationship formation. Attachment style (e.g. secure, avoidant, anxious) is proposed to shape individuals’ response to bereavement, particularly regarding the loss of their attachment figure. For example, avoidant attachment is characterized by a lack of trust in others, compulsive independence, and tendency to avoid attachment-related emotions; following bereavement, these individuals are likely to suppress grief in favor of restoration-oriented actions (Stroebe et al. Reference Stroebe, Schut and Stroebe2005). By contrast, anxiously attached individuals experience difficulty coping with attachment-related feelings, and tend to be loss-oriented, preoccupied and emotional in bereavement (Stroebe et al. Reference Stroebe, Schut and Stroebe2005). Both avoidant and anxiously attached individuals thus show less psychological flexibility than securely attached individuals (Cohen and Katz Reference Cohen and Katz2015), impeding oscillation between grief processes. Consequently, both attachment styles are associated with complicated grief reactions (Boelen and Klugkist Reference Boelen and Klugkist2011; Ho et al. Reference Ho, Chan and Ma2013; Meier et al. Reference Meier, Carr and Currier2013; Russ et al. Reference Russ, Stopa and Sivyer2022; Scheidt et al. Reference Scheidt, Hasenburg and Kunze2012; Schenck et al. Reference Schenck, Eberle and Rings2016; Wijngaards-de Meij et al. Reference Wijngaards-de Meij, Stroebe and Schut2007), including greater guilt following bereavement (Cohen and Katz Reference Cohen and Katz2015; Waskowic and Chartier Reference Waskowic and Chartier2003).

Experiential avoidance

Following bereavement, avoidance may support functioning by facilitating the processing of painful information about the loss and restoring capacity to pursue life satisfaction (Shear Reference Shear2010). However, longer-term avoidance is thought to impede acceptance of the loss (Shear Reference Shear2010; Shear et al. Reference Shear, Monk and Houck2007), stalling the bereaved individual in the loss-oriented stage of coping and impeding restorative processes. Experiential avoidance predicts complicated and prolonged grief among bereaved adults (Nam Reference Nam2016; Shear et al. Reference Shear, Monk and Houck2007), including parentally bereaved young adults (Murrell et al. Reference Murrell, Jackson and Lester2018). Avoidance behaviors have also been identified as a core process mediating the relationship between individual risk factors (such as attachment style) and the development of complicated grief (Boelen and Klugkist Reference Boelen and Klugkist2011; Boelen et al. Reference Boelen, van den Hout and van den Bout2006; Eisma et al. Reference Eisma, Stroebe and Schut2013). For example, insecurely attached individuals are thought to have more difficulties integrating and processing the loss and engage in avoidance behaviors to cope, which impedes their adaptation to the loss (Boelen and Klugkist Reference Boelen and Klugkist2011).

Psychological flexibility

Psychological flexibility describes the individual’s capacity to be open to and accepting of difficult internal experiences (thoughts/feelings), and to respond flexibly to different situational demands by shifting thinking patterns, behavior, and coping strategies (Kashdan and Rottenberg Reference Kashdan and Rottenberg2010; Landstra et al. Reference Landstra, Ciarrochi and Deane2013). It is associated with psychological well-being (Kashdan et al. Reference Kashdan, Barrios and Forsyth2006; Kashdan and Rottenberg Reference Kashdan and Rottenberg2010), and is also thought to influence adjustment to bereavement (Burton et al. Reference Burton, Yan and Pat-Horenczyk2012) by helping individuals shift between loss- and restoration-oriented coping. Consequently, individuals with low psychological flexibility are more likely to report self-judgement about their grief and engage in avoidance- or suppression-based coping strategies (Bonanno et al. Reference Bonanno, Papa and Lalande2004; Stroebe and Schut Reference Stroebe and Schut1999). Similarly, those who do not flexibly apply coping strategies are more likely to experience complicated grief and bereavement guilt (Prigerson and Jacobs Reference Prigerson, Jacobs, Stroebe, Hansson, Stroebe and Schut2001; Schenck et al. Reference Schenck, Eberle and Rings2016; Simon Reference Simon2012). Conversely, flexible coping is associated with milder grief reactions and improved adjustment to bereavement, including meaning making and accessing social support (Cohen and Katz Reference Cohen and Katz2015). Psychological flexibility may also mediate the relationship between attachment and bereavement outcomes: securely attached adults reported greater flexibility, which was associated with less grief and higher post-traumatic growth, after the death of a sibling (Cohen and Katz Reference Cohen and Katz2015).

Present study

Previous literature suggests that insecure attachments, experiential avoidance, and low psychological flexibility may contribute to bereaved adults’ vulnerability to developing complicated grief reactions. However, it is unclear to what extent findings from studies of middle to older adults will generalize to younger adults who have lost an attachment figure, who may still be developing coping abilities and flexibility. Additionally, the role of these processes in contributing to bereavement guilt specifically has yet to be investigated. Young adulthood is a particularly interesting developmental stage at which to study guilt, as it seems to mark a transition from largely adaptive guilt in childhood (e.g. as a driver of moral cognitions and behaviors) to the variably mal/adaptive guilt of adulthood (including, but not limited to, bereavement guilt; Luck and Luck-Sikorski Reference Luck and Luck-Sikorski2021). As young adults are typically moving from dependence on childhood attachment figures toward greater independence (Ribar and Wong Reference Ribar, Wong, Baxter, Lam, Povey, Lee and Zubrick2022), the loss of such an attachment figure at this stage may also produce different reactions compared to other ages.

The aim of this study is to examine associations between attachment style, experiential avoidance, psychological flexibility, and bereavement guilt among young adults bereaved by a caregiver’s cancer. As previous literature has suggested that experiential avoidance and psychological flexibility may be involved in the relationship between attachment style and bereavement outcomes, we also investigate the potential role of these factors as mediators. Research questions were as follows:

RQ1: Are attachment style (1a), experiential avoidance (1b), and psychological flexibility (1c) related to young adults’ experience of bereavement guilt following the death of a caregiver to cancer?

RQ2: Are the effects of attachment style on bereavement guilt mediated by experiential avoidance and psychological flexibility?

Based on previous literature, it was hypothesized that bereavement guilt would be positively associated with anxious attachment, avoidant attachment (RQ1a), and experiential avoidance (RQ1b), and negatively associated with psychological flexibility (RQ1c). It was additionally hypothesized that the negative relationships between attachment styles and bereavement guilt would be mediated by experiential avoidance and psychological flexibility (RQ2).

Method

Study design

Data was collected through an online survey hosted on Qualtrics. After indicating consent to participate, participants completed brief demographic and cancer questions, followed by study measures (ordered as follows: trait guilt, bereavement guilt, attachment style, psychological flexibility, and experiential avoidance). The survey took 20–30 minutes to complete. The study received ethical approval from Macquarie University.

Participants and recruitment

Participants were recruited to the study via an undergraduate psychology student research pool from an Australian university, and from Canteen Australia (a not-for-profit organization supporting young people impacted by cancer, including young adults bereaved by a parent/guardian’s cancer). To be eligible, participants had to: (a) be aged between 18 and 25 years; (b) be fluent in English; (c) have had at least 1 primary caregiver die from cancer; and (d) not be experiencing clinical levels of depression or anxiety. University participants received course credit while Canteen Australia recruits received a $10 online groceries voucher as compensation for their time.

Measures

Outcome: Bereavement guilt

Bereavement guilt was assessed using the 14-item Bereavement Guilt Scale (Li et al. Reference Li, Stroebe and Chan2017), covering 5 components: responsibility for death, hurting the deceased, survivor guilt, indebtedness guilt, and guilt feelings. Participants rate items on a 5-point Likert scale (does not describe me at alldescribes me very well). Total scores range from 14 to 70 (higher scores indicate higher guilt). In this study, the scale had good internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = .84).

Attachment style

The Experience in Close Relationships-Revised questionnaire (Fraley et al. Reference Fraley, Waller and Brennan2000) comprises 2 subscales assessing attachment-related anxiety (18 items; Cronbach’s α = .89) and avoidance (18 items; Cronbach’s α = .91). Participants indicate the extent to which statements are characteristic of their relationship with the deceased using a 7-point Likert scale (disagree stronglyagree strongly). Total scores range from 18 to 126, with higher scores indicating greater anxiety or avoidance.

Experiential avoidance

The 7-item Acceptance and Action Questionnaire II (AAQ-II; Bond et al. Reference Bond, Hayes and Baer2011) assessed experiential avoidance. Participants rate the extent to which items are true of them on a 7-point scale (never truealways true), with total scores ranging from 7 to 49. The AAQ-II had good internal consistency in this study (Cronbach’s α = .86).

Psychological flexibility

The 20-item Psychological Flexibility Questionnaire (PFQ; Ben-Itzhak et al. Reference Ben-Itzhak, Bluvstein and Maor2014) assessed psychological flexibility. Participants rated the extent to which items characterized them on a scale from disagree strongly to agree strongly. Due to an administrative error, this rating was made using a 7-point scale rather than the originally intended 6-point scale (additional midpoint: neither agree nor disagree); however, scale anchors were the same. The PFQ had good internal consistency in this study (Cronbach’s α = .88).

Demographics and bereavement characteristics

Participants reported their gender, age, cultural background, relationship status, and education. They also reported which parent/carer had died, time since bereavement, if they lived with the deceased, the impact of the death on their life (1 = no effect–5 = extremely strong effect), and if they had received counseling or support related to the bereavement (yes/no).

Data cleaning and analysis

All data were downloaded into IBM’s Statistical Package for the Social Sciences v.27 (IBM Corp 2020) and checked. Of 131 responses, 35 were excluded due to insufficient measure completion (n = 26), being outliers on time since bereavement (greater than 2SD above the sample mean; n = 5), or repeat survey completion (only earlier survey retained; n = 4). Two participants had small amounts of missing data (1–2 items); these datapoints were imputed using mean substitution.

Bivariate correlations

Pearson’s bivariate correlations explored associations between attachment style, psychological flexibility, experiential avoidance, and bereavement guilt.

Mediation analyses

A series of mediation analyses were conducted to examine the association between attachment style (anxious, avoidant) and bereavement guilt, and whether these associations were mediated by psychological flexibility and experiential avoidance. Mediation effects were tested separately, given the limited sample size and the correlation between mediator variables (Preacher and Hayes Reference Preacher and Hayes2008). As perceived impact of bereavement (r = .314, p < .001) and engagement with bereavement counseling/support (r = −.222, p = .029) were significantly correlated with bereavement guilt, these factors were included as covariates in the analyses.

Results

Participant characteristics

The final dataset included responses from 97 participants; most were female (77%), Caucasian (72%), and born in Australia (85%). Respondents were 20.2 years old (SD = 2.3 years, range: 17–25 years) and 4.1 years post-bereavement (SD = 3.2 years, range: 1 month–12 years), on average. Full demographic details and means for study measures are presented in Table 1.

Table 1. Participant demographics, cancer, and bereavement characteristics

Bivariate correlations

Table 2 displays correlations between key study variables. Anxious and avoidant attachment styles were significantly positively correlated with each other; both were negatively correlated with psychological flexibility, and anxious attachment was also positively correlated with experiential avoidance. Bereavement guilt was positively correlated with anxious attachment and experiential avoidance, and negatively correlated with psychological flexibility. Bereavement guilt was not significantly correlated with avoidant attachment. Time since bereavement was only significantly associated with avoidant attachment.

Table 2. Correlations between study variables

* p ≤ .05; **p ≤ .01; ***p ≤ .001.

Mediation analyses

Figure 1 presents the path diagram for the mediation; Table 3 summarizes results of these analyses. The effect of anxious attachment on bereavement guilt was significantly mediated by experiential avoidance (β = .0617, 95% CI [.0060, .1554]): anxious attachment was associated with greater experiential avoidance (significant path A), which was in turn associated with greater bereavement guilt (significant path B). The overall association between anxious attachment and bereavement guilt (path C) was significantly altered after accounting for the indirect effect of experiential avoidance (path C′) but remained significant, indicating a partial mediation effect. Psychological flexibility did not significantly mediate the association between anxious attachment and bereavement guilt (β = .0381, 95% CI [−.0059, .1115]). Mediation analyses were not run for the association between avoidant attachment and bereavement guilt, as the bivariate correlation between these variables was not statistically significant.

Figure 1. Path diagram for mediation analyses.

Table 3. Mediation of associations between anxious attachment and bereavement guilt

* p ≤ .05; **p ≤ .01; ***p ≤ .001.

Discussion

While many bereaved young adults are able to adapt to the death of a caregiver to cancer (Angelhoff et al. Reference Angelhoff, Sveen and Alvariza2021; Hoffmann et al. Reference Hoffmann, Kaiser and Kersting2018), a significant proportion experience persistent or complicated grief symptoms (Bylund-Grenklo et al. Reference Bylund-Grenklo, Fürst and Nyberg2016), including bereavement guilt. Attachment style (Russ et al. Reference Russ, Stopa and Sivyer2022), psychological flexibility (Bonanno et al. Reference Bonanno, Papa and Lalande2004), and experiential avoidance (Murrell et al. Reference Murrell, Jackson and Lester2018; Shear et al. Reference Shear, Monk and Houck2007) have been implicated in shaping bereaved individuals’ grief and adaptation to their loss. In this study, bereavement guilt was associated with anxious, but not avoidant attachment to the deceased; additionally, the association between anxious attachment and bereavement guilt was partially mediated by experiential avoidance. While psychological flexibility was not a mediator of the attachment–guilt relationship, it was negatively correlated with bereavement guilt, as was previous engagement with bereavement counseling. These findings offer insights into how bereavement interventions and support can be tailored for young adults experiencing more complicated forms of grief following the loss of a caregiver, particularly where guilt is involved.

Consistent with hypotheses and previous work (Boelen and Klugkist Reference Boelen and Klugkist2011; Cohen and Katz Reference Cohen and Katz2015; Meier et al. Reference Meier, Carr and Currier2013), anxious attachment was significantly and positively related to bereavement guilt. Bowlby’s (Reference Bowlby1969) attachment theory describes these individuals as investing more heavily and developing greater dependency within their relationships, which may predispose them to more pronounced and intense grief reactions following bereavement (Mikulincer and Shaver Reference Mikulincer, Shaver, Stroebe, Hansson, Schut and Stroebe2008; Stroebe and Schut Reference Stroebe and Schut1999). This may include failure to accept the loss, preoccupation with the bereavement, or maladaptive appraisals about their role in the death of the deceased (a component of bereavement guilt; Mikulincer and Shaver Reference Mikulincer, Shaver, Stroebe, Hansson, Schut and Stroebe2008; Stroebe et al. Reference Stroebe, Schut and Stroebe2005). Accordingly, research has shown that anxiously attached adults experience more complications in bereavement (Delespaux et al. Reference Delespaux, Ryckebosch-Dayez and Heeren2013; Fraley and Bonnano Reference Fraley and Bonnano2004). The findings from this study indicate the same is true of young adults, affirming anxious attachment style as a risk factor for bereavement guilt.

By contrast, avoidant attachment was not significantly associated with bereavement guilt for young adults in this study. Previous studies examining this relationship have also documented mixed evidence (Kho et al. Reference Kho, Kane and Priddis2015; Mikulincer and Shaver Reference Mikulincer, Shaver, Stroebe, Hansson, Schut and Stroebe2008). In fact, 1 study found avoidantly attached individuals reported less intense grief reactions following the death of a romantic partner, as well as more restoration-oriented (i.e. avoidant) strategies (Delespaux et al. Reference Delespaux, Ryckebosch-Dayez and Heeren2013). This outcome is consistent with attachment theory and the dual process model (Stroebe et al. Reference Stroebe, Schut and Stroebe2005); the tendency to avoid attachment-related emotions and maintain distance and independence in relationships may protect individuals from more complex manifestations of grief (Stroebe et al. Reference Stroebe, Schut and Stroebe2005), including bereavement guilt.

As hypothesized based on previous literature (Murrell et al. Reference Murrell, Jackson and Lester2018; Nam Reference Nam2016; Shear et al. Reference Shear, Monk and Houck2007), experiential avoidance was a significant partial mediator of the relationship between anxious attachment and bereavement guilt. While experiential avoidance may help to create emotional distance that is adaptive in the immediate wake of a loss, longer-term experiential avoidance appears to impede acceptance and adjustment to bereavement (Boelen and Klugkist Reference Boelen and Klugkist2011; Shear Reference Shear2010). For participants in this study, who were on average 4.1 years post-bereavement, the tendency to avoid difficult feelings and thoughts may have obstructed the processing of grief, and was associated with ongoing bereavement guilt, particularly among those who were anxiously attached to the deceased. This may also in part explain why greater time since bereavement was not significantly associated with lower bereavement guilt in this study. Longitudinal research tracking bereaved young adults in the wake of their loss may help to clarify how these processes unfold over time.

It is perhaps surprising that psychological flexibility did not significantly mediate the anxious attachment–guilt relationship, as hypothesized (Bonanno et al. Reference Bonanno, Papa and Lalande2004; Stroebe and Schut Reference Stroebe and Schut1999) and as was the case for experiential avoidance. The discrepancy between findings regarding psychological flexibility and experiential avoidance is particularly interesting as the experiential avoidance measure (AAQ-II) is often used to assess psychological flexibility (Cherry et al. Reference Cherry, Vander Hoeven and Patterson2021), although it has been argued to be more indicative of neuroticism and/or negative affect (e.g. Rochefort et al. Reference Rochefort, Baldwin and Chmielewski2018). Given that psychological flexibility was significantly correlated with both attachment styles and bereavement guilt, and the mediation by psychological flexibility approached significance, this study may have had insufficient statistical power to detect a significant effect. Future work may benefit from extended recruitment periods to ensure that a sufficient sample size can be obtained.

Clinical implications

This study’s findings identify potential risk (anxious attachment, experiential avoidance) and protective (psychological flexibility) factors that are implicated in the occurrence of bereavement guilt, which may be used to tailor supportive interventions for bereaved young adults. For example, psychological flexibility may be enhanced through acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT), which is increasingly applied in psycho-oncology (González-Fernández and Fernández-Rodríguez Reference González-Fernández and Fernández-Rodríguez2019), including with young adults (Allison et al. Reference Allison, Patterson and McDonald2022; Clarke et al. Reference Clarke, Patterson and McDonald2021; Patterson et al. Reference Patterson, McDonald and Bibby2022) and end-of-life carers (Davis et al. Reference Davis, Deane and Lyons2017). ACT-, mindfulness-, and self-compassion-based approaches may also be helpful in reducing experiential avoidance (Arch and Mitchell Reference Arch and Mitchell2015; Yela et al. Reference Yela, Crego and Buz2022). While attachment style tends to be relatively stable over time (Sutton Reference Sutton2019) and may be difficult to alter (particularly as it relates to attachment to the deceased), understanding bereaved young adults’ attachment styles may help to tailor interventions to better support their individual needs. For example, approaches such as interpersonal therapy, complicated grief therapy, and schema therapy have been suggested as potentially helpful approaches for bereaved individuals who are insecurely attached (Schenck et al. Reference Schenck, Eberle and Rings2016). However, few psychotherapeutic interventions have been developed for young people bereaved by familial cancer (Ing et al. Reference Ing, Patterson and Szabo2019), and more work is needed to develop and evaluate support services for this population. Promisingly, however, participants who had previously accessed bereavement counseling/support reported lower levels of bereavement guilt, suggesting that this outcome may be ameliorated through psychotherapeutic intervention. This is particularly notable given that time since bereavement was not significantly associated with lower bereavement guilt, suggesting that guilt does not necessarily resolve itself over time.

Strengths, limitations, and future research directions

A key strength of this work was the recruitment of almost 100 young adults bereaved by a caregiver’s death to cancer – a population that has historically been difficult to recruit. However, this necessitated including participants who were several years post-bereavement, by which time they may have better adjusted to the loss (regardless of attachment style and coping approach) – although interestingly, bereavement guilt was not significantly correlated with time since bereavement. The study was also underpowered to examine multiple mediators in the same statistical model, as well as to examine potential variations in experiences between subgroups (e.g. genders, relation to the deceased). Given that participants were predominantly Caucasian women, it is also unclear whether these findings would generalize to other demographic groups. The cross-sectional design means that causality cannot be conclusively determined, and further longitudinal research is needed to confirm the relationship between attachment style, experiential avoidance, psychological flexibility, and bereavement guilt. Subsequent work may also benefit from the use of alternative measures of experiential avoidance, given criticisms of the AAQ-II (Cherry et al. Reference Cherry, Vander Hoeven and Patterson2021; Rochefort et al. Reference Rochefort, Baldwin and Chmielewski2018), including measures which can delineate different aspects of experiential avoidance, psychological flexibility, and guilt. Finally, it must be noted that the erroneous inclusion of a seventh, “neither agree nor disagree” response option on the PFQ altered the nature of this scale, and findings drawing from this measure must therefore be interpreted with caution.

Conclusions

Given the limited literature on cancer-related bereavement in young adulthood, this study offers important theoretical and clinical insights into factors associated with more complex aspects of grief in this population. Specifically, this work identified that anxious attachment is associated with ongoing bereavement complications in the years following the death of a caregiver to cancer, with experiential avoidance partially mediating this relationship. While further research is needed to better understand the interaction between these factors and other related constructs, such as psychological flexibility, these findings may be helpful in selecting therapeutic approaches to use with this population.

Acknowledgments

We thank the young people who participated in this study for their contributions to this project. We also thank Agustina Himawan for her help in manuscript preparation. The authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose.

Funding

Three of the authors were employed by Canteen, a registered charity which receives funding from a variety of government and individual sources. This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Competing interests

There are no conflicts of interest to disclose.

References

Ainsworth, MS (1989) Attachments beyond infancy. American Psychologist 44(4), 709716. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.44.4.709CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allison, KR, Patterson, P, McDonald, FEJ, et al. (2022) Truce: Feasibility and acceptability of an Acceptance and Commitment Therapy-based intervention for adolescents and young adults impacted by parental cancer. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science 28, 91101. doi:10.1016/j.jcbs.2023.03.014CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Angelhoff, C, Sveen, J, Alvariza, A, et al. (2021) Communication, self-esteem and prolonged grief in parent-adolescent dyads, 1–4 years following the death of a parent to cancer. European Journal of Oncology Nursing 50, . doi:10.1016/j.ejon.2020.101883CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arch, JJ and Mitchell, JL (2015) An Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) group intervention for cancer survivors experiencing anxiety at re-entry. Psychooncology 25(5), 610615. doi:10.1002/pon.3890CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asselmann, E, Wittchen, H-U, Lieb, R, et al. (2015) Does low coping efficacy mediate the association between negative life events and incident psychopathology? A prospective-longitudinal community study among adolescents and young adults. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences 25(2), 171180. doi:10.1017/S204579601500013XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ben-Itzhak, S, Bluvstein, I and Maor, M (2014) The Psychological Flexibility Questionnaire (PFQ): Development, reliability and validity. WebMed Central Psychology 5(4), .Google Scholar
Birgisdóttir, D, Bylund-Grenklo, T, Kreicbergs, U, et al. (2019) Losing a parent to cancer as a teenager: Family cohesion in childhood, teenage, and young adulthood as perceived by bereaved and non‐bereaved youths. Psychooncology 28(9), 18451853. doi:10.1002/pon.5163CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boelen, PA and Klugkist, I (2011) Cognitive behavioral variables mediate the associations of neuroticism and attachment insecurity with Prolonged Grief Disorder severity. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping 24(3), 291307. doi:10.1080/10615806.2010.527335CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boelen, PA, van den Hout, MA and van den Bout, J (2006) A cognitive-behavioral conceptualization of complicated grief. Clinical Psychology 13, 109128. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2850.2006.00013.xGoogle Scholar
Bonanno, GA, Papa, A, Lalande, K, et al. (2004) The importance of being flexible: The ability to both enhance and suppress emotional expression predicts long-term adjustment. Psychological Science 15(7), 482487. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00705.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bond, FW, Hayes, SC, Baer, RA, et al. (2011) Preliminary psychometric properties of the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire-II: A revised measure of psychological inflexibility and experiential avoidance. Behavior Therapy 42(4), 676688. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2011.03.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowlby, J (1969) Attachment and Loss. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Boyle, P and Levin, B (2008) World Cancer Report 2008. Lyon, International Agency for Research on Cancer Press.Google Scholar
Burton, CL, Yan, OH, Pat-Horenczyk, R, et al. (2012) Coping flexibility and complicated grief: A comparison of American and Chinese samples. Depression and Anxiety 29(1), 1622. doi:10.1002/da.20888CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bylund-Grenklo, T, Fürst, CJ, Nyberg, T, et al. (2016) Unresolved grief and its consequences. A nationwide follow-up of teenage loss of a parent to cancer 6–9 years earlier. Supportive Care in Cancer 24, 30953103. doi:10.1007/s00520-016-3118-1Google ScholarPubMed
Bylund-Grenklo, T, Kreicbergs, U, Hauksdóttir, A, et al. (2013) Self-injury in teenagers who lost a parent to cancer: A nationwide, population-based, long-term follow-up. JAMA Pediatrics 167(2), 133140. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.430CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camacho, D, Pérez-Nieto, and Gordillo, F (2018) The role of rumination in the guilt associated with bereavement according to cause of death. Psychiatry Research 270, 10531058. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2018.04.065CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cherry, KM, Vander Hoeven, E, Patterson, TS, et al. (2021) Defining and measuring “psychological flexibility”: A narrative scoping review of diverse flexibility and rigidity constructs and perspectives. Clinical Psychology Review 84, . doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2021.101973CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clarke, K, Patterson, P, McDonald, FEJ, et al. (2021) The development and process evaluation of a 3-day acceptance and commitment therapy group program for adolescent cancer survivors. Child and Youth Care Forum 50, 229246. doi:10.1007/s10566-020-09571-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, O and Katz, M (2015) Grief and growth of bereaved siblings as related to attachment style and flexibility. Death Studies 39, 158164. doi:10.1080/07481187.2014.923069CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, EL, Deane, FP, Lyons, GCB, et al. (2017) Feasibility randomised controlled trial of a self-help acceptance and commitment therapy intervention for grief and psychological distress in carers of palliative care patients. Journal of Health Psychology 25(3), 322339. doi:10.1177/1359105317715091CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delespaux, E, Ryckebosch-Dayez, A-S, Heeren, A, et al. (2013) Attachment and severity of grief: The mediating role of negative appraisal and inflexible coping. OMEGA - Journal of Death and Dying 67(3), 269289. doi:10.2190/OM.67.3.bCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisma, MC, Stroebe, MS, Schut, HAW, et al. (2013) Avoidance processes mediate the relationship between rumination and symptoms of complicated grief and depression following loss. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 122(4), 961970. doi:10.1037/a0034051CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiore, J (2021) A systematic review of the dual process model of coping with bereavement (1999–2016). OMEGA - Journal of Death and Dying 84(2), 414458. doi:10.1177/0030222819893139CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fraley, RC and Bonnano, GA (2004) Attachment and loss: A test of three competing models on the association between attachment-related avoidance and adaptation to bereavement. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 30(7), 878890. doi:10.1177/0146167204264289CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fraley, RC, Waller, NG and Brennan, KA (2000) An item response theory analysis of self-reported measures of adult attachment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 78(2), 350365. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.2.350CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
González-Fernández, S and Fernández-Rodríguez, C (2019) Acceptance and commitment therapy in cancer: Review of applications and findings. Behavioral Medicine 45(3), 255269. doi:10.1080/08964289.2018.1452713CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herberman Mash, HB, Fullerton, CS, Shear, K, et al. (2014) Complicated grief and depression in young adults: Personality and relationship quality. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 202(7), 539543. doi:10.1097/NMD.0000000000000155CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ho, SMY, Chan, ISF, Ma, EPW, et al. (2013) Continuing bonds, attachment style, and adjustment in the conjugal bereavement among Hong Kong Chinese. Death Studies 37(3), 248268. doi:10.1080/07481187.2011.634086CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoffmann, R, Kaiser, J and Kersting, A (2018) Psychosocial outcomes in cancer-bereaved children and adolescents: A systematic review. Psychooncology 27(10), 23272338. doi:10.1002/pon.4863CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
IBM Corp (2020) IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, version 27.0. Armonk, NY, IBM Corp.Google Scholar
Ing, V, Patterson, P, Szabo, M, et al. (2019) Interventions available to adolescents and young adults bereaved by familial cancer: A systematic literature review. BMJ Supportive and Palliative Care 12(e5), e632e640. doi:10.1136/bmjspcare-2019-001959CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joa, B and Newberg, AB (2023) Neuropsychological comparison of guilt and grief: A review of guilt aspects in prolonged grief disorder. OMEGA - Journal of Death and Dying 87(2), 591613. doi:10.1177/00302228211024111CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karlsson, E, Andersson, K and Ahlström, H (2013) Loneliness despite the presence of others – Adolescents’ experiences of having a parent who becomes ill with cancer. European Journal of Oncology Nursing 17(6), 697703. doi:10.1016/j.ejon.2013.09.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kashdan, TB, Barrios, V, Forsyth, JP, et al. (2006) Experiential avoidance as a generalized psychological vulnerability: Comparisons with coping and emotion regulation strategies. Behaviour Research and Therapy 44(9), 13011320. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2005.10.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kashdan, TB and Rottenberg, J (2010) Psychological flexibility as a fundamental aspect of health. Clinical Psychology Review 30(7), 865878. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.03.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kho, Y, Kane, RT, Priddis, L, et al. (2015) The nature of attachment relationships and grief responses in older adults: An attachment path model of grief. PLoS One 10(10), . doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0133703CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, Y, Lucette, A and Loscalzo, M (2013) Bereavement needs of adults, children, and families after cancer. The Cancer Journal 19(5), 444457. doi:10.1097/PPO.0b013e3182a5babeCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landstra, JMB, Ciarrochi, J, Deane, FP, et al. (2013) Identifying and describing feelings and psychological flexibility predict mental health in men with HIV. British Journal of Health Psychology 18(4), 844857. doi:10.1111/bjhp.12026CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LeBlanc, NJ, Toner, ER, O’Day, EB, et al. (2020) Shame, guilt, and pride after loss: Exploring the relationship between moral emotions and psychopathology in bereaved adults. Journal of Affective Disorders 263, 405412. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2019.11.164CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, J, Stroebe, M, Chan, CLW, et al. (2014) Guilt in bereavement: A review and conceptual framework. Death Studies 38(1-5), 165171. doi:10.1080/07481187.2012.738770CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, J, Stroebe, M, Chan, CLW, et al. (2017) The Bereavement Guilt Scale: Development and preliminary validation. OMEGA - Journal of Death and Dying 75(2), 166183. doi:10.1177/0030222815612309CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, J, Tendeiro, JN and Stroebe, M (2019) Guilt in bereavement: Its relationship with complicated grief and depression. International Journal of Psychology 54(4), 454461. doi:10.1002/ijop.12483CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luck, T and Luck-Sikorski, C (2021) Feelings of guilt in the general adult population: Prevalence and association with depression. Psychology, Health and Medicine 26(9), 11431153. doi:10.1080/13548506.2020.1859558CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lundberg, T, Forinder, U, Olsson, M, et al. (2018) Bereavement stressors and psychosocial well-being of young adults following the loss of a parent – A cross-sectional survey. European Journal of Oncology Nursing 35, 3338. doi:10.1016/j.ejon.2018.05.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDonald, FEJ, Patterson, P and Tindle, R (2020) What young people need when a family member dies of cancer. Supportive Care in Cancer 28, 16311638. doi:10.1007/s00520-019-04973-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meier, AM, Carr, DR, Currier, JM, et al. (2013) Attachment anxiety and avoidance in coping with bereavement: Two studies. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology 32(3), 315334. doi:10.1521/jscp.2013.32.3.315CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mikulincer, M and Shaver, PR (2008) An attachment perspective on bereavement. In Stroebe, MS, Hansson, RO, Schut, H and Stroebe, W (eds), Handbook of Bereavement Research and Practice: Advances in Theory and Intervention. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, 87112.Google Scholar
Murrell, AR, Jackson, R, Lester, EG, et al. (2018) Psychological flexibility and resilience in parentally bereaved college students. OMEGA - Journal of Death and Dying 76(3), 207226. doi:10.1177/0030222817693154CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nam, I (2016) Suicide bereavement and complicated grief: Experiential avoidance as a mediating mechanism. Journal of Loss and Trauma 21(4), 325334. doi:10.1080/15325024.2015.1067099CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oechsle, K, Ullrich, A, Marx, G, et al. (2020) Prevalence and predictors of distress, anxiety, depression, and quality of life in bereaved family caregivers of patients with advanced cancer. American Journal of Hospice & Palliative Medicine 37(3), 201213. doi:10.1177/1049909119872755CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patterson, P, McDonald, FEJ, Bibby, H, et al. (2022) What helps distressed Australian adolescents impacted by cancer? Mechanisms of improvement of the PEER program. Health and Social Care in the Community Manuscript under review. doi:10.1111/hsc.13873CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patterson, P and Rangganadhan, A (2010) Losing a parent to cancer: A preliminary investigation into the needs of adolescents and young adults. Palliative and Supportive Care 8, 255265. doi:10.1017/S1478951510000052CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Preacher, KJ and Hayes, AF (2008) Asymptomatic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods 40, 879891. doi:10.3758/BRM.40.3.879CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prigerson, HO and Jacobs, SC (2001) Traumatic grief as a distinct disorder: A rationale, consensus criteria, and a preliminary empirical test. In Stroebe, MS, Hansson, RO, Stroebe, W and Schut, H (eds), Handbook of Bereavement Research: Consequences, Coping, and Care. American Psychological Association, 613645.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ribar, DC and Wong, C (2022) Emerging adulthood in Australia: How is this stage lived? In Baxter, J, Lam, J, Povey, J, Lee, R and Zubrick, SR (eds), Family Dynamics over the Life Course. Cham: Springer, 157175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rochefort, C, Baldwin, AS and Chmielewski, M (2018) Experiential avoidance: An examination of the construct validity of the AAQ-II and MEAQ. Behavior Therapy 49(3), 435449. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2017.08.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russ, V, Stopa, L, Sivyer, K, et al. (2022) The relationship between adult attachment and complicated grief: A systematic review. OMEGA - Journal of Death and Dying Advance online publication. doi:10.1177/00302228221083110Google ScholarPubMed
Scheidt, CE, Hasenburg, A, Kunze, M, et al. (2012) Are individual differences of attachment predicting bereavement outcome after perinatal loss? A prospective cohort study. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 73(5), 375382. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2012.08.017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schenck, LK, Eberle, KM and Rings, JA (2016) Insecure attachment styles and complicated grief severity: Applying what we know to inform future directions. OMEGA - Journal of Death and Dying 73(3), 231249. doi:10.1177/0030222815576124CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Semple, CJ, McCaughan, E, Beck, ER, et al. (2021) ‘Living in parallel worlds’ – Bereaved parents’ experience of family life when a parent with dependent children is at end of life from cancer: A qualitative study. Palliative Medicine 35(5), 933942. doi:10.1177/02692163211001719CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shear, K, Monk, T, Houck, P, et al. (2007) An attachment-based model of complicated grief including the role of avoidance. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 257, 453461. doi:10.1007/s00406-007-0745-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shear, MK (2010) Exploring the role of experiential avoidance from the perspective of attachment theory and the dual process model. OMEGA - Journal of Death and Dying 61(4), 357369. doi:10.2190/OM.61.4.fCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simon, NM (2012) Is complicated grief a post-loss stress disorder? Depression and Anxiety 29(7), 541544. doi:10.1002/da.21979CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stoppelbein, L and Greening, L (2000) Posttraumatic stress symptoms in parentally bereaved children and adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 39(9), 11121119. doi:10.1097/00004583-200009000-00010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stroebe, M and Schut, H (1999) The dual process model of coping with bereavement: Rationale and description. Death Studies 223(3), 197224. doi:10.1080/074811899201046Google Scholar
Stroebe, M, Schut, H and Stroebe, W (2005) Attachment in coping with bereavement: A theoretical integration. Review of General Psychology 9(1), 4866. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.9.1.48CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stroebe, M, Stroebe, W, van de Shoot, R, et al. (2014) Guilt in bereavement: The role of self-blame and coping with loss. PLoS One 9(5), . doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0096606CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sung, H, Ferlay, J, Siegel, RL, et al. (2021) Global cancer statistics 2020: GLOBOCAN estimates of incidence and mortality worldwide for 36 cancers in 185 countries. CA: A Cancer Journal for Clinicians 71(3), 209249. doi:10.3322/caac.21660Google ScholarPubMed
Sutton, TE (2019) Review of attachment theory: Familial predictors, continuity and change, and intrapersonal and relational outcomes. Marriage and Family Review 55(1), 122. doi:10.1080/01494929.2018.1458001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Varathakeyan, A, McDonald, FEJ, Patterson, P, et al. (2017) Accessing support before or after a parent dies from cancer and young people’s current wellbeing. Supportive Care in Cancer 26, 797805. doi:10.1007/s00520-017-3891-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waskowic, TD and Chartier, BM (2003) Attachment and the experience of grief following the loss of a spouse. OMEGA - Journal of Death and Dying 47(1), 7791. doi:10.2190/0CMC-GYP5-N3QH-WEH4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wijngaards-de Meij, L, Stroebe, M, Schut, H, et al. (2007) Patterns of attachment and parents’ adjustment to the death of their child. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 33, 537548. doi:10.1177/0146167206297400CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yela, JR, Crego, A, Buz, J, et al. (2022) Reductions in experiential avoidance explain changes in anxiety, depression and well-being after a mindfulness and self-compassion (MSC) training. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice 95, 402422. doi:10.1111/papt.12375CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zisook, S and Shear, K (2009) Grief and bereavement: What psychiatrists need to know. World Psychiatry: Official Journal of the World Psychiatric Association (WPA) 8(2), 6774. doi:10.1002/j.2051-5545.2009.tb00217.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zordan, RD, Bell, ML, Price, M, et al. (2019) Long-term prevalence and predictors of prolonged grief disorder amongst bereaved cancer caregivers: A cohort study. Palliative and Supportive Care 17, 507514. doi:10.1017/S1478951518001013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Participant demographics, cancer, and bereavement characteristics

Figure 1

Table 2. Correlations between study variables

Figure 2

Figure 1. Path diagram for mediation analyses.

Figure 3

Table 3. Mediation of associations between anxious attachment and bereavement guilt