Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-g8jcs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T05:43:09.702Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Kiasu and Creativity in Singapore: An Empirical Test of the Situated Dynamics Framework

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 November 2017

Chi-Ying Cheng*
Affiliation:
Singapore Management University, Singapore
Ying-Yi Hong
Affiliation:
Chinese University of Hong Kong, China
*
Corresponding author: Chi-Ying Cheng ([email protected])

Abstract

This article investigates how Singaporeans’ creativity is influenced by Kiasu, an indigenous construct corresponding to fear of losing out. We examine the impact of Kiasu on creativity, both as a personal value and a shared cultural norm in four studies. Study 1 showed that Singaporeans’ Kiasu value endorsement predicts lower individual creativity. Study 2 demonstrated that this negative relationship is mediated by a self-regulatory focus on prevention. Study 3 further showed the impact of Kiasu as a personal value and a cultural norm by finding a significant three-way interaction effect of Kiasu prime, personal Kiasu value endorsement, and need for cognitive closure on participants’ creativity. Study 4 addressed the Singaporean paradox and found that Singaporeans exhibit higher creativity when primed with their multi-ethnic culture than under control conditions. However, those who associated Singapore with Kiasu lost this advantage. These findings support the situated dynamics framework of cultural influence on behavior such that values, norms, and situational cues play a role in producing a cultural pattern of creative performance. This research also has implications for how to incubate creative performance in Asian countries.

摘要:

摘要:

本文探讨「怕输」、原文「惊输」这个新加坡本土构念如何影响个体的创造力。在本研究中, 「怕输」作为个人价值观与文化行为规范的效果均被检验。研究一显示, 新加坡受试者的个人「怕输」价值观负向预测其创造力。研究二显示受试者的预防取向中介前述负向关系。研究三显示个人「怕输」价值观、「怕输」文化行为规范, 以及认知闭合需求与受试者的创造力产生交互作用。研究四显示, 新加坡受试者在经历新加坡的多族裔文化的触发效应之后, 展现出较高的创造力。然而, 当触发新加坡文化令受试者对「怕输」构念产生联想时, 该触发效应无法提升受试者的创造力。本研究支持了文化情境动力论, 显示文化价值观、行为规范、及情境线索联合影响个体的创造力。本研究结果为亚洲国家如何提升创造力提供了理论基础和实践的建议。

यह शोध पत्र इस पर अनुसंधान करता है की किस प्रकार सिंगापुरवासियों की रचनात्मकता कियासु- हारने के भय सम्बन्धी एक देशज संरचना- से प्रभावित है. हमने चार अध्ययनों के माध्यम से कियासु के रचनात्मकता पर प्रभाव का निजी मूल्य तथा सहभाजीत सांस्कृतिक कायदे के रूप में किया है. प्रथम अध्ययन के अनुसार सिंगापुरवासियों का कियासु मूल्य अनुमोदन निम्न व्यक्तिगर सृजनात्मकता सूचित करता है. दूसरे अध्ययन ने यह दिखाया की इस नकारात्मक सम्बन्ध में स्व-नियंत्रक केन्द्रीयता मध्यस्थ है. तीसरे अध्ययन ने मूल कियासु, व्यक्तिगत कियासु मूल्य अनुमोदन व प्रतिभागियों की रचनात्मकता के संज्ञानात्मक समापन की आवश्यकता की त्रि-आयामी अन्तः क्रिया दिखाकर कियासु के व्यक्तिगत मूल्य तथा सांस्कृतिक कायदे के रूप में प्रभाव को दर्शाया है. चौथे अध्ययन ने सिंगापुर विडम्बना पर प्रकाश डाला दिखाया की सिंगापुरवासी बहुसांस्कृतिक मूल में नियंत्रण स्थिति की तुलना में अधिक रचनात्मकता दिखाते हैं. लेकिन जिन्होंने सिंगापुर को कियासु से जोड़ा उन्होंने यह अवसर खो दिया. यह निष्कर्ष प्रतिष्ठित गतिशीलता संरचना का समर्थन करते हैं जो व्यवहार पर सांस्कृतिक प्रभाव का अनुमोदन करता है जिससे मूल्य, कायदे और पारिस्थितिकीय संकेत रचनात्मक प्रदर्शन की सांस्कृतिक संरचना बनाने में योगदान देते हैं. इस शिव का एशिया में रचनात्मक प्रदर्शन incubate करने पर भी आशय है.

Sumário:

SUMÁRIO:

Este artigo investiga como a criatividade dos cingapurianos é influenciada por Kiasu, um construto local que corresponde ao medo da perda. Examinamos os impactos do Kiasu na criatividade, tanto como um valor pessoal quanto uma norma cultural compartilhada em quatro estudos. O estudo 1 mostrou que o endosso do valor de Kiasu dos cingapurianos prediz uma menor criatividade individual. O estudo 2 demonstrou que esse relacionamento negativo é mediado por um foco de autorregulação na prevenção. O estudo 3 mostrou ainda os impactos de Kiasu como um valor pessoal e uma norma cultural ao encontrar um significativo efeito de interação de três vias do Kiasu principal, endosso do valor pessoal de Kiasu e necessidade de fechamento cognitivo na criatividade dos participantes. O estudo 4 abordou o paradoxo de Cingapura e descobriu que os cingapurianos exibem uma maior criatividade quando preparados com sua cultura multiétnica do que sob condições de controle. No entanto, aqueles que associaram Singapura com o Kiasu perderam essa vantagem. Essas descobertas sustentam o modelo da dinâmico da situação da influência cultural sobre o comportamento, de modo que valores, normas e sinais situacionais desempenham um papel na produção de um padrão cultural de desempenho criativo. Esta pesquisa também tem implicações sobre como incubar o desempenho criativo em países asiáticos.

Аннотация:

АННОТАЦИЯ:

В этой статье исследуется, как креативность сингапурцев зависит от киасу, т.е. национальной черты, которая подразумевает боязнь проигрыша. Мы изучаем влияние киасу на творчество, как в качестве личностной ценности, так и общей культурной нормы в четырех исследованиях. Первое исследование показало, что принятие ценности киасу у сингапурцев связано с более низкой индивидуальной креативностью. Второе исследование продемонстрировало, что эта обратная взаимосвязь опосредована саморегулируемым фокусом внимания. Далее, в третьей работе изучалось влияние киасу, в качестве личностной ценности и общей культурной нормы, на креативность участников через тройное взаимодействие первичного киасу, индивидуального принятия ценности киасу и потребности в когнитивном закрытии. В четвертом исследовании рассматривался сингапурский парадокс и было установлено, что сингапурцы демонстрируют более высокий творческий потенциал, когда они ориентированы на свою многонациональную культуру, чем в условиях контроля. Однако те, кто связывал Сингапур с киасу, потеряли это преимущество. Эти результаты подтверждают концепцию ситуативной динамики в отношении культурного влияния на поведение, т.е. что поведенческие ценности, нормы и ситуационные сигналы играют определенную роль в культурной структуре творческой деятельности. Данное исследование также делает практическое выводы о том, как стимулировать творческую деятельность в азиатских странах.

Resumen:

RESUMEN:

Este artículo investiga como la creatividad de los singapurenses es influenciada por Kiasu, un constructo autóctono correspondiente al miedo a perder. Examinamos los impactos de Kiasu en la creatividad, tanto como un valor personal como una norma cultural compartida en cuatro estudios. El estudio 1 mostró que el respaldo al valor de Kiasu de los singapurenses predice una creatividad individual menor. El estudio 2 demuestra que esta relación negativa es mediada por un enfoque auto-regulatorio en prevención. El estudio 3 mostró además los impactos del Kiasu como un valor personal y como una norma cultural al encontrar una interacción de tres vías significante del efecto del Kiasu principal, el respaldo al Kiasu personal, y la necesidad por un cierre cognitivo de la creatividad de los participantes. El estudio 4 aborda la paradoja singapurense y encuentra que los singapurenses exhiben una mayor creatividad cuando están preparados con una cultura multiétnica que bajo condiciones de control. Sin embargo, aquellos que asocian Singapur con Kiasu pierden esta ventaja. Estos hallazgos apoyan el marco de dinámicas situadas de la influencia cultural en el comportamiento de modo que los valores, las normas, y los impulsos situacionales juegan un papel en producir un patrón cultural de desempeño creativo. Esta investigación también tiene implicaciones en como incubar el desempeño creativo en países Asiáticos.

Type
Special Issue Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Association for Chinese Management Research 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Amabile, T. M. 1986. The social psychology of creativity: A componential conceptualization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45 (2): 357376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amabile, T. M. 1988. A model of creativity and innovation in organizations. In Staw, B. M. & Cummings, L. L. (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior, 10: 123167. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. W. 1985. Ideals, central tendency, and frequency of instantiation as determinants of graded structure in categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11 (4): 629654.Google ScholarPubMed
Battig, W. F., & Montague, W. E. 1969. Category norms for verbal items in 56 categories: A replication and extension of the Connecticut norms. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 80 (3): 146.Google Scholar
Berry, J. W. 1990. Imposed etics, emics, and derived etics: Their conceptual and operational status in cross-cultural psychology. In Headland, T. N., Pike, K. L., & Harris, M. (Eds.), Frontiers of anthropology, Vol. 7 Emics and etics: The insider/outsider debate: 8499. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Chan, R. 2012. Aug 23. Being ‘kiasu’ is top value in Singapore society, survey finds. The Straits Times. [Cited 28 April 2016]. Available from URL: http://news.asiaone.com/News/Latest+News/Singapore/Story/A1Story20120823-367155.html Google Scholar
Chen, T., Leung, K., Li, F., & Ou, Z. 2015. Interpersonal harmony and creativity in China. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 36 (5): 648672.Google Scholar
Cheng, C.-Y., Leung, A. K.-y., & Wu, T. Y. 2011. Going beyond the multicultural experience–creativity link: The mediating role of emotions. Journal of Social Issues, 67 (4): 806824.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheng, C.-Y., Sanchez-Burks, J., & Lee, F. 2008. Connect the dots within: Creative performance and identity integration. Psychological Science, 19 (11): 11781184.Google Scholar
Cheung, F. M., Leung, K., Zhang, J.-X., Sun, H. F., Gan, Y.-Q., Song, W.-Z., & Xie, D. 2001. Indigenous Chinese personality constructs is the five-factor model complete? Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32 (4): 407433.Google Scholar
Chiu, C.-y., Morris, M. W., Hong, Y., & Menon, T. 2000. Motivated cultural cognition: The impact of implicit cultural theories on dispositional attribution varies as a function of need for closure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78 (2): 247259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chua, Y. M. 2016, April 6. ‘Kiasu’ culture is stifling originality in business: NMP Kuik Shiao-Yin. The Straits Times. [Cited 4 March 2017]. Available from URL: http://www.straitstimes.com/singapore/kiasucultureisstiflingoriginalityinbusinessnmpkuikshiaoyin Google Scholar
Chua, R. Y. J., & Zremski, G. 2016. Can Asians be creative? Asian management insights (Vol. 6). Singapore: Singapore Management University.Google Scholar
Cornell University, INSEAD, & WIPO. 2016. The Global Innovation Index 2016. [Cited 31 January 2017]. Available from URL: https://www.globalinnovationindex.org/analysis-indicator Google Scholar
Cropley, A. 2006. In praise of convergent thinking. Creativity Research Journal, 18 (3): 391404.Google Scholar
Davie, S. 2016, December 25. Pisa and the creativity puzzle. The Straits Time. [Cited 4 March 2017]. Available from URL: http://www.straitstimes.com/singapore/education/pisaandthecreativitypuzzle Google Scholar
Dizik, A. 2014, November 3. Does your child really need a private tutor? BBC Capital. [Cited 28 February 2017]. Available from URL: http://www.bbc.com/capital/story/20131016theglobaltutoringeconomy Google Scholar
Finke, R. A., Ward, T. B., & Smith, S. M. 1992. Creative cognition: Theory, research, and applications. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ford, T. E., & Kruglanski, A. W. 1995. Effects of epistemic motivations on the use of accessible constructs in social judgment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21 (9): 950962.Google Scholar
Friedman, R. S., & Förster, J. 2001. The effects of promotion and prevention cues on creativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81 (9): 10011013.Google Scholar
Friedman, R. S., & Förster, J. 2002. The influence of approach and avoidance motor actions on creative cognition. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38 (1): 4155.Google Scholar
Fu, H.-y., Morris, M. W., Lee, S.-l., Chao, M.-c., Chiu, C.-y., & Hong, Y.-Y. 2007. Epistemic motives and cultural conformity: Need for closure, culture, and context as determinants of conflict judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92 (2): 191207.Google Scholar
Goh, C. 2015. ‘Fear of losing out’ and ‘greed’ as components of the kiasu construct (Honors thesis). Singapore Management University, Singapore.Google Scholar
Gravetter, F., & Wallnau, L. 2014. Essentials of statistics for the behavioral sciences (8th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.Google Scholar
Hayes, A. F. 2012. PROCESS: A versatile computational tool for observed variable mediation, moderation, and conditional process modeling [White paper]. [Cited 31 January 2017]. Available from URL: http://www.afhayes.com/public/process2012.pdf Google Scholar
Higgins, E. T. 1996. Knowledge activation: Accessibility, applicability, and salience. In Higgins, E. T. & Kruglanski, A. W. (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles: 133168. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Higgins, E. T. 1997. Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52 (12): 12801300.Google Scholar
Higgins, E. T., Friedman, R. S., Harlow, R. E., Idson, L. C., Ayduk, O. N., & Taylor, A. 2001. Achievement orientations from subjective histories of success: Promotion pride versus prevention pride. European Journal of Social Psychology, 31 (1): 323.Google Scholar
Ho, J. T. S., Ang, C. E., Loh, J., & Ng, I. 1998. A preliminary study of kiasu behavior: Is it unique to Singapore? Journal of Managerial Psychology, 13 (6): 359370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hong, Y., & Khei, M. 2014. Dynamic multiculturalism: The interplay of socio-cognitive, neural, and genetic mechanisms. In Benet-Martínez, V. & Hong, Y. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of multicultural identity: Basic and applied psychological perspectives. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hong, Y., Morris, M., Chiu, C., & Benet-Martínez, V. 2000. Multicultural minds: A dynamic constructivist approach to culture and cognition. American Psychologist, 55 (7): 709720.Google Scholar
Hogg, M. A. 2016. Social identity theory. In McKeown, S., Haji, R., & Ferguson, N. (Eds.), Understanding peace and conflict through social identity theory: 317. New York: Springer International Publishing.Google Scholar
Hwang, A., Ang, S., & Francesco, A. M. 2002. The silent Chinese: The influence of face and kiasuism on student feedback-seeking behaviors. Journal of Management Education, 26 (1): 7098.Google Scholar
Hyper Island. 2014. Hyper Island executive study: Personality trumps skill in search for talent. [Cited 4 March 2017]. Available from URL: https://www.hyperisland.com/community/news/hyper-island-executive-study Google Scholar
IBM. 2010. IBM 2010 global CEO study: Creativity selected as most crucial factor for future success. [Cited 17 April 2016]. Available from URL: https://www-03.ibm.com/press/us/en/pressrelease/31670.wss Google Scholar
Ip, G. W.-m., Chen, J., & Chiu, C.-y. 2006. The relationship of promotion focus, need for cognitive closure, and categorical accessibility in American and Hong Kong Chinese university students. Journal of Creative Behavior, 40 (3): 201215.Google Scholar
Kirby, E. G., Kirby, S. L., Bell, J. D., & Schafer, C. 2010. Exploring the factors affecting the use of Kiasu tactics. Journal of Behavioral and Applied Management, 11 (3): 249262.Google Scholar
Kirby, E. G., & Ross, J. K. 2007. Kiasu tendency and tactics: A study of their impact on task performance. Journal of Behavioral and Applied Management, 8 (2): 108121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kruglanski, A. W. 1990. Motivations for judging and knowing: Implications for causal attribution. In Higgins, E. T. & Sorrentino, R. M. (Eds.), The handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundation of social behavior (Vol. 2): 333368. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Kruglanski, A. W., & Webster, D. M. 1996. Motivated closing of the mind: ‘Seizing’ and ‘freezing’. Psychological Review, 103 (2): 263283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lau, C. 2007. Why some Singaporeans seem to be lacking creativity? [Cited 28 February 2016]. Available from URL: http://www.charleslau.com/blog/why-some-singaporeans-seem-to-be-lack-of-creativity/ Google Scholar
Leung, A. K.-y., & Chiu, C.-y. 2010. Multicultural experiences, idea receptiveness, and creativity. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 41 (6): 723741.Google Scholar
Leung, A. K.-y., Maddux, W. W., Galinsky, A. D., & Chiu, C.-y. 2008. Multicultural experience enhances creativity: The when and how? American Psychologist, 63 (3): 169181.Google Scholar
Leung, K., Koch, P. T., & Lu, L. 2002. A dualistic model of harmony and its implications for conflict management in Asia. Asia Pacific Journal of Management, 19 (2): 201220.Google Scholar
Leung, K., & Morris, W. M. 2015. Values, schemas, and norms in the culture-behavior nexus: A situated dynamics framework. Journal of International Business Studies, 46 (9): 10281050.Google Scholar
Li, J., & Karakosky, L. 2002. Cultural malleability in an East Asian context: An illustration of the relationship between government policy, national culture, and firm behavior. Administration and Society, 34 (2): 176201.Google Scholar
Liou, S., & Lan, X. 2017. Situational salience of norms moderates cultural differences in the originality and usefulness of creative ideas generated or selected by teams. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. doi/10.1177/0022022116640897 Google Scholar
Maddux, W. W., & Galinsky, A. D. 2009. Cultural borders and mental barriers: The relationship between living abroad and creativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96 (5): 10471061.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mahbubani, K. 2002. Can Asians think? Understanding the divide between East and West. South Royalton: Steerforth Press.Google Scholar
McElhinney, S. 2008. Singapore: The missing T in creativity. Journal of Media Arts Culture. [Cited 4 March 2017]. Available from URL: http://scan.net.au/scan/magazine/display.php?journal_id=52 Google Scholar
Mednick, M. T., Mednick, S. A., & Mednick, E. V. 1964. Incubation of creative performance and specific associative priming. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 69 (1): 8488.Google Scholar
Miles, G. 2013, July 22. Is the Singapore soil fertile for creativity? Today. [Cited 31 January 2017]. Available from URL: http://www.todayonline.com/commentary/singaporesoilfertilecreativity?page=4 Google Scholar
Morris, M. W., & Leung, K. 2010. Creativity east and west: Perspectives and parallels. Management and Organization Review, 6 (3): 313327.Google Scholar
Morris, M. W., Leung, K., Ames, D., & Lickel, B. 1999. Views from inside and outside: Integrating emic and etic insights about culture and justice judgment. Academy of Management Review, 24 (4): 781796.Google Scholar
Murray, C. 2003. Human accomplishment: The pursuit of excellence in the arts and sciences, 800 BC to 1950. New York: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Ng, A. K. 2001. Why Asians are less creative than Westerners? Singapore: Prentice-Hall, Pearson Education Asia.Google Scholar
Ng, A. K. 2003. A cultural model of creative and conforming behavior. Creativity Research Journal, 14 (2–3): 223233.Google Scholar
Ng, E., & Bligh, M. C. 2010. Leading innovation ‘in the trenches’: The concept of a continuum of innovation and strategies to overcome barriers to innovation in cultures like Singapore and beyond. Paper presented at the 8th Annual Asia Academy of Management Conference, Macau.Google Scholar
Pike, K. L. 1967. Language in relation to a unified theory of the structure of human behavior. The Hague: Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roets, A., Kruglanski, A. W., Kossowska, M., Pierro, A., & Hong, Y. 2015. The motivated gatekeeper of our minds: New directions in need for closure theory and research. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 52: 221283.Google Scholar
Scott, J. C. 1998. Seeing like a state: How certain schemes to improve the human condition have failed. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Senge, P., & Carstedt, G. 2001. Innovating our way to the next industrial revolution. MIT Sloan Management Review, 42: 2438.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. 1999. The concept of creativity: Prospects and paradigms. In Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.), Handbook of creativity: 316. London: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sue, S. 1983. Ethnic minorities in psychology: A reexamination. American Psychologist, 38: 583592.Google Scholar
Tadmor, C., Galinsky, A. D., & Maddux, W. W. 2012. Getting the most out of living abroad: Biculturalism and integrative complexity as key drivers of creative and professional success. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103 (3): 520542.Google Scholar
Tan, A.-G., & Law, L.-C. 2000. Teaching creativity: Singapore's experiences. Korean Journal of Problem Solving, 10 (1): 7996.Google Scholar
Vasagar, J. 2016, July 22. Why Singapore's kids are so good at math. Financial Times. [Cited 4 March 2017] Available from URL: https://www.ft.com/content/2e4c61f2-4ec8-11e6-8172-e39ecd3b86fc Google Scholar
Ward, T. B., Smith, S. M., & Vaid, J. 1997. Conceptual structures and processes in creative thought. In Ward, T. B., Smith, S. M., & Vaid, J. (Eds.), Creative thought: An investigation of conceptual structures and processes. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association Books.Google Scholar
Ward, T. B., Patterson, M. J., Sifonis, C. M., Dodds, R. A., & Saunders, K. N. 2002. The role of graded category structure in imaginative thought. Memory & Cognition, 30 (2): 199216.Google Scholar
Webster, D. M., & Kruglanski, A. W. 1994. Individual differences in need for cognitive closure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67 (6): 10491062.Google Scholar
Wyer, R. S. Jr., & Srull, T. K. 1989. Memory and cognition in its social context. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Zhao, X., Lynch, J. G. Jr., & Chen, Q. 2010. Reconsidering Baron and Kenny: Myths and truths about mediation analysis. Journal of Consumer Research, 37: 197206.Google Scholar
Zhou, J., & Su, Y. 2010. A missing piece of the puzzle: The organizational context in cultural patterns of creativity. Management and Organization Review, 6 (3): 391413.Google Scholar
Zou, X., Morris, M. W., & Benet-Martínez, V. 2008. Identity motives and cultural priming: Cultural (dis)identification in assimilative and contrastive responses. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44: 11511159.Google Scholar
Supplementary material: PDF

Cheng and Hong supplementary material

Translated abstracts

Download Cheng and Hong supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 74.9 KB
Supplementary material: PDF

Cheng and Hong supplementary material

Translated abstracts

Download Cheng and Hong supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 66.2 KB
Supplementary material: PDF

Cheng and Hong supplementary material

Translated abstracts

Download Cheng and Hong supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 52.6 KB
Supplementary material: PDF

Cheng and Hong supplementary material

Translated abstracts

Download Cheng and Hong supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 146.7 KB
Supplementary material: PDF

Cheng and Hong supplementary material

Translated abstracts

Download Cheng and Hong supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 53.7 KB