Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-g7gxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-06T11:49:57.329Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Real Interrogation: What Actually Happens When Cops Question Kids

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2024

Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Although the Supreme Court repeatedly cautioned that youthfulness adversely affects juveniles' ability to exercise Miranda rights or make voluntary statements, it endorsed the adult waiver standard—knowing, intelligent, and voluntary—to gauge juveniles' Miranda waivers. By contrast, developmental psychologists question whether young people understand or possess the competence necessary to exercise Miranda rights. This article analyzes quantitative and qualitative data of interrogations of three hundred and seven (307) sixteen- and seventeen-year old youths charged with felony offenses. It reports how police secure Miranda waivers, the tactics they use to elicit information, and the evidence youths provide. The findings bear on three policy issues—procedural safeguards for youths, time limits for interrogations, and mandatory recording of interrogations.

Type
Articles
Copyright
© 2013 Law and Society Association.

The Supreme Court has decided more cases about interrogating youths than any other aspect of juvenile justice (Haley v. Ohio 1948; Gallegos v. Colorado 1962; In re Gault 1967; Fare v. Michael C. 1979; Yarborough v. Alvarado 2004; J.D.B. v. North Carolina 2011). Although the Court repeatedly cautioned that youthfulness adversely affects juveniles' ability to exercise Miranda rights or make voluntary statements, it has not required special procedures to protect young suspects. Rather, it endorsed the adult standard—knowing, intelligent, and voluntary—to gauge juveniles' Miranda waivers (Fare v. Michael C. 1979).

By contrast, developmental psychologists question whether young people understand Miranda or possess the competence necessary to exercise rights. Younger and mid-adolescent youths may not understand Miranda's words or the rights it conveys, may not be as competent as adults are to exercise rights, and may require additional protections (Reference GrissoGrisso 1980; Reference GrissoGrisso et al. 2003). However, most youths sixteen years of age and older appear to understand Miranda on par with adults, although they lack mature judgment and remain susceptible to influence by adult authority (Reference GrissoGrisso 1980, Reference Grisso1981). Youth's vulnerabilities increase their likelihood to confess falsely (Reference KassinKassin et al. 2010).

This article empirically analyzes what happens when police interrogate older youths charged with felony offenses. Part I analyzes the legal framework of juvenile interrogation and research on adolescents' competence to exercise rights. Part II examines interrogation tactics and empirical research on interrogation practices. Part III describes the study's data and methodology. Part IV presents quantitative and qualitative data about routine interrogation of 307 delinquents sixteen years of age or older whom prosecutors charged with felonies. It reviews how police secure Miranda waivers, how they question youths, and how juveniles respond. Part V considers policy implications of the study.

Interrogating Juveniles: Legal Expectations and Developmental Psychology

Haley v. Ohio (1948) and Gallegos v. Colorado (1962) held that youthfulness, lengthy questioning, and absence of counsel or parents rendered juveniles' statements involuntary. In re Gault (1967) granted delinquents the privilege against self-incrimination, among other procedural rights, and reiterated concern about youths' vulnerability during questioning. In re Winship (1970) and Breed v. Jones (1975) fostered a further procedural convergence between juvenile and criminal courts (Reference FeldFeld 1999). Fare v. Michael C. (1979) held that the totality of the circumstances test used to evaluate adults' Miranda waivers governed juveniles' waivers. Fare held that Miranda provided an objective basis to evaluate waivers, denied that developmental differences necessitated special procedures, and required children to assert rights clearly. J.D.B. v. North Carolina (2011) ruled that age was an objective factor and concluded that officers could evaluate how a youth's age would affect feelings of custodial restraint.

Most states use the same Miranda framework for juveniles and adults and require only an understanding of rights and not collateral consequences (Reference FeldFeld 2006a, Reference Feld2006b). Trial judges consider characteristics of the offender—age, education, I.Q., and prior police contacts—and the context of interrogation—location, methods, and length of questioning—when they evaluate Miranda waivers. About ten states require a parent to assist juveniles to invoke or waive Miranda (Reference Drizin, Colgan and LassiterDrizin & Colgan 2004; Reference FarberFarber 2004; Reference LarsonLarson 2003; Reference WoolardWoolard et al. 2008).

Roper v. Simmons (2005) barred states from executing offenders for murder they committed when younger than eighteen-years of age because of reduced culpability. Graham v. Florida (2010) extended Roper and banned sentences of life without parole for youths convicted of non-homicide crimes. Roper and Graham's proportionality analyses offered several reasons why states could not punish youths as severely as they do adults. Those developmental characteristics—immaturity, impulsivity, and susceptibility to social influences—also heighten youths' vulnerability in the interrogation room.

Developmental psychologists distinguish between cognitive ability and maturity of judgment. By mid-adolescence, most youths' cognitive abilities are comparable with adults. They can distinguish right from wrong and reason similarly as their elders (Reference Scott and SteinbergScott & Steinberg 2008; Reference SteinbergSteinberg et al. 2009; Reference Steinberg and CauffmanSteinberg & Cauffman 1999). However, the ability to make good choices with complete information in a laboratory differs from the ability to make adult-like decisions under stressful conditions with incomplete information (Reference SpearSpear 2000; Reference Steinberg and CauffmanSteinberg & Cauffman 1996).

Since the mid-1990s, the MacArthur Network on Adolescent Development and Juvenile Justice has studied decision-making and adjudicative competence (Reference Scott and SteinbergScott & Steinberg 2008). The research distinguishes between cognitive ability and psycho-social maturity of judgment and self-control (Reference Scott and SteinbergScott & Steinberg 2008). While most youths sixteen years of age or older exhibit cognitive abilities comparable with adults, they do not develop mature judgment and adult-like competence until their twenties.

Differences in knowledge, experience, time-perspective, attitude toward risk, impulsivity, and appreciation of consequences contribute to youths' poorer decisions (Reference Scott and GrissoScott & Grisso 1997; Reference SteinbergSteinberg 2005; Reference Scott and SteinbergScott & Steinberg 2008). Compared with adults, adolescents underestimate the amount and likelihood of risks, use a shorter frame, and focus on gains rather than losses (Reference Furby and Beyth-MaromFurby & Beyth-Marom 1992; Reference Grisso, Grisso and SchwartzGrisso 2000). The widest divergence between juveniles' and adults' perception of and preference for risk occurs during mid-teens when youths' criminal activity increases (Reference Scott and SteinbergScott & Steinberg 2008). Neuroscientists attribute differences in how adolescents and adults think and behave to brain maturation and the increased ability of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) to perform executive functions and control impulses (Reference BairdBaird et al. 1999; Reference DahlDahl 2001; Reference Gruber and Yurgelun-ToddGruber & Yurgelun-Todd 2006; Reference MaroneyMaroney 2009; Reference SpearSpear 2000).

Despite the Court's repeated acknowledgment of developmental differences, most states do not provide safeguards to protect juveniles from their immature decisions and use adult standards to gauge their Miranda waivers. Some juveniles may not understand the words of Miranda (Reference RogersRogers et al. 2007; Rogers et al. Reference Rogers2008a, Reference Rogers2008b). Some concepts—the meaning of a right, the term appointed to secure counsel, and waive—render Miranda perplexing to many juveniles (Reference Goldstein and GoldsteinGoldstein & Goldstein 2010). Dumbed-down juvenile warnings often are longer than those used for adults and may inhibit understanding (Reference RogersRogers et al. 2008a, Reference Rogers2008b).

Reference GrissoThomas Grisso (1980, Reference Grisso1981; Reference GrissoGrisso et al. 2003) has studied juveniles' ability to exercise Miranda rights for more than three decades and reported that many youths do not adequately understand the warning. Most adults understood Miranda and most juveniles sixteen years or older understood it about as well as did adults, although substantial minorities of both groups misunderstood some components (Reference GrissoGrisso 1980). Age-related improvements in cognitive ability, competence, and Miranda understanding appear in other studies (Reference KassinKassin et al. 2010; Reference ViljoenViljoen et al. 2007; Reference Viljoen and RoeschViljoen & Roesch 2005). Even youths who understand Miranda's words may be unable to exercise their rights as well as adults do. Juveniles do not fully appreciate the function or importance of rights (Reference GrissoGrisso 1980, Reference Grisso1981), or view them as an entitlement, rather than as a privilege that authorities allow, but may unilaterally withdraw (Reference Grisso and PuritzGrisso et al. 2002).

A defendant must be able to understand proceedings, make rational decisions, and assist counsel to be competent to stand trial (Drope v. Missouri 1975; Dusky v. United States 1960). Development limitations impair youths' competence similarly to how mental illness renders adults incompetent (Reference GrissoGrisso et al. 2003; Reference Scott and GrissoScott & Grisso 2005). Many juveniles fourteen years of age or younger were as severely impaired as adults found incompetent to stand trial (Reference Bonnie, Grisso, Grisso and SchwartzBonnie & Grisso 2000). Even nominally competent adolescents often made poorer decisions than did young adults because of differences in maturity and judgment (Reference GrissoGrisso et al. 2003; Reference Scott and GrissoScott & Grisso 2005). Youths' compromised competence bears on their ability to exercise Miranda rights.

Roper and Graham emphasized that youths' susceptibility to social influences reduced culpability. Miranda characterized custodial interrogation as inherently compelling because police dominate the setting, control the flow of information, and create psychological pressures to comply. Children questioned by authority figures yield more easily to negative pressure (Reference BillingsBillings et al. 2007; Reference GudjonssonGudjonsson 2003), and acquiesce more readily to suggestions during questioning than do adults (Reference AinsworthAinsworth 1993; Reference Bull and CorranBull & Corran 2003; Reference Drizin and LeoDrizin & Leo 2004). Thus, even older youths who understand Miranda may feel more constrained, more susceptible to power differentials, and less able voluntarily to relinquish rights.

Interrogation Practices and Empirical Assessments

Most police interrogators in the United States who have received formal training are schooled in the Reid Method (Reference LeoLeo 2008). It teaches isolation and psychological manipulations—maximization and minimization techniques—to elicit confessions (Reference InbauInbau et al. 2004). Police use both negative incentives—confrontational tactics to scare or intimidate a suspect—and positive incentives—themes, scenarios, or sympathetic alternatives—to make it easier to confess (Reference Kassin and McNallKassin & McNall 1991; Reference Ofshe and LeoOfshe & Leo 1997; Reference LeoLeo 2008). Maximization tactics “convey the interrogator's rock-solid belief that the suspect is guilty and that all denials will fail. Such tactics include making an accusation, over-riding objections, and citing evidence, real or manufactured, to shift the suspect's mental statement from confident to hopeless” (Reference KassinKassin et al. 2010:12). Minimization techniques “provide the suspect with moral justification and face-saving excuses for having committed the crime in question. Using this approach, the interrogator offers sympathy and understanding; normalizes and minimizes the crime” (Reference KassinKassin et al. 2010:12). The Reid Method does not modify interrogation tactics to accommodate developmental differences between youths and adults (Reference Meyer and ReppucciMeyer & Reppucci 2007; Reference Owen-KostelnikOwen-Kostelnik et al. 2006). It teaches police to question juveniles and adults similarly—“principles discussed with respect to adult suspects are just as applicable for use with younger ones” (Reference InbauInbau et al. 2004:298).

Interrogation protocols in the United Kingdom are less confrontational and designed to elicit information rather than to secure a confession (Reference Bull, Milne and GlaserBull & Milne 2004; Reference Milne and BullMilne & Bull 1999). In England and Wales, the Police and Criminal Evidence Act (PACE 1984) has required police to record interrogations for nearly two decades (Reference Bull, Soukara, Lassiter and MeissnerBull & Soukara 2010; Reference Milne and BullMilne & Bull 1999). Police, psychologists, and lawyers collaborated to develop an information-gathering method of interviewing that avoids the more confrontational aspects of the Reid approach (Reference GudjonssonGudjonsson 2003; Reference Milne and BullMilne & Bull 1999). The mnemonic PEACE describes the five components of this interview approach—“Planning and Preparation,” “Engage and Explain,” “Account,” “Closure,” and “Evaluate” (Reference Milne and BullMilne & Bull 1999). Minnesota interrogation practices reflect both Reid and PEACE elements (Reference NelsonNelson 2006).

In the decades since Miranda, psychologists, criminologists, and legal scholars have conducted few studies of how police question people (Reference LeoLeo 2008). Post-Miranda research in the late-1960s evaluated whether police warned suspects, how warnings affected their ability to obtain confessions, and reported minimal changes in interrogation practices or outcomes (Reference FeldFeld 2006a; Reference LeoLeo 1996a, Reference Leo1996c; Reference WaldWald et al. 1967). In the mid-1990s, Richard Leo (Reference Leo1996b, Reference Leo1996c) conducted the only field study of interrogation in the United States. Legal scholars and criminologists have used indirect methods and studied tapes and transcripts of interrogations (Reference FeldFeld 2006a, Reference Feld2006b; Reference King and SnookKing & Snook 2009), or attended prosecutors' charging sessions and interviewed police about interrogations (Reference Cassell and HaymanCassell & Hayman 1996). In England and Wales, analyses of PACE recordings have generated a substantial body of empirical research (Reference Bull, Soukara, Lassiter and MeissnerBull & Soukara 2010; Reference GudjonssonGudjonsson 2003; Reference Milne and BullMilne & Bull 1999). Psychologist Saul Kassin and associates have conducted laboratory research on interrogation for decades (Reference KassinKassin 2005; Reference KassinKassin et al. 2010; Reference Kassin and GudjonssonKassin & Gudjonsson 2004). Studies of false confessions provide another glimpse into how police interrogate suspects and highlight the vulnerability of youths (Reference Drizin and LeoDrizin & Leo 2004; Garrett 2011).

Methodology and Data

The Minnesota Supreme Court in State v. Scales (1994) required police to record custodial interrogation of all criminal suspects, including juveniles. Delinquency trials of sixteen- and seventeen-year-old youths charged with felony offenses are public proceedings, which obviated some confidentiality concerns (Minn. Stat. Ann. 2005). County attorneys in Minnesota's four largest counties—Anoka, Dakota, Hennepin (Minneapolis), and Ramsey (St. Paul)—allowed me to search their closed files of sixteen- and seventeen-year-old youths charged with a felony and to copy those in which police interrogated or juveniles invoked Miranda. Police conducted these interviews between 2003 and 2006. These four most populous of Minnesota's eight-seven counties account for almost half (47.6 percent) of the state's population and nearly half (45.6 percent) of the delinquency petitions filed. Prosecutors charged about one-quarter of urban delinquents and one-fifth of suburban delinquents with felony-level offenses (Reference Feld and SchaeferFeld & Schaefer 2010b). I identified, copied, and coded three hundred and seven (307) files in which juveniles invoked or waived Miranda. I obtained sixty-three (20.5 percent) interrogation files in Anoka County, eighty (26.1 percent) in Dakota County, ninety-eight (31.9 percent), in Hennepin County, and sixty-six (21.5 percent) in Ramsey County. The two urban counties accounted for somewhat more than half (53.4 percent) the files. These files contained Scales interrogation recordings or transcripts, police reports, juvenile court records, and sentences. Court Orders authorized access to juvenile courts files, but they included confidentiality stipulations to protect juveniles' identity and imposed methodological limitations.Footnote 1 The University of Minnesota Institutional Review Board (IRB) approved the study.

I reviewed police reports to learn about the crime, the context of interrogation, and evidence police possessed when they questioned a suspect. I obtained, modified, and expanded codebooks used in prior interrogation research (Reference LeoLeo 1996b; Reference Pearse, Gudjonsson and GudjonssonPearse & Gudjonsson 2003; Reference WaldWald et al. 1967).Footnote 2 I coded each file to analyze where, when, and who was present at an interrogation, how police administered Miranda, whether juveniles invoked or waived, whether officers used Reid Method maximization and minimization techniques, and how juveniles responded to their interrogators. The 307 files reflect some sample selection bias because they are charged cases involving serious delinquents, more likely to go to trial, and perhaps include a larger proportion of juveniles who waived Miranda.Footnote 3 Despite these caveats, the study includes a range of serious crimes and analyzes the largest number of routine felony interrogations in the United States. More than 150 officers from more than 50 agencies interviewed these suspects. I conducted saturation interviews with police, prosecutors, defense lawyers, and juvenile court judges to elicit their views, learn from their experience, and validate my findings.Footnote 4

Police Interrogation

These analyses focus on several aspects of what happens in the interrogation room. I examine characteristics of youths who waived or invoked Miranda. I analyze how police secured Miranda waivers and questioned the vast majority of youths. I focus on how long police questioned them and the outcomes of interrogations.

Sample Characteristics

As indicated in Table 1, males comprised the vast majority (89.3 percent) of the 307 youths whom police questioned. Prosecutors charged more than half (55.0 percent) with property offenses—burglary, larceny, and auto-theft. They charged nearly one-third (31.6 percent) of youths with crimes against person—murder, armed robbery, aggravated assault, and criminal sexual conduct. They charged the remaining youths with drug crimes (6.2 percent), firearm offenses (5.5 percent), and other felonies (1.6 percent). Prosecutors charged more than half (56.4 percent) with only one felony, an additional quarter (25.1 percent) with two crimes, and the remainder with three or more. The group lacks some of the most serious offenders because prosecutors filed certification motions and juvenile court judges transferred them to criminal court.

Table 1. Characteristics of Juveniles Interrogated

a Crimes against property include: burglary, theft of a motor vehicle, arson, receiving stolen property, possession of stolen property, possession of burglary tools, criminal damage to property, theft, forgery, theft by swindle, and credit card fraud.

b Crimes against the person include: aggravated and simple robbery, aggravated assault, murder and attempted murder, criminal vehicular homicide, criminal sexual conduct, and terroristic threats.

c Drug crimes include: sale or possession of a controlled substance—crack, methamphetamine, marijuana, codeine, ecstasy, heroin—possession of a forged prescription, and tampering with anhydrous ammonia equipment (methamphetamine).

d Firearm crimes include: possession of a firearm, discharge of a firearm, theft of a firearm, possession of an explosive device, and drive-by shooting.

e Other offenses are fleeing a police officer.

Nearly one-third (30.6 percent) of juveniles had no prior arrests. Police previously had taken into custody more than one-third of these youths for non-criminal status offenses (15.3 percent) or misdemeanors (22.8 percent). About one-third of these youths (35.1 percent) had one or more prior felony arrests and more than half (57 percent) had prior juvenile court referrals. Nearly one-third (29.9 percent) were under court supervision—probation, placement, or parole status—when police questioned them. About half of the youths were white (52.1 percent) and the remainder (47.9 percent) members of ethnic and racial minority groups—Black, Hispanic, Native American, and Asian. Black juveniles accounted for more than one-third (34.9 percent) of the sample. Compared with these counties' 16- and 17-year-old felony caseloads, the interrogation group included a larger proportion of males, more youths charged with property and violent crimes and with prior court referrals, and fewer charged with drug offenses (Reference Feld and SchaeferFeld & Schaefer 2010a, Reference Feld and Schaefer2010b).

Securing Miranda Waivers

When police take suspects into custody and interrogate them, Miranda requires officers to warn them to dispel the inherent coercion of isolation and questioning. Police had formally arrested the vast majority (86.6 percent) of these juveniles prior to questioning. They made a Scales recording of each interrogation whether they initially arrested or later released a youth. Police detained nearly two-thirds (61.7 percent) of those whom they questioned and released the others to parents. More than half (55.7 percent) of interrogations took place in police stations. Another quarter (23.1 percent) occurred at juvenile detention centers. Thus, police questioned more than three-quarters (78.8 percent) of youths in interrogation rooms. Nearly one-tenth (8.1 percent) of interrogations took place in a police car at the place of arrest. Police conducted 6.2 percent of interrogations at juveniles' homes and another 6.2 percent in schools. Every juvenile in the sample received a proper Miranda warning and one-fifth (19.5 percent) of the files contained an initialed and signed warning form.

Although Miranda requires police to warn suspects, officers' goal to solve crimes provides no incentive to encourage them to invoke their rights. This inherent contradiction requires officers to engage in a quasi-confidence game—“systematic use of deception, manipulation, and the betrayal of trust in the process of eliciting a suspect's confession” (Reference LeoLeo 1996b:259). Police used several tactics to predispose suspects to waive Miranda without alerting them to its significance—admonishing them to tell the truth, minimizing the warning, or advising that it is the only opportunity to tell their story (Reference LeoLeo 1996b; Reference Leo and WhiteLeo and White 1999). They also may ask routine booking questions before they issue a warning, during which time they may establish rapport and predispose youths to waive (Pennsylvania v. Muniz 1990; Rhode Island v. Innis 1980; Reference WeisselbergWeisselberg 2008).

In about half of cases (52.8 percent), police gave the Miranda warning immediately after identifying the suspect. In the other half of cases (47.2 percent), police asked juveniles booking questions—name, age and date of birth, address and telephone number, grade in school, and the like—and sometimes used juveniles' responses to engage in casual conversations, to put youths at ease, and to accustom them to answering questions.

Police predispose youths to waive by emphasizing the importance to tell the truth, by nodding while reading the warning to cue the suspect to agree, or by telling the person that the interview constitutes his only opportunity to tell his story (Reference Redlich, Drizin, Kessler and KrausRedlich & Drizin 2007). Training manuals instruct police to blend the warning into the conversation, to describe it as a formality, or to summarize evidence, which a suspect can explain only if he waives (Reference WeisselbergWeisselberg 2008).

Police sometimes framed a Miranda waiver as a prerequisite to a juvenile's opportunity to tell his side of the story. Police communicated the value of talking—“telling her story”—and telling the truth before they gave a Miranda warning. Officers characterized the warning as an administrative formality to complete before the suspect can talk. Officers sometimes referred to the warning as “paper-work” to emphasize its bureaucratic quality or as a ritual with which to comply. A waiver form provides another opportunity to convert Miranda into a bureaucratic exercise.

Dickerson v. United States noted that Miranda warnings “have become part of our national culture” (Dickerson v. United States 2000: 430). Police invoke its cultural pervasiveness to minimize the warnings by “referring to their dissemination in popular American television shows and cinema, perhaps joking that the suspect is already well aware of his rights and probable can recite them from memory” (Reference Leo and WhiteLeo & White 1999:434–35). Officers regularly referred to youths' familiarity with Miranda from television and movies. Miranda's cultural ubiquity may detract from youths' understanding, as the warning becomes background noise at an interrogation.

After police warn a suspect, he or she must either waive or clearly invoke the rights to silence and to counsel (Berghuis v. Thompkins 2010; Davis v. United States 1994; Fare v. Michael C. 1979). Police establish that a juvenile understands his or her rights by reading the warning and then eliciting an affirmative response. In this study, officers read each right to the youth followed by the question “Do you understand that?” Juveniles acknowledged receiving each warning on-the-record—the Scales tape—and, in some departments, initialed and signed a Miranda form. Police in this study consistently obtained express waivers. After they ascertained juveniles understood the warning, they concluded the waiver process, “Bearing in mind that I'm a police officer and I've just read your rights, are you willing to talk to me about this matter?” Another version of the waiver formula ended, “Having these rights in mind, do you wish to talk to us now?”

Miranda reasoned that police must warn a suspect to dispel the inherent coercion of custodial interrogation. Justice White's Miranda dissent asked why those compulsive pressures do not coerce a waiver as readily as an unwarned statement (Miranda v. Arizona 1966). Legal analysts and criminologists concur that after police isolate a suspect in a police-dominated environment, a warning cannot adequately empower them to invoke their rights (Reference WeisselbergWeisselberg 2008; Reference WhiteWhite 1997). Post-Miranda studies consistently report about 80 percent or more of adults waive Miranda (Reference Cassell and HaymanCassell & Hayman 1996; Reference KassinKassin et al. 2007; Reference LeoLeo 1996b, Reference Leo2008; Reference WaldWald et al. 1967).

Juveniles waive Miranda rights at somewhat higher rates than do adults. Three decades of research reports that more than 90 percent of juveniles waive Miranda rights (Reference Goldstein and GoldsteinGoldstein & Goldstein 2010; Reference GrissoGrisso 1980; Reference Grisso and PomiciterGrisso & Pomiciter 1977). Juveniles' higher waiver rates may reflect their lack of understanding or inability to invoke Miranda effectively. Equally plausible, waivers may reflect prior justice system involvement and juveniles will have had less experience than adults (Reference Viljoen and RoeschViljoen & Roesch 2005). Table 2 reports that the vast majority of youths (92.8 percent) waived Miranda. This high rate is consistent with other juvenile studies and ten-percent higher than rates reported for adults. Interviews with justice system personnel confirmed the accuracy of these findings—almost all delinquents waived Miranda.Footnote 5

Table 2. Juveniles Who Waive or Invoke by Offense and Prior RecordFootnote *

* Statistically Significant at: χ2(1, N = 300) = 5.7, P < 0.05.

a Seven juveniles (2.3%) initially waived their Miranda rights and subsequently invoked them during interrogation, at which point interrogation ceased. Because they were truncated interrogation, I exclude them from analyses of police interrogation tactics.

Fare v. Michael C. (1979) cited his prior experience with police when it found a valid waiver. Analysts report a relationship between prior police contacts and Miranda invocations (Reference KassinKassin 2005; Reference LeoLeo 1996b). Post-Miranda research reported that defendants with prior arrests and felony convictions gave fewer confessions than did those with less experience. Older youths and those with prior felony referrals invoked more frequently than did younger juveniles and those without prior contacts (Reference GrissoGrisso 1980; Reference Grisso and PomiciterGrisso & Pomiciter 1977).

About one-third (35.1 percent) of these youths had one or more felony arrests prior to the offense for which police questioned them. Juveniles with one or more prior felony arrests waived their rights at significantly lower rates (86.9 percent) than did those with fewer or less serious police contacts (94.9 percent). Several factors likely contribute to more invocation by those with more extensive police contacts. Youths who waived at prior interrogations may have learned that confessing redounds to their disadvantage. The amount of time youths spend with lawyers contributes to greater understanding of rights, and those with prior arrests have more learning opportunities. Youths questioned previously may have learned to cope with and resist the pressures of interrogation.

Maximization and Minimization Interrogation Tactics

Police question suspects to obtain incriminating admissions or leads to other evidence—physical evidence, other participants, witnesses, or stolen property—which strengthen prosecutors' cases and facilitate guilty pleas. They seek suspects' statements—true or false—to pin them down, to control changes they later make in their stories, and to impeach their credibility. Police often described their roles to the two hundred eighty five juveniles who waived Miranda as dispassionate fact-finders. Minnesota training advises officers to portray themselves as neutral report writers who want to learn what happened to put in a statement for prosecutors and judges to evaluate (Reference NelsonNelson 2006). They frequently advise suspects that the interview provides their opportunity to “tell their story.” Depending on how forthcoming a youth is initially, they may use maximization and minimization tactics to elicit a statement.

Detectives may overstate a crime's seriousness, confront suspects with real or false evidence, accuse them of lying, challenge inconsistencies, emphasize the implausibility of their stories, and describe the negative impact that false statements would have on prosecutors and judges. The Reid Method instructs police to ask emotionally-charged Behavioral Analysis Interview (BAI) questions early in a suspect's interview to provoke a reaction (Reference InbauInbau et al. 2004). Although Reid advises officers to use BAI questions at a preliminary interview to screen the likely innocent from the probably guilty, few of these files indicated that police had any conversations prior to Scales recordings and none in which they interrogated these youths.

Police reported that they used maximization techniques regularly. They initially encouraged a suspect to commit to a story—true or false—and then used more confrontational tactics to challenge her version thereafter. Table 3 summarizes maximization strategies police used: confronted juveniles with evidence (54.4 percent); accused them of lying (32.6 percent); exhorted them to tell the truth (29.5 percent); asked BAI questions (28.8 percent); challenged inconsistencies (20.0 percent); emphasized the seriousness of the offense (14.4 percent); and accused them of other crimes (8.4 percent).

Table 3. Maximization Questions: Types and Frequency

In nearly one-third (30.9 percent) of interviews, police did not use any maximization techniques. In another quarter (23.1 percent) of interrogations, they used only one, which suggests that most juveniles did not require a lot of persuasion or intimidation to cooperate. Police used three or more maximization tactics in fewer than one-third (31.6 percent) of cases.

In about half (54.4 percent) the interrogations, police confronted juveniles with statements from witnesses or co-offenders, or referred to physical evidence. In most cases, DNA, surveillance, or fingerprint evidence will not be available in the short time between a suspect's arrest and interrogation. Sometimes, police described an investigation as if they already had obtained the evidence. In other instances, they questioned youths about potential evidence that later investigation would reveal. They asked a juvenile how he would respond to hypothetical evidence—“what if I told you” that someone had identified him or police found his fingerprints? In another version, officers might ask a juvenile “is there any reason why” his DNA might be on a gun or he would appear on surveillance video?

In about one-third (32.6 percent) of cases, officers accused juveniles of lying. Police typically allowed juveniles to commit to a story and then confronted them. In nearly one-third of cases (29.5 percent), officers urged juveniles to be honest and tell the truth.

Officers reaffirmed their roles as objective fact-gatherers and neutral conduits who would accurately convey juveniles' statements to prosecutors and judges. Police intimated that their recommendations could affect prosecutors' charge evaluations and judges' decisions. They cautioned that prosecutors and judges reacted negatively to an implausible story and predicted that judges responded more favorably to truthful defendants.

Inbau and Reid advise interrogators at a preliminary interview to ask “behavior-provoking questions that are specifically designed to evoke behavioral responses” (2004:173). They posit that innocent and guilty people respond differently to emotionally provocative questions and enable investigators accurately to classify them. Leo (Reference Leo1996b) reported that officers asked BAI questions in about 40 percent of interrogations. In this study, police used BAI questions in more than one-quarter (28.8 percent) of interviews, most commonly “Do you know why I have asked to talk to you here today?”

In one-fifth (20 percent) of cases, officers confronted and challenged suspects' assertions. They pointed out inconsistencies, disputed claims, and questioned youths' credibility to increase anxiety and undermined confidence. Officers regularly responded to juveniles' claims of innocence with a barnyard epithet—“Bullshit.”

Many interviews began with an invitation to a youth to tell his story. But, police warned that it was a time-limited opportunity. Officers cautioned that if youth did not take advantage of this chance to explain their involvement, then they might regret it later. Police withheld information from juveniles about the investigation to increase uncertainty and anxiety and cautioned a reluctant youth that without his version, other co-offenders might shift responsibility to him or make a deal at his expense.

Although police contamination—disclosure of information known only to police or a true perpetrator about the crime and later incorporated by a suspect—is a recurring theme in studies of false confessions (Reference GarrettGarrett 2010, 2011; Reference LeoLeo 2008), contamination appears unlikely in these interrogations. As will be seen, interrogations were surprisingly brief, most youths confessed or made admissions at the outset, and officers confronted youths with any evidence in only half (54.4 percent) the cases.

Minimization tactics offer face-saving excuses or moral justifications that reduce a crime's seriousness, provide a less odious motivation, or shift blame to a victim or accomplice (Reference KassinKassin et al. 2010). Themes imply a suspect will feel better or will derive benefit if he confesses (Reference LeoLeo 2008). Table 4 reports that police used minimization tactics in fewer than one-fifth (17.3 percent) of these interrogations, far less often than they used any maximization tactics (69.1 percent). Although prosecutors charged all these youths with felonies, one officer explained that “most of these are fairly minor, so you don't have to do a whole lot of minimizing.” Officers used scenarios or themes to reduce suspects' guilt or culpability in 15.4 percent of cases; appealed to self-interest in one-tenth (11.9 percent) of cases; expressed empathy in one-tenth of cases (10.5 percent); and used other tactics in a few cases. The relative paucity of minimization tactics is consistent with research in the United Kingdom, and Minnesota training that discourages their use (Reference SoukaraSoukara et al. 2009; Reference NelsonNelson 2006).

Table 4. Minimization Questions: Types and Frequencies

The Reid Method teaches police to develop a theme or scenario to neutralize guilt, minimize responsibility, and make it easier to confess (Reference InbauInbau et al. 2004). Criminologists have used techniques of neutralization to understand how youths rationalize delinquent behavior (Reference Sykes and MatzaSykes & Matza 1957). Many themes are extensions of criminal law defenses—provocation, intoxication, or insanity—that provide rationales to reduce moral constraints (Reference MatzaMatza 1964). For example, delinquents may reject mental illness—insanity—as an excuse, but embrace the idea of “going crazy” or “being mad” to rationalize criminal conduct. Police sometimes suggested that getting mad, losing control, or excitement accounted for youths' criminal misconduct. Intoxication provides an explanation for bad behavior, and juveniles readily invoked drinking alcohol or using drugs to excuse criminal conduct.

Police diffused juveniles' responsibility by suggesting that they succumbed to negative peer influences. Juveniles often commit their crimes in groups (Reference Snyder and SickmundSnyder & Sickmund 2006), and police can blame others and allow juveniles to shift blame as well. Parents regularly refer to errant children's behavior as a mistake and youths learn that mistakes can mitigate responsibility. Police regularly encouraged juveniles to attribute their delinquency to a mistake.

Police described benefits juveniles might derive and appealed to self-interest in one-tenth (11.9 percent) of cases. They offered to investigate further and assist juveniles to receive help. They intimated that prosecutors and judges would view more favorably youths who confessed than those who lied and might deal with them more leniently.

Officers minimized seriousness by describing the triviality of a youth's crime compared with the gravity of other delinquents' offenses. Even a serious crime—a drive-by shooting—could have been worse if the shooter had hit the intended target. The rationale of juvenile courts—treatment rather than punishment—provided officers with another theme with which to offer help and to minimize seriousness.

Juveniles' Responses

I examined how the 285 youths who waived Miranda responded to police and how their attitudes affected how much information they provided. I classified the outcome of an interrogation based on the evidentiary value of a statement (Reference Cassell and HaymanCassell & Hayman 1996; Reference WaldWald et al. 1967). Table 5 reports outcomes of interrogations which are coded into three categories—confess, admit, or deny.Footnote 6

Table 5. Outcome of Interrogation and Youths' Attitude

* Statistically Significant at: χ2(1, N = 285) = 7.84, P < 0.001.

A majority (58.6 percent) of juveniles confessed within a few minutes of waiving Miranda and did not require prompting by police. British research confirms that the majority of suspects confessed and “almost all did so near the beginning of the interviews” (Soukara et al. Reference Soukara2009:495). UK analysts conclude that “suspects enter a police interview having already decided whether to admit or deny the allegations against them” and interrogation tactics have little impact on whether they admit (Milne & Bull Reference Milne and Bull1999:81).

An additional one-third (29.8 percent) of juveniles provided statements of some evidentiary value, for example, admitting that they served as a look-out during a robbery or participated in a burglary even if they did not personally steal property. Justice personnel agreed that most juveniles made some incrimination admissions.

Other studies corroborate similar high rates of admissions and confessions. Leo (Reference Leo1996b) found such outcomes in three-quarters (76 percent) of cases in which adults waived Miranda. The Yale-New Haven study reported that about two-thirds (64 percent) of interrogations produced incriminating evidence (Wald et al. Reference Wald1967). A survey of police investigators estimated that two-thirds (68 percent) of suspects made incriminating statements (Kassin et al. Reference Kassin2007). Other UK research reports a rate of 77 percent, ranging from 64 percent to 97 percent among various police stations (Bull & Milne Reference Bull, Milne and Glaser2004; Evans Reference Evans1993). More than half (55 percent) of delinquents held in detention reported they had confessed (Viljoen et al. Reference Viljoen2005).

Only a small proportion (11.6 percent) of juveniles made no incriminating admissions. Without invoking Miranda outright, forms of resistance included non-cooperation, denial of knowledge and culpability, lying, evasion, silence, or blame shifting. When confronted with resistance, police used more maximization techniques than they did with cooperative youths, but did not question them for longer periods. Once they recognized a youth was resistant, they concluded the interview with the observation that prosecutors and judges who reviewed their interrogation would not view them favorably.

Criminologists have studied the interplay between police discretion and juveniles' attitudes (Clarke & Sykes Reference Clarke, Sykes and Glaser1974). For less serious crimes, deferential youths reduce likelihood of arrest and contumacious ones increase it (LaFave Reference LaFave1965; Skolnick Reference Skolnick1967; Pilliavin & Briar Reference Pilliavin and Briar1964; Bittner Reference Bittner and Rosenheim1976). Studies of police and probation officers report that a youth's attitude affected how officials perceived, imputed moral character, and responded to them (Cicourel Reference Cicourel1995; Emerson Reference Emerson and Glaser1974). When youths' attitudes affect police decisions, minority youths typically fare worse than do their white counterparts (Bittner Reference Bittner and Rosenheim1976; Black & Reiss Reference Black and Reiss1970).

Police reported that juveniles' attitudes ranged the gamut—“some are scared to death, and others, it's almost a joke.” Many officers described youths as scared, especially “the kids that are new to the process.” Although police described some youths as confrontational, justice system personnel viewed most youths as compliant or submissive. “I would say that 90 percent or more would probably be cooperative and the other percentage would be the frequent-fliers so to speak.” Several officers used the same expression—“deer in the headlights”—to describe youths' demeanor in the interrogation room. Public defenders described most juveniles as humbled or defeated when they confessed.

Ethnographers emphasize the importance of attitude—“rude or impolite, aggressive or passive, laughter or tears, and the like”—and its impact on justice system processing (Cicourel Reference Cicourel1995:xv). Juveniles exhibited many attitudes during interrogation—polite, cooperative, distressed, remorseful, frightened, cocky, resistant, aggressive, and confrontational—which could fluctuate from one minute to the next.

Police reports frequently included comments about juveniles' demeanor and behavior during interrogation. They documented whether they believed suspects told the truth or lied and indicated whether they cooperated or resisted. Officers often described youths' emotional or behavioral responses to their interrogators. Based on my impressions and officers' reports, I dichotomized attitude as cooperative or resistant. Other research used similar categories and described eighty percent of suspects as cooperative (Baldwin Reference Baldwin1993). Juveniles cooperate for many reasons—human decency in social interactions, fear and anxiety, dependency on authority figures, or the coercive pressures of isolation—but most exhibited positive attitudes.

As Table 5 indicates, the vast majority of juveniles (79.6 percent) exhibited a cooperative demeanor and only one-fifth (20.4 percent) appeared resistant. Not surprisingly, the vast majority (96.5 percent) of cooperative juveniles confessed or made incriminating admissions. By contrast, fewer than one-in-ten (8.5 percent) resistant juveniles confessed and almost half (43.1 percent) provided no useful admissions. Only one-tenth (11.6 percent) of youths denied involvement, but those who exhibited resistant attitudes accounted for more than three-quarters of them (75.8 percent).

Police question suspects to elicit admissions or obtain statements that prosecutors can use to impeach testimony. Suspects' answers may lead to other evidence—witnesses, co-defendants, or property. Table 5 reports the proportion of cases in which interrogations yielded corroborating evidence. I defined corroborating evidence as evidence which police did not possess prior to questioning—leads to physical evidence, a crime scene diagram, identity of a co-offender, or unknown witness. By this conservative standard, fewer than one-fifth (18.2 percent) of interviews yielded information that police did not already have. Interrogation did not produce much collateral evidence and gathering it appears to be a secondary goal.

Some police attributed the relatively low-yield of corroborating evidence to time pressure and volume of cases under which they labored. Once police obtained an admission—which they did quickly—they did not press youths for additional evidence. Prosecutors confirmed that interrogations did not often lead to corroborating evidence, but they attributed that to good preliminary investigations. Police questioned more than two-thirds (69.7 percent) of juveniles within less than 24-hours of their crimes—effectively, they were “caught in the act.” Police and prosecutors had strong enough evidence with which to convict youths without an interview in about two-thirds (63.2 percent) of cases. Police and prosecutors said that juveniles' statements often provided bases to obtain search warrants, which produced additional evidence not disclosed by the interview.

Length of Interrogation

It can take a long time and rigorous questioning to elicit a false confession. Although police may obtain some false confessions within an hour or two, they elicited eighty-five percent of false confessions after suspects had been in custody or interrogated for six hours or longer (Drizin & Leo Reference Drizin and Leo2004). Table 6 reports the length of interrogations, length of time by type of offense, and length of time by whether the offense involved a firearm.Footnote 7 Routine felony interrogations are brief. Police completed three-quarters (77.2 percent) of interviews in less than fifteen minutes and concluded nine-in-ten (90.5 percent) in less than thirty minutes. In the longest interviews, police questioned three youths (1.1 percent) for more than one and one-half hours. Although prosecutors charged youths with one or more felonies, brief interrogations are unlikely to cause false confessions (White Reference White2001).

Table 6. Length of Interrogation by Type of OffenseFootnote * and WeaponFootnote **

* Statistically Significant at: χ2(1, N = 285) = 32.3, P < 0.05.

** Statistically Significant at: χ2(1, N = 285) = 9.4, P < 0.01.

Although these short interviews initially seemed surprising, other research confirms that interrogations of even two or three hours are exceptional and frequently problematic (Drizin & Leo Reference Drizin and Leo2004; Kassin et al. Reference Kassin2007). The Yale-New Haven study reported that police questioned suspects for more than an hour in only 15 percent of cases (Wald et al. Reference Wald1967). Leo (Reference Leo1996c) reported that police questioned only one-quarter (28.7 percent) of suspects for more than one hour. Cassell and Hayman (Reference Cassell and Hayman1996) reported that only 13 percent of interrogations took more than 30 minutes and only one lasted longer than an hour. Research on British interrogations of juveniles reported that “[i]nterviews tended to be very brief with the majority taking less than fifteen minutes (71.4 percent). Although the average length of interviews was around 14 minutes, the most frequent length was around 7 minutes” (Evans Reference Evans1993:26). Analyses of taped UK interrogations reported that “most were short and surprisingly amiable discussions” in which more than one-third of suspects confessed at the outset (Baldwin Reference Baldwin1993:331). Kassin and Gudjonsson (Reference Kassin and Gudjonsson2004:46) summarized research and reported that “[m]ost of the interviews were short (80 percent lasted less than 30 minutes; 95 percent were completed within 1 hour), the confession rate was 58 percent, little interrogative pressure was applied, and very few suspects who initially denied guilt eventually confessed.” Inbau et al. (Reference Inbau2004) warn against interrogations that last longer than four hours, a duration substantially longer than observed in any research. By contrast, police extracted most false confessions only after interrogations of six hours or longer (Drizin & Leo Reference Drizin and Leo2004).

I asked justice professionals to estimate the lengths of interviews and they universally agreed, “They're actually very short.”Footnote 8 When asked why police concluded felony interrogations so quickly, justice system personnel attributed brevity to several factors. Many professionals referred to police workload pressures. Police conducted a form of triage and questioned suspects longer in more serious cases, but did not regard most juvenile felonies as serious crimes. Several officers attributed brief interrogations to the relative simplicity of most youth crime and their ability to elicit admissions quickly.

In this study, a statistically significant relationship appeared between length of interrogation and type of offense. Police questioned more youths charged with property and drug crimes for fifteen minutes or less than they did youths charged with other types of offenses. Crimes that involved some physical evidence—drugs, stolen property, or automobiles—may have provided police with more evidence with which to confront these juveniles.

Cases involving firearms resulted in longer interrogations. Although police questioned only 9.5 percent of suspects for longer than thirty minutes, they interrogated twice as many (20 percent) juveniles charged with firearms offenses for longer than thirty minutes. I compared the lengths of interrogation in all cases that involved guns—armed robbery, assault with a gun, firearms possession, or burglary in which youths stole guns—with cases in which juveniles used other weapons—knives, blunt instruments, or automobiles—or did not use a weapon.

Guns provide an indicator of offense seriousness (Reference Podkopacz and FeldPodkopacz & Feld 1996), and police questioned these juveniles longer and more aggressively. Guns affected the tactics as well as the length of interrogations. Police wanted to recover guns used or stolen by youths, and they used maximization and minimization tactics more extensively to retrieve them. Officers referred to the benefits that would accrue to a youth who helped to recover a gun and described the dangers guns posed to people who held them and those around them. Only two interrogations in this study raised constitutional issues of voluntariness and both involved questioning to recover guns. In each case, police questioned juveniles for the longest time (one and one-half to two hours), used the most maximization techniques, and made explicit quid-pro-quo promises of leniency to recover guns used or stolen by juveniles.

Police and justice system personnel confirmed the relationship between guns and length of interrogation and agreed that guns provide a proxy for seriousness. Police associated guns with youths' involvement with gangs—another indicator of seriousness. Police questioned youths to recover the gun and learn about other youths who had contact with the weapon. Youths knew that gun crimes garnered serious consequences, raised the stakes, and gave them greater incentive to resist interrogators. Serious crimes are more likely to go to trial and police invested more energy to strengthen prosecutors' cases.

Policy Implications

Theoretically, defendants enjoy the protections of the Due Process model—an adversarial system—in which they may invoke procedural safeguards and force the state to prove its case (Reference PackerPacker 1968). In reality, the justice system more closely resembles the Crime Control model—an inquisitorial system—in which confessions lead to guilty pleas (Reference PackerPacker 1968). A confession tilts the balance of advantage to the state (Reference LeoLeo 2009). Adults who confess seldom have a jury trial and receive fewer plea concessions than do those who remained silent (Reference NeubauerNeubauer 1974). Prosecutors charge those who confess with a greater number and more serious crimes (Reference CassellCassell 1996), set higher bail, and offer fewer charge reductions (Reference Cassell and HaymanCassell & Hayman 1996; Reference Ofshe and LeoOfshe & Leo 1997). Reduced negotiating leverage impels defense attorneys to pressure clients who confessed to accept guilty pleas to avoid harsher sentences (Reference KassinKassin et al. 2010; Reference NardulliNardulli et al. 1988).

Scales's requirement to record interrogations affected filing of motions to suppress evidence. Some attribute the paucity of suppression motions to defense lawyers' heavy caseloads, lack of resources, and courtroom cultures hostile to adversarial litigation (Reference PuritzPuritz et al. 1995; Reference Goldstein and GoldsteinGoldstein & Goldstein 2010). Even when defense counsel filed motions to suppress confessions, judges rarely excluded statements at suppression hearings or on appeal (Reference CassellCassell 1996; Reference Goldstein and GoldsteinGoldstein & Goldstein 2010).

More significantly than public defenders' caseload pressures, Scales recordings virtually have eliminated suppression motions to challenge juveniles' statements. Interviews with prosecutors, defense attorneys, and judges confirmed that defenders filed few motions to suppress evidence for Miranda violations. Even when defense counsel file suppression motions, Scales recordings obviate hearings.

Justice system personnel attributed Scales's reduction of suppression motions to several factors. First, police acted professionally and complied with Miranda's protocol—there is no ambiguity about warnings and waivers. In addition, most juveniles confess and tapes provide unimpeachable evidence of their statements. Juveniles' statements limited defense attorneys' options and re-enforce a system of plea bargains, rather than trials. Scales enhanced police professionalism, documented Miranda compliance, obviated suppression hearings, led quickly to guilty pleas, and focused lawyers' attention on appropriate sentences rather than guilt or innocence. Scales enables professionals to administer an inquisitorial model of justice “on the record,” expedites processing of routine cases, and reserves court resources for complex cases.

Protecting Young Offenders in the Interrogation Room

Although Miranda purported to bolster the adversary system and protect citizens, warnings failed to achieve those goals. Decisions since Miranda have limited its scope and applicability and adverse consequences when police fail to comply (Reference SloboginSlobogin 2007; Reference WeisselbergWeisselberg 2008). Miranda's assumption that a warning would enable suspects to resist the compulsive pressures of interrogation is demonstrably wrong. Post-Miranda research reports that the vast majority of suspects waive and only some sophisticated suspects invoke. Eighty percent of adults and ninety percent of juveniles waive their sole protection in the interrogation room. Although Miranda recognized that those compulsive pressures threatened the adversarial process, waivers provide police with a window of opportunity to conduct an inquisitorial examination. Perversely, Miranda allows judges to focus on ritualistic compliance with a procedural formality rather than to assess the voluntariness or reliability of a statement (Reference GodseyGodsey 2005; Reference LeoLeo 2008; Reference WeisselbergWeisselberg 2008). Judicial review of a Miranda waiver is the beginning and end of regulating interrogation (Missouri v. Seibert 2004).

Miranda is especially problematic for younger juveniles who may not understand its words or concepts. The Court has recognized youths' vulnerability and distinguished between younger and older youths. Developmental psychologists corroborate their differing abilities. Younger juveniles' incomplete understanding, impaired judgment, and heightened vulnerability warrant greater assistance—a non-waivable right to counsel—to assure voluntariness of a Miranda waiver and statement. Psychologists distinguish between youths' cognitive ability—capacity to understand—and ability to make mature decisions and exercise self-control. Miranda requires only the ability to understand words, which developmental psychologists conclude that most sixteen-and seventeen-year-old youths can do.

This study corroborates that sixteen- and seventeen-year-old juveniles appear to understand and exercise Miranda similarly to adults. This consistency inferentially bolsters research that younger juveniles increasingly lack ability to understand and competence to exercise rights. Psychologists report that many, if not most, children fifteen or younger do not understand Miranda or possess competence to make legal decisions (Reference GrissoGrisso 1980; Reference GrissoGrisso et al. 2003). Research on false confessions underscores the unique vulnerability of younger juveniles (Reference Drizin and LeoDrizin & Leo 2004; Garrett 2011; Reference GrossGross et al. 2005). Police obtained more than one-third (35 percent) of proven false confessions from suspects younger than eighteen (Reference Drizin and LeoDrizin & Leo 2004), and younger adolescents are at greater risk to confess falsely than older ones (Reference TepferTepfer et al. 2010).

Developmental psychologists attribute their over-representation among false confessors to reduced cognitive ability, developmental immaturity, and increased susceptibility to manipulation (Reference Bonnie, Grisso, Grisso and SchwartzBonnie & Grisso 2000; Reference Tobey, Grisso and SchwartzTobey et al. 2000; Reference RedlichRedlich 2004). They have fewer life experiences or psychological resources with which to resist the pressures of interrogation (Reference Drizin and LuloffDrizin & Luloff 2007; Reference RedlichRedlich 2004). Juveniles' lower social status and societal expectations of obedience to authority create pressures to waive (Reference GudjonssonGudjonsson 2003; Reference LeoLeo 2009). Juveniles are more likely than are adults to comply with authority figures, tell police what they think they want to hear, and respond to negative feedback (Reference GudjonssonGudjonsson 2003; Reference LyonLyon 1999). The stress and anxiety of interrogation intensify their desire to extricate themselves in the short-run by waiving and confessing (Reference Goldstein and GoldsteinGoldstein & Goldstein 2010; Reference Owen-KostelnikOwen-Kostelnik et al. 2006). Impulsive decision-making and limited ability to consider long-term consequences heighten their risk (Reference Redlich, Lassiter and MeissnerRedlich 2010). The immature adolescent brain contributes to impulsive behavior and heightened vulnerability (Reference BirckheadBirckhead 2008; Reference MaroneyMaroney 2009).

Despite youths' heightened susceptibility, police do not incorporate developmental differences into the tactics they employ (Reference Owen-KostelnikOwen-Kostelnik et al. 2006). Techniques designed to manipulate adults—aggressive questioning, presenting false evidence, and leading questions—may create unique dangers when employed with youths (Reference Kaban and TobeyKaban & Tobey 1999; Reference Redlich, Drizin, Kessler and KrausRedlich & Drizin 2007; Reference Tanenhaus and DrizinTanenhaus & Drizin 2002). Police in this study did not report receiving special training to question juveniles and used the same tactics that Leo (Reference Leo1996b) reported they employed with adults.

The Court in Haley, Gallegos, Gault, Fare, Alvarado, and J.D.B. excluded statements taken from youths fifteen years of age or younger and admitted those obtained from sixteen- and seventeen-year-olds. The Court's de facto line—fifteen and younger versus sixteen and older—closely tracks what psychologists have found about youths' ability to understand the warning and concepts. State courts and legislatures should formally adopt the functional line that the Court and psychologists discern between youths sixteen and older and those fifteen and younger.

Analysts advocate that juveniles younger than sixteen years of age “should be accompanied and advised by a professional advocate, preferably an attorney, trained to serve in this role” (Reference KassinKassin et al. 2010:28). Juveniles should consult with an attorney, rather than to rely on parents, before they exercise or waive constitutional rights (Reference Bishop and FarberBishop & Farber 2007; Reference FarberFarber 2004). More than three decades ago, the American Bar Association endorsed mandatory, non-waivable counsel because it recognized that “Few juveniles have the experience and understanding to decide meaningfully that the assistance of counsel would not be helpful” (American Bar Association, Institute of Judicial Administration 1980:92).

Requiring a child to consult an attorney assures an informed and voluntary waiver (Reference Drizin and LuloffDrizin & Luloff 2007; Reference FarberFarber 2004). If youths fifteen years of age or younger consult with counsel prior to waiver, it will limit somewhat police's ability to secure confessions. However, if most younger juveniles cannot understand and exercise rights without legal assistance, then to treat them as if they do denies fundamental fairness and enables the state to exploit their vulnerability. Constitutional rights exist to assure factual accuracy, promote equality, and protect individuals from governmental over-reaching, and inevitably diminish the state's ability to fight crime (Escobedo v. Illinois 1964). Fare emphasized lawyers' unique role in the justice system, and Haley, Gallegos, and Gault recognized younger juveniles' exceptional need for assistance.

Limiting the Length of Interrogations

Most false confessions emerge only after lengthy interrogations and youthfulness exacerbates those dangers (Reference Drizin and LeoDrizin & Leo 2004; Reference GrossGross et al. 2005; Reference Kassin and GudjonssonKassin & Gudjonsson 2004). The Court recognizes that lengthy interrogations can produce involuntary confessions (Ashcraft v. Tennessee 1944), and found that questioning juveniles for even five or six hours could produce an involuntary statement (Gallegos v. Colorado 1962; Haley v. Ohio 1948). Policy-makers should create a sliding-scale presumption of involuntariness based on length of interrogation and examine a confession's reliability more closely as length of questioning increases (Reference LeoLeo et al. 2006).

The vast majority of interrogations are very brief. Police concluded ninety percent of these felony interrogations in less than thirty minutes. Every study reviewed reports that police completed nearly all interrogations in less than an hour and few take as long as two hours. By contrast, interrogations that elicit false confessions are usually long inquiries that wear-down an innocent person's resistance—eighty-five percent took at least six hours (Reference Drizin and LeoDrizin & Leo 2004).

This study does not enable me to prescribe outer time limits because I did not encounter either lengthy or factually problematic interrogations. However, states should create a sliding-scale presumption that police elicited an involuntary confession as the length of questioning increases. Police complete nearly all felony interrogations in less than one hour, but extract most false confessions only after grilling suspects for six hours or longer. These times provide a framework to limit interrogations and strengthen the presumption of coercion. The contrast between the short duration of routine questioning and lengthy interrogation associated with false confessions leads analysts to conclude that interrogation should not extend more than four hours (Reference KassinKassin et al. 2010; Reference WhiteWhite 2001). Four hours provides ample opportunity for police to obtain true confessions from guilty suspects willing to talk without coercion without increasing the risk of eliciting false confessions from innocent people.

On the Record

Within the past decade, legal scholars, psychologists, law enforcement, and justice system personnel have reached consensus that recording interrogations reduces coercion, diminishes dangers of false confessions, and increases reliability (Reference CassellCassell 1998; Reference Drizin and ReichDrizin & Reich 2004; Reference GarrettGarrett 2010, 2011; Reference GudjonssonGudjonsson 2003; Reference Milne and BullMilne & Bull 1999; Reference SullivanSullivan 2004, Reference Sullivan2006, Reference Sullivan, Lassiter and Meissner2010). About a dozen states require police to record interrogations, albeit some under limited circumstances—homicide or young suspects (Garrett 2011; Reference LeoLeo 2008; Reference Sullivan, Lassiter and MeissnerSullivan 2010). Many police departments have policies to record interrogations for some crimes (Reference SullivanSullivan 2006, Reference Sullivan, Lassiter and Meissner2010).

Recording creates an objective record and an independent basis to resolve disputes between police and defendants about Miranda warnings, waivers, or statements (Reference SloboginSlobogin 2003). A complete record enables fact-finders to decide whether a statement contains facts known to a guilty perpetrator or police supplied them to an innocent suspect during questioning (Reference GarrettGarrett 2010, 2011; Reference WhiteWhite 1997). Recording protects police from false claims of abuse (Reference CassellCassell 1998; Reference WhiteWhite 1997). It enables police to focus on suspects' responses, to review details of an interview not captured in written notes, and to test them against subsequently discovered facts (Reference Drizin and ReichDrizin & Reich 2004). It reduces the need for an officer to take notes or a second person to witness a statement, which may chill a suspect's willingness to talk. Recording avoids distortions that occur when interviewers rely on memory or notes to summarize a statement (Reference Milne and BullMilne & Bull 1999).

A recorded confession greatly strengthens prosecutors' plea bargain advantage. It enables them to avoid suppression hearings, negotiate better pleas, and obtain convictions (Reference SullivanSullivan 2006; Reference WhiteWhite 1997). Defense lawyers can review recordings rather than rely on clients' imperfect recollection of a stressful event. Scales recordings have virtually eliminated motions to suppress confessions because tapes provide unimpeachable evidence. This generates substantial savings because police, prosecutors, and defense counsel do not have to prepare for and judges do not have to conduct suppression hearings about Miranda warnings, coercive tactics, or the accuracy of a statement.

Police must record all conversations—preliminary interviews and interrogations—rather than just a final statement—a “post-admission narrative” (Reference GudjonssonGarrett 2011; Gudjonsson 2003). Otherwise, police may conduct a pre-interrogation interview, elicit incriminating information, and then record only a final confession after the “cat is out of the bag”—a variation of the two-step practice condemned in Missouri v. Siebert. Only a complete record of every interaction can protect against a final statement that ratifies an earlier coerced one or against a false confession contaminated by non-public facts that police supplied a suspect (Garrett 2011; Reference KassinKassin 1997).

The Court repeatedly insists that American criminal and juvenile justice is an adversary system. Repeated assertions do not alter the reality that states establish most defendants' guilt through an inquisitorial system in which suspects seal their fate in the interrogation room and render trial procedures a nullity—interrogation elicits confessions and confessions produce guilty pleas. Because states do not and need not provide full adversarial testing in every case, we need stronger mechanisms to assure factual reliability of inquisitorial justice. Recording imposes no great burden on police, illuminates the inner-workings of the interrogation room, and provides an objective record on which a defendant may appeal to a judge. Because the vast majority of defendants do not receive a trial, judicial review of the record provides an alternative check to assure the reliability of routine felony justice.

Data Limitations and More Research

This study suffers from several methodological limitations. There is a major problem of sample selection bias. I could not randomly select the files I analyzed from a larger universe of interrogations because such an array of cases simply does not exist. The study includes only juveniles whom prosecutors actually charged with felonies and whose files contained a record of their invocation or interrogation. Because of confidentiality restrictions on data access, I personally coded all of the interrogations and could not use multiple coders or obtain inter-rater reliability scores. The four counties in this study represent about half the population and delinquency petitions in Minnesota. Because Scales had required Minnesota police to record interrogations for more than a decade, police practices probably differ from and may be more benign that those used in other states or for more serious crimes. Despite these caveats, this study represents the largest number of interrogations and of juveniles in the criminological literature. While not necessarily a representative sample, it provides important insights into routine police questioning of older juveniles and a baseline of practice against which to evaluate conditions of interrogation likely to elicit false confessions.

This study is only the second naturalistic empirical study of police interrogation in the United States in the past three decades (Reference LeoLeo 1996b), and the first involving juveniles. We need far more empirical research on interrogations practices in general, in a number of different settings, and with more knowledge about characteristics of suspects. As more jurisdictions adopt taping and recording requirements, we will have further opportunity to conduct this type of research. Recordings provide opportunities for psychologists, criminologists, police, and others to study systematically what actually occurs in the interrogation room. This will increase our fund of knowledge, enable us to develop more effective techniques to elicit true confessions from guilty defendants, reduce the likelihood of extracting false confessions from innocent suspects, and provide a stronger basis for systemic policy prescriptions.

Footnotes

The National Science Foundation #0813807 provided support to code and analyze these data. The editors and anonymous reviewers at LSR provided insightful comments and constructive criticism that helped to improve the final article. Ms. Jamie Buskirk, University of Minnesota Law School, Class of 2013, provided outstanding research assistance.

1. I personally transcribed interrogation tapes and coded all of the files to address county attorneys and juvenile court judges' concerns about data confidentiality. Court-ordered confidentiality restrictions precluded use of multiple coders, so I could not obtain inter-rater reliability scores. Earlier studies of interrogation in the United States did not use multiple-coders or provide inter-rater reliability scores (Reference LeoLeo 1996b; Reference WaldWald et al. 1967).

2. Copies of the 180 variable codebook are available upon request—.

3. The sample includes only juveniles whom prosecutors charged with a felony and for whom an interrogation or invocation record exists. The four counties identified almost 1,400 youths sixteen and seventeen and charged with a felony, but only 307 that reported youths invoked or waived Miranda. Other evidence being equal, prosecutors are more likely to charge suspects who waive than those who invoke Miranda because they have plea bargain advantage. Police made these Scales recordings during custodial interrogation and the files do not include unrecorded, non-custodial interviews. I do not know how the felony cases that prosecutors charged and that contained transcripts differ from those in which juveniles invoked Miranda and police did not forward the cases, cases that prosecutors did not charge, or those that they charged, but which did not contain tapes or transcripts.

4. I conducted structured, open-ended interviews with nineteen (19) police officers, six (6) juvenile prosecutors, nine (9) juvenile defense lawyers, and five (5) juvenile court judges from both urban and suburban counties. The police officers averaged 18.4 years of professional experience; the prosecutors averaged 14.5 years; the public defenders averaged 13.3 years; and the juvenile court judges, 16 years. Four of the five judges presided in urban county juvenile courts. Half of the prosecutors worked in the urban counties and the other half in the suburban counties. Two-thirds of the defense lawyers worked in the urban counties and one-third in the suburban counties. Seven police officers worked in suburban counties and twelve in urban counties. I interviewed sergeants, detectives or investigators, and school resource officers (SROs)—the ranks and specialties that conduct most custodial interrogations of juveniles. The recorded interviews lasted between 30 and 80 minutes, and averaged about 45 minutes. The interviews provide thick descriptions of the process.

I purposively recruited justice system professionals to interview. I called juvenile court judges directly. I recruited prosecutors and defense attorneys through their juvenile division administrators who solicited volunteers. I recruited police in several ways. I contacted police juvenile division administrators who recruited juvenile officer volunteers to interview. In several departments, I used a snowball sampling technique—initial interviewees recruited other officers with relevant background and experience from their own and other departments. In those instances, officers acted as referrals and intermediaries to other officers. I conducted saturation interviews until I reached a point of diminishing returns—no new data, themes, or conceptual relationships emerged.

5. When asked how many juveniles waived Miranda, one officer said, “almost all of them. I couldn't even tell you the last time a kid told me he didn't want to talk.” Another estimated, “Ninety percent, not very many kids that don't talk to you.” Other police said, “I haven't had very many not speak to me. I would have to say 95% of them or more talk,” a second confirmed, “I'd say better than 95%,” and a third said, “Vast majority, I'd say high-90s.” Almost all personnel thought that 90 percent or more of youths waived Miranda and none estimated that fewer than 80 percent waived.

6. Police elicited a confession when a juvenile admitted that he committed the crime with supporting details or when his cumulative responses satisfied all of the elements of an offense, i.e., act and intent. Questioners received an admission when it linked a youth to a crime or provided direct or circumstantial evidence of one or more elements of the offense. Admissions often occurred when a get-away driver, look-out, or co-defendant admitted participating, but minimized her role or responsibility. Police heard denials when a juvenile disavowed knowledge or responsibility or gave an explanation that did not include any incriminating admissions.

7. To measure the length of interrogation, in some cases, I directly timed the tape. In most transcripts, officers stated the start and stop times at the beginning and ending of an interrogation. In other cases, I estimated the duration of interrogation from the length of the transcript. I cross tabulated the number of transcript pages and length of interrogation in cases in which I had both to approximate the length of interrogations for which I had only transcripts. I always rounded estimates to the longer interval.

8. They estimated the average length of interviews and confirmed the validity of these findings: “fifteen minutes,” “twenty or twenty-five minutes,” “ten to twenty minutes,” “maybe thirty minutes,” “less than fifteen minutes,” “ten to fifteen minutes.” A fifteen-year veteran officer reported that “My longest has maybe been an hour.” One judge opined, “fifteen or twenty minutes,” a second judge confirmed, “usually ten to twenty minutes,” and a third judge agreed, “It doesn't take very long to get them to ‘fess up. Twenty minutes.” A prosecutor said interrogations are “Very short, usually. I would say under 10 minutes, the vast majority, under ten minutes.” Public defenders thought that typical interrogations took “not more than 20 minutes.”

References

References

Ainsworth, Janet E. (1993) “In a Different Register: The Pragmatics of Powerlessness in Police Interrogation,” 103 The Yale Law J. 259322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
American Bar Association, Institute of Judicial Administration (1980) Juvenile Justice Standards Relating to Pretrial Court Proceedings. Cambridge, MA: Ballinger.Google Scholar
Baird, Abigail A., et al. (1999) “Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging of Facial Affect Recognition in Children and Adolescents,” 38 J. of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 195–99.Google ScholarPubMed
Baldwin, John (1993) “Police Interview Techniques: Establishing Truth or Proof?,” 33 British J. of Criminology 325–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Billings, James F., et al. (2007) “Can Reinforcement Induce Children to Falsely Incriminate Themselves?31 Law and Human Behavior 125–39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Birckhead, Tamar R. (2008) “The Age of the Child: Interrogating Juveniles after Roper V. Simmons,” Washington and Lee Law Rev. 65 385450.Google Scholar
Bishop, Donna M., & Farber, Hillary B. (2007) “Joining the Legal Significance of Adolescent Developmental Capacities with the Legal Rights Provided by In Re Gault,” 60 Rutgers Law Rev. 125–73.Google Scholar
Bittner, Egon (1976) “Policing Juveniles: The Social Context of Common Practice,” in Rosenheim, Margaret, ed., Pursuing Justice for the Child. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. 6993.Google Scholar
Black, Donald J., & Reiss, J. Albert Jr. (1970) “Police Control of Juveniles,” 35 American Sociological Rev. 6377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonnie, Richard J., & Grisso, Thomas (2000) “Adjudicative Competence and Youthful Offenders,” in Grisso, Thomas, & Schwartz, Richard G., eds., Youth on Trial: A Developmental Perspective on Juvenile Justice. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. 73104.Google Scholar
Bull, Ray, & Soukara, Stavroula (2010) “Four Studies of What Really Happens in Police Interviews,” in Lassiter, G. D., & Meissner, C. A., eds., Police Interrogations and False Confessions: Current Research, Practice, and Policy Recommendations. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. 8196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bull, Ray, & Corran, Eleanor (2003) “Interviewing Child Witnesses: Past and Future,” 4 International J. of Police Science and Management 315–22.Google Scholar
Bull, Ray, & Milne, Rebecca (2004) “Attempts to Improve the Police Interviewing of Suspects,” in Glaser, Daniel, ed., Interrogations, Confessions, and Entrapment. New York: Kluwer Academic. 181–96.Google Scholar
Cassell, Paul G. (1996) “Miranda's Social Costs: An Empirical Reassessment,” 90 Northwestern Univ. Law Rev. 387499.Google Scholar
Cassell, Paul G. (1998) “Protecting the Innocent from False Confessions and Lost Confessions—and from Miranda,” 88 J. of Criminal Law and Criminology 497556.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cassell, Paul G., & Hayman, Bret S. (1996) “Police Interrogation in the 1990s: An Empirical Study of the Effects of Miranda,” 43 UCLA Law Rev. 839932.Google Scholar
Cicourel, Aaron V. (1995) The Social Organization of Juvenile Justice. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction.Google Scholar
Clarke, John P., & Sykes, Richard E. (1974) “Some Determinants of Police Organization and Practice in a Modern Industrial Democracy,” in Glaser, Daniel, ed., Handbook of Criminology. Chicago, IL: Rand-McNally. 455–94.Google Scholar
Dahl, Ronald E. (2001) “Affect Regulation, Brain Development, and Behavioral/Emotional Health in Adolescence,” 6 CNS Spectrums 6072.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Drizin, Steve A., & Reich, Marissa J. (2004) “Heeding the Lessons of History: The Need for Mandatory Recording of Police Interrogations to Accurately Assess the Reliability and Voluntariness of Confessions,” 52 Drake Law Rev. 619–46.Google Scholar
Drizin, Steve A., & Colgan, Beth A. (2004) “Tales from the Juvenile Confession Front: A Guide to How Standard Police Interrogation Tactics Can Produce Coerced and False Confessions from Juvenile Suspects,” in Lassiter, G. Daniel, ed., Interrogations, Confessions, and Entrapment. New York: Kluwer Academic. 127–62.Google Scholar
Drizin, Steve A., & Leo, Richard A. (2004) “The Problem of False Confessions in the Post-DNA World,” 82 North Carolina Law Rev. 8911008.Google Scholar
Drizin, Steve A., & Luloff, Greg (2007) “Are Juvenile Courts a Breeding Ground for Wrongful Convictions?,” 34 Northern Kentucky Law Rev. 257322.Google Scholar
Emerson, Robert M. (1974) “Role Determinants in Juvenile Court,” in Glaser, Daniel, ed., Handbook of Criminology. Chicago, IL: Rand McNally. 621–50.Google Scholar
Evans, Roger (1993) The Conduct of Police Interviews with Juveniles. Royal Commission on Criminal Justice Report. London: HMSO.Google Scholar
Farber, Hillary B. (2004) “The Role of the Parent/Guardian in Juvenile Custodial Interrogations: Friend or Foe?,” 41 American Criminal Law Rev. 1277–312.Google Scholar
Feld, Barry C. (1999) Bad Kids: Race and the Transformation of the Juvenile Court. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Feld, Barry C. (2006a) “Juveniles' Competence to Exercise Miranda Rights: An Empirical Study of Policy and Practice,” 91 Minnesota Law Rev. 26100.Google Scholar
Feld, Barry C. (2006b) “Police Interrogation of Juveniles: An Empirical Study of Policy and Practice,” 97 J. Criminal Law and Criminology 219316.Google Scholar
Feld, Barry C., & Schaefer, Shelly (2010a) “The Right to Counsel in Juvenile Court: The Conundrum of Attorneys as an Aggravating Factor at Disposition,” 27 Justice Quarterly 713–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feld, Barry C., & Schaefer, Shelly (2010b) “The Right to Counsel in Juvenile Court: Law Reform to Deliver Legal Services and Reduce Justice by Geography,” 9 Criminology & Public Policy 327–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Furby, Lita, & Beyth-Marom, Ruth (1992) “Risk Taking in Adolescence: A Decision-Making Perspective,” 12 Developmental Rev. 144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garrett, Brandon L. (2010) “The Substance of False Confessions,” 62 Stanford Law Rev. 1051–118.Google Scholar
Godsey, Mark A. (2005) “Rethinking the Involuntary Confession Rule: Toward a Workable Test for Identifying Compelled Self-Incrimination,” 93 California Law Rev. 465540.Google Scholar
Goldstein, Alan, & Goldstein, Naomi E. Sevin (2010) Evaluating Capacity to Waive Miranda. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Grisso, Thomas (1980) “Juveniles' Capacities to Waive Miranda Rights: An Empirical Analysis,” 68 California Law Rev. 1134–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grisso, Thomas (1981) Juveniles' Waivers of Rights: Legal and Psychological Competence. Vol. 3, Perspectives in Law & Psychology. New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grisso, Thomas (2000) “What We Know about Youth's Capacities as Trial Defendants,” in Grisso, Thomas, & Schwartz, Robert G., eds., Youth on Trial: A Developmental Perspective on Juvenile Justice. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. 139–72.Google Scholar
Grisso, Thomas, & Pomiciter, Carolyn (1977) “Interrogation of Juveniles: An Empirical Study of Procedures, Safeguards and Rights Waiver,” 1 Law and Human Behavior 321–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grisso, Thomas, et al. (2002) “Juveniles' Competence to Stand Trial: New Questions for An Era of Punitive Juvenile Justice Reform,” in Puritz, Patricia, ed., More Than Meets the Eye: Rethinking, Assessment, Competency and Sentencing for a Harsher Era of Juvenile Justice. Washington, DC: American Bar Association Juvenile Justice Center. 2338.Google Scholar
Grisso, Thomas, et al. (2003) “Juveniles' Competence to Stand Trial: A Comparison of Adolescents' and Adults' Capacities as Trial Defendants,” 27 Law and Human Behavior 333–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gross, Samuel R., et al. (2005) “Exonerations in the United States: 1989 through 2003,” 95 J. of Criminal Law and Criminology 523–60.Google Scholar
Gruber, Stacy A., & Yurgelun-Todd, Deborah A. (2006) “Neurobiology and the Law: A Role in Juvenile Justice,” 3 Ohio State J. of Criminal Law 321–40.Google Scholar
Gudjonsson, Gisli H. (2003) The Psychology of Interrogations and Confessions: A Handbook. West Sussex: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Inbau, Fred E., et al. (2004) Criminal Interrogation and Confessions, 4th ed. Sudbury, MA: Jones and Bartlett Learning. First edition published in 1962.Google Scholar
Kaban, Barbara, & Tobey, Ann E. (1999) “When Police Question Children, Are Protections Adequate?,” 1 J. of the Center for Children and the Courts 151–60.Google Scholar
Kassin, Saul M. (1997) “The Psychology of Confession Evidence,” 52 American Psychologist 221–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kassin, Saul M. (2005) “On the Psychology of Confessions: Does Innocence Put Innocents at Risk?,” 60 American Psychologist 215–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kassin, Saul M., & Gudjonsson, Gisli H. (2004) “The Psychology of Confession Evidence: A Review of the Literature and Issues,” 5 Psychological Science in the Public Interest 3367.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kassin, Saul M., & McNall, Karlyn (1991) “Police Interrogation and Confessions: Communicating Promises and Threats by Pragmatic Implication,” 15 Law and Human Behavior 233–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kassin, Saul M., et al. (2007) “Police Interviewing and Interrogation: A Self-Report Survey of Police Practices and Beliefs,” 31 Law and Human Behavior 381400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kassin, Saul M., et al. (2010) “Police-Induced Confessions: Risk Factors and Recommendations,” 34 Law and Human Behavior 338.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, Lesley, & Snook, Brent (2009) “Peering Inside the Canadian Interrogation Room: An Examination of the Reid Model of Interrogation, Influence Tactics, and Coercive Strategies,” 36 Criminal Justice and Behavior 674–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LaFave, Wayne R. (1965) Arrest. Boston, MA: Little Brown.Google Scholar
Larson, Kimberly (2003) “Improving the ‘Kangaroo Courts:’ A Proposal for Reform in Evaluating Juveniles' Waiver of Miranda,” 48 Villanova Law Rev. 629–68.Google Scholar
Leo, Richard A. (1996a) “The Impact of Miranda Revisited,” 86 J. of Criminal Law and Criminology 621–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leo, Richard A. (1996b) “Inside the Interrogation Room,” 86 J. of Criminal Law and Criminology 266303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leo, Richard A. (1996c) “Miranda's Revenge: Police Interrogation as a Confidence Game,” 30 Law & Society Rev. 259–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leo, Richard A. (2008) Police Interrogation and American Justice. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leo, Richard A. (2009) “False Confessions: Causes, Consequences and Implications,” 37 J. of American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law 332–43.Google ScholarPubMed
Leo, Richard A., & White, Welsh S. (1999) “Adapting to Miranda: Modern Interrogators” Strategies for Dealing with the Obstacles Posed by Miranda,” 84 Minnesota Law Rev. 397472.Google Scholar
Leo, Richard A., et al. (2006) “Bringing Reliability Back in: False Confessions and Legal Safeguards in the Twenty-First Century,” 2006 Wisconsin Law Rev. 479539.Google Scholar
Lyon, Thomas D. (1999) “The New Wave in Children's Suggestibility Research: A Critique,” 84 Cornell Law Rev. 1004–87.Google Scholar
Maroney, Terry A. (2009) “The False Promise of Adolescent Brain Science in Juvenile Justice,” 85 The Notre Dame Law Rev. 89176.Google Scholar
Matza, David (1964) Delinquency and Drift. New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Meyer, Jessica R., & Reppucci, Dickon N. (2007) “Police Practices and Perceptions Regarding Juvenile Interrogation and Interrogative Suggestibility,” 25 Behavioral Sciences & the Law 757–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milne, Rebecca, & Bull, Ray (1999) Investigative Interviewing: Psychology and Practice. West Sussex: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Nardulli, Peter F., et al. (1988) The Tenor of Justice: Criminal Courts and the Guilty Plea Process. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, Neil (2006) Strategies for the Recorded Interview. St. Paul, MN: St. Paul Police Department.Google Scholar
Neubauer, David W. (1974) “Confessions in Prairie City: Some Causes and Effects,” 65 J. of Criminal Law and Criminology 103–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ofshe, Richard J., & Leo, Richard A. (1997) “The Decision to Confess Falsely: Rational Choice and Irrational Action,” Denver University Law Rev. 74 9791122.Google Scholar
Owen-Kostelnik, Jessica, et al. (2006) “Testimony and Interrogation of Minors: Assumptions about Maturity and Morality,” 61 American Psychologist 286304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Packer, Herbert L. (1968) The Limits of the Criminal Sanction. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearse, John, & Gudjonsson, Gisli H. (2003) “The Identification and Measurement of ‘Oppressive’ Police Interviewing Tactics in Britain,” in Gudjonsson, Gisli H., ed., The Psychology of Interrogations and Confessions: A Handbook. West Sussex: John Wiley & Sons. 75114.Google Scholar
Pilliavin, Irving, & Briar, Scott (1964) “Police Encounters with Juveniles,” 2 American J. of Sociology 206–14.Google Scholar
Podkopacz, Marcy R., & Feld, Barry (1996) “The End of the Line: An Empirical Study of Judicial Waiver,” 86 J. of Criminal Law and Criminology 449–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Police and Criminal Evidence Act (PACE) (1984) c. 60, pt. V (Eng.) (revised 1991).Google Scholar
Puritz, Patricia, et al. (1995) A Call for Justice: An Assessment of Access to Counsel and Quality of Representation in Delinquency Proceedings. Washington, DC: American Bar Association, Juvenile Justice Center.Google Scholar
Redlich, Allison D. (2004) “Law & Psychiatry: Mental Illness, Police Interrogations, and the Potential for False Confessions,” 55 Psychiatric Services 1921.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Redlich, Allison D. (2010) “False Confessions, False Guilty Pleas: Similarities and Differences,” in Lassiter, G. David, & Meissner, Christian A., eds., Police Interrogations and False Confessions: Current Research, Practice, and Police Recommendations. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. 4966.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Redlich, Allison D., & Drizin, Steven (2007) “Police Interrogation of Youth,” in Kessler, Carol L., & Kraus, Louis J., eds., The Mental Health Needs of Young Offenders: Forging Paths Toward Reintegration and Rehabilitation. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. 6178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogers, Richard, et al. (2007) “An Analysis of Miranda Warnings and Waivers: Comprehension and Coverage,” 31 Law and Human Behavior 177–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, Richard, et al. (2008a) “The Language of Miranda Warnings in American Jurisdictions: A Replication and Vocabulary Analysis,” 32 Law and Human Behavior 124–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, Richard, et al. (2008b) “The Comprehensibility and Content of Juvenile Miranda Warnings,” 14 Psychology, Public Policy and Law 6387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scott, Elizabeth S., & Grisso, Thomas (1997) “The Evolution of Adolescence: A Developmental Perspective on Juvenile Justice Reform,” 88 J. of Criminal Law and Criminology 137–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scott, Elizabeth S., & Grisso, Thomas (2005) “Developmental Incompetence, Due Process, and Juvenile Justice Policy,” 83 North Carolina Law Rev. 793846.Google Scholar
Scott, Elizabeth S., & Steinberg, Laurence (2008) Rethinking Juvenile Justice. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Skolnick, Jerome H. (1967) Justice without Trial: Law Enforcement in Democratic Society. New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Slobogin, Christopher (2003) “Toward Taping,” 1 Ohio State J. of Criminal Law 309–22.Google Scholar
Slobogin, Christopher (2007) “Lying and Confessing,” 39 Texas Tech Law Rev. 1275–92.Google Scholar
Snyder, Howard, & Sickmund, Melissa (2006) Juvenile Offenders and Victims: 2006 National Report. Washington, DC: Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention.Google Scholar
Soukara, Stavroula, et al. (2009) “What Really Happens in Police Interviews of Suspects? Tactics and Confessions,” 15 Psychology, Crime & Law 493506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spear, L. P. (2000) “The Adolescent Brain and Age-Related Behavioral Manifestations,” 24 Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Revs. 417–63.Google ScholarPubMed
Steinberg, Laurence (2005) “Cognitive and Affective Development in Adolescence,” 9 Trends in Cognitive Science 6974.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steinberg, Laurence, & Cauffman, Elizabeth (1996) “Maturity of Judgment in Adolescence: Psychosocial Factors in Adolescent Decision-Making,” 20 Law and Human Behavior 249–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steinberg, Laurence, & Cauffman, Elizabeth (1999) “The Elephant in the Courtroom: A Developmental Perspective on the Adjudication of Youthful Offenders,” 6 Virginia J. of Social Policy & the Law 389418.Google Scholar
Steinberg, Laurence, et al. (2009) “Age Differences in Future Orientation and Delay Discounting,” 80 Child Development 2844.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sullivan, Thomas P. (2004) Police Experiences with Recording Custodial Interrogations. Chicago, IL: Northwestern University School of Law.Google Scholar
Sullivan, Thomas P. (2006) “The Time Has Come for Law Enforcement Recordings of Custodial Interviews, Start to Finish,” 37 Golden Gate Univ. Law Rev. 175–90.Google Scholar
Sullivan, Thomas P. (2010) “The Wisdom of Custodial Recording,” in Lassiter, G. Daniel, & Meissner, Christian A., eds., Police Interrogations and False Confessions: Current Research, Practice, and Policy Recommendations. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. 127–42.Google Scholar
Sykes, Gresham M., & Matza, David (1957) “Techniques of Neutralization: A Theory of Delinquency,” 22 American Sociological Rev. 664–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tanenhaus, David S., & Drizin, Steven A. (2002) “ ‘Owing to the Extreme Youth of the Accused’: The Changing Legal Response to Juvenile Homicide,” 92 J. of Criminal Law and Criminology 641706.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tepfer, Joshua A., et al. (2010) “Arresting Development: Convictions of Innocent Youth,” 62 Rutgers Law Rev. 887941.Google Scholar
Tobey, Ann, et al. (2000) “Youths' Trial Participation as Seen by Youths and Their Attorneys: An Exploration of Competence-Based Issues,” in Grisso, Thomas, & Schwartz, Richard G., eds., Youth on Trial: A Developmental Perspective on Juvenile Justice. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. 225–42.Google Scholar
Viljoen, Jodi, & Roesch, Ronald (2005) “Competence to Waive Interrogation Rights and Adjudicative Competence in Adolescent Defendants: Cognitive Development, Attorney Contact, and Psychological Symptoms,” 29 Law and Human Behavior 723–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Viljoen, Jodi, et al. (2005) “Legal Decisions of Preadolescent and Adolescent Defendants: Predictors of Confessions, Pleas, Communication with Attorneys, and Appeals,” 29 Law and Human Behavior 253–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Viljoen, Jodi, et al. (2007) “Adjudicative Competence and Comprehension of Miranda Rights in Adolescent Defendants: A Comparison of Legal Standards,” 25 Behavioral Sciences & the Law 119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wald, Michael, et al. (1967) “Interrogations in New Haven: The Impact of Miranda,” 76 The Yale Law J. 1519–648.Google Scholar
Weisselberg, Charles D. (2008) “Mourning Miranda,” 96 California Law Rev. 1519–601.Google Scholar
White, Welsh S. (1997) “False Confessions and the Constitution: Safeguards against Unworthy Confessions,” 17 Harvard Civil Rights-Civil Liberties Law Rev. 105–58.Google Scholar
White, Welsh S. (2001) “Miranda's Failure to Restrain Pernicious Interrogation Practices,” 99 Michigan Law Rev. 1211–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woolard, Jennfier L., et al. (2008) “Anticipatory Injustice Among Adolescents: Age and Racial/Ethnic Differences in Perceived Unfairness of the Justice System,” 26 Behavioral Sciences and the Law 207–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Cases Cited

Ashcraft v. Tennessee (1944) 322 U.S. 143.Google Scholar
Berghuis v. Thompkins (2010) 130 S.Ct. 2250.Google Scholar
Breed v. Jones (1975) 421 U.S. 519.Google Scholar
Davis v. United States (1994) 512 U.S. 452.Google Scholar
Dickerson v. United States (2000) 530 U.S. 428.Google Scholar
Drope v. Missouri (1975) 420 U.S. 162.Google Scholar
Dusky v. United States (1960) 362 U.S. 402.Google Scholar
Escobedo v. Illinois (1964) 378 U.S. 478.Google Scholar
Fare v. Michael C. (1979) 442 U.S. 707.Google Scholar
Gallegos v. Colorado (1962) 370 U.S. 49.Google Scholar
Graham v. Florida (2010) 130 S. Ct. 2011.Google Scholar
Haley v. Ohio (1948) 332 U.S. 596.Google Scholar
In re Gault (1967) 387 U.S. 1.Google Scholar
In re Winship (1970) 397 U.S. 358.Google Scholar
J.D.B. v. North Carolina (2011) 131 S. Ct. 2394.Google Scholar
Miranda v. Arizona (1966) 384 U.S. 436.Google Scholar
Missouri v. Seibert (2004) 542 U.S. 600.Google Scholar
Pennsylvania v. Muniz (1990) 496 U.S. 582.Google Scholar
Rhode Island v. Innis (1980) 446 U.S. 291.Google Scholar
Roper v. Simmons (2005) 543 U.S. 551.Google Scholar
State v. Scales (Minn. 1994) 518 N.W.2d 587.Google Scholar
Yarborough v. Alvarado (2004) 541 U.S. 652.Google Scholar

Statutes Cited

Minn. Stat. Ann. (2005) §260B.163.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Characteristics of Juveniles Interrogated

Figure 1

Table 2. Juveniles Who Waive or Invoke by Offense and Prior Record*

Figure 2

Table 3. Maximization Questions: Types and Frequency

Figure 3

Table 4. Minimization Questions: Types and Frequencies

Figure 4

Table 5. Outcome of Interrogation and Youths' Attitude

Figure 5

Table 6. Length of Interrogation by Type of Offense* and Weapon**