Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rcrh6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T09:16:16.946Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A Simple Method of Constructing Certain Magic Rectangles of Even Order

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 November 2016

J. P. N. Phillips*
Affiliation:
Dept. of Psychology, The University, Hull

Extract

Magic squares might reasonably be supposed to be amongst the most useless of mathematical diversions. Yet, like other apparently fruitless amusements such as Eulerian squares and Kirkman’s school-girls problem, they have found applications in the field of the design of experiments. In an investigation of the effects of two variables upon a phenomenon, where the observations are obtained one after the other, magic squares make it possible to take account of linear trend in the results. For example (the writer is only competent to offer illustrations in the field of psychology : others in bis own field will doubtless occur to the reader) Miller and Selfridge investigated how much subjects could remember of series of words which varied both in their length and the degree to which they resembled everyday English. Again Fitts and Annett, Golby and Kay investigated how fast a subject could alternately tap with a metal stylus two separate metal plates, and how his speed was affected by the width of the plates and the distance between them. In both cases the subjects performed the task a number of times under different combinations of levels of the variables (length and resemblance to English, and width and distance respectively), so that any analysis of the data is liable to be vitiated by the fact that each subject will probably improve with practice throughout the experiment, unless special account is taken of this trend.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Mathematical Association 1968

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

[1] Miller, G. A. and Selfridge, J. A., 1950. “Verbal context and the recall of meaningful material.” Amer. J. Psychol., 63, 176185.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
[2] Pitts, P. M., 1954. “The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement.” J. exp. Psychol. 47, 381391.Google Scholar
[3] Annett, J., Golby, G. W. and Kay, H, 1958. “The measurement of elements in an assembly task—the information output of the human motor system.” Q. J. exp. Psychol., 10, 111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
[4] Phillips, J. P. N., 1964. “The use of magic squares for balancing and assessing order effects in some analysis of variance designs.” Applied Statistics, 13, 6773.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
[5] Harmuth, T., 1881. “Ueber magische Quadrate and áhnliche Zahlenflguren.” Arch. Math. Phys., 66, 286313.Google Scholar
[6] Harmuth, T., 1881. “Ueber magische Rechtecke mit ungeraden Seitenzahlen.” Arch. Math. Phys., 66, 413447.Google Scholar
[7] Andrews, W. S., 1917. “The construction of magic squares and rectangles by the method of ‘complementary differences’,” in “Magic squares and cubes.” Open Court.Google Scholar