Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T08:11:28.985Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

On competing indexicalities in southern Peninsular Spanish. A sociophonetic and perceptual analysis of affricate [ts] through time

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 October 2022

Matilde Vida-Castro*
Affiliation:
Malaga University, Spain
*
Corresponding author. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This paper examines linguistic, cognitive, and social factors in the development of an ongoing sound change in Andalusian Spanish related to the crosslinguistically well-known process of syllable coda lenition. The resyllabification of word internal /-s/ when followed by dental plosive /t/, in words such as lingüística [liŋ⋅ˈgujs⋅ti⋅ka] ‘linguistics’ realized as [liŋ⋅ˈguj⋅tsi⋅ka], results in an affricate sound [ts] that may be indexed in different ways within the speech community. Findings are reported from a trend study of two sample surveys separated by a twenty-year time gap, acoustic analysis, and two perception experiments. Acoustic phonetics, historical linguistics, theoretical phonology, and sociolinguistic studies provide the theoretical background to help explain the development of this sound change and its connection with other phonological features of Andalusian Spanish. Development of the affricate allophone is a natural outcome consistent with universal constraints boosted by the recent emergence of a regional koine, where its indexicality is undetermined.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Southern varieties of European Spanish are characterized by innovative features sharply contrasting with those of northern Spanish, the basis of the Castilian Spanish standard and, therefore, the most prestigious variety in Spain. Apart from a different phonemic system in onset position, which is a consequence of a set of mergers during the late Middle Ages (e.g., Penny, Reference Penny2002:98–103), southern European Spanish is known for the lenition process that affects consonants in coda position. For example, words such as reloj ‘clock’ [reˈlox] and Madrid ‘Madrid’ [maˈðrið] are usually realized as [reˈlo] or [maˈðri]. The most salient phoneme undergoing this process is the voiceless alveolar fricative /-s/, which presents aspirated and deleted variants, not only in Spain but also in most Latin American speech communities (Penny, Reference Penny2002:106-108). In Malaga, the lenition process of postnuclear /-s/ (either word-internal or word-final) is almost completed, as the most frequent variant is the total deletion of this segment (words like adios /aˈðjos/ ‘good bye’ or isla /ˈisla/ ‘island’ are categorically pronounced as [aˈðjo] or [ˈila]), with no statistically significant influence of any external factors (Vida-Castro, Reference Vida-Castro2004:155-78).

The single exception to this rule is when word-internal /-s/ is followed by a voiceless plosive phoneme /p, t, k/. In this particular context and position, the most frequent variants are a set of postaspirated realizations (Torreira, Reference Torreira, Sagarra and Toribio2006, Reference Torreira, Prieto, Mascaró and Solé2007, Reference Torreira2012), so words like chispa /ˈt͡ʃispa/ ‘spark,’ agosto /aˈɣosto/ ‘August,’ or casco /ˈkasko/ ‘helmet’ are usually pronounced as [ˈt͡ʃipha], [aˈɣotho] or [ˈkakho], especially among the youngest and most educated groups of speakers, while the oldest and less educated ones seem to favor the deletion rule (Vida-Castro, Reference Vida-Castro2004:159, 192). More specifically, when word-internal /-s/ is followed by the dental voiceless plosive /t/, it is nowadays common to hear an allegedly new affricate variant, such that words like estudiar /estuˈðjar/ ‘to study’ or fiesta /ˈfjesta/ ‘party’ are pronounced as [etsuˈðjaɾ] or [ˈfjetsa], especially by young female speakers (Vida-Castro, Reference Vida-Castro and Ortega Arjonilla2018:106-107).

In this paper, historical linguistics, theoretical phonology, and sociolinguistic studies provide the theoretical background to help us explore the development of this sound change. Data are extracted from (1) a trend study where two sample surveys separated by a twenty-year time gap, between 1995 and 2015, are compared; (2) an acoustic analysis based on the 2015 corpus of data; and (3) two perception experiments. The results show that (i) the acoustic characteristics of the affricate sound can be interpreted as the result of a re-syllabification process where postnuclear /-s/ changes its position to the onset of the following syllable; (ii) the acoustic cues that define the acoustic distance between the new sound and the voiceless palatal affricate /t͡ʃ/ seem to be operative in the perceptual discrimination tasks, especially among the youngest listeners; (iii) the frequency of the resyllabified variant has significantly increased over the last twenty years; and (iv) its social distribution has also changed during the same period of time, so that different and competing indexicalities may be associated to the affricate (new) allophone in the context of the emergence of an intermediate variety between the southern vernacular varieties and the national standard (Villena-Ponsoda & Vida-Castro, Reference Villena-Ponsoda, Matilde Vida-Castro, Buchstaller and Siebenhaar2017, Reference Villena-Ponsoda, Vida-Castro, Cerruti and Tsiplakou2020).

Theoretical background

Universal linguistic constraints on the coda lenition process

The lenition process of codas is a crosslinguistically well-known language-internal tendency that affects consonants in a weak syllabic position. Examples can be found in different languages such as English (Guy, Reference Guy and Labov1980; Labov, Reference Labov1972:216-23; Neu, Reference Neu and Labov1980; Raymond, Dautricourt, & Hume, Reference Raymond, Dautricourt and Hume2006; Wolfram, Reference Wolfram1969); Dutch (Hinskens, Reference Hinskens, Kügler, Féry and van de Vijver2009; Hinskens & van Hout, Reference Hinskens, van Hout and Viereck1994:297-310), German (Gilles & Hinskens, Reference Gilles and Hinskens2019), Brazilian Portuguese (Naro, Reference Naro1981; Scherre & Naro, Reference Scherre and Naro1991) or Spanish (Penny, Reference Penny2002:106-108), among many others. Although some coda lenition examples can be considered specific cases of simplification of consonant clusters ending in /-t, -d/, the deletion process usually applies to every sort of consonant (and not only plosives or fricatives, but also nasal, laterals, and rhotics) in postvocalic positions–and even with grammatical functions.

In the particular context of postnuclear /-s/ variable realization in European Spanish, it can be assumed in the framework of Optimality Theory that a set of conflicting constraints are either triggering or blocking the deletion process (Boersma, Dekkers, & van de Weijer, Reference Boersma, Dekkers, van de Weijer, Dekkers, van der Leeuw and van de Weijer2000:2-4; Prince & Smolensky, Reference Prince and Smolensky2004:1-10). Thus, among the more conservative varieties of Spanish (those upon which the Castilian standard variety has been formed and that preserve syllable-final /-s/), the Faithfulness family of linguistic constraints (those that make it possible that every segment of the input has a physical representation in the output) dominates over Well-formedness constraints (which promote open-syllable structures); on the contrary, among innovative varieties (those which frequently delete syllable-final /-s/), syllabic well-formedness is the most dominant constraint even when the morphological affixation takes placeFootnote 1 (Vida-Castro, Reference Vida-Castro2004:33-49).

The aspiration rule, which is usually considered the initial stage of the deletion process (López-Morales, Reference López-Morales1983:48-51; Samper-Padilla, Reference Samper-Padilla1990:127-29; Sankoff, Reference Sankoff, Núñez-Cedeño, Páez-Urdaneta and Guitart1986:109-15; Terrell, Reference Terrell1979:154-65), can also be understood in the same framework. Aspirated codas guarantee the phonetic representation of the underlying form, and, at the same time, they can be regarded as more harmonic than fricative or plosive realizations in the postnuclear position according to the sonority scale of sounds (Clements, Reference Clements, Kingston and Beckman1990; Goldsmith, Reference Goldsmith1990:111; Jiménez & Lloret, Reference Jiménez, Lloret and Val-Álvaro2011). Furthermore, the postaspirated allophones that usually appear in sequences of /-s/ plus voiceless stop (henceforth /-s⋅C/ sequences (words such as casco /ˈkasko/ ‘helmet’ pronounced as [ˈkakho] or estudiar /estuˈdiar/ ‘to study’ realized as [ethuˈðjaɾ]) completely satisfy both the faithfullness and well-formedness families of constraints.

In the framework of Articulatory Phonology, Parrell (Reference Parrell2012:38) provided evidence that, in Western Andalusian Spanish, postaspirated allophones of /-s⋅C/ sequences are the outcome of a shift in the phasing relationship of the articulatory gestures between the glottal spreading gesture for coda /-s/ and the oral closure gesture for the voiceless stop, so the two gestures begin simultaneously (in-phase relationship). This finding not only provides an articulatory basis to explain the resyllabification process in preconsonantal contexts but also reveals that its outcome produces an open-syllable structure which follows the Well-formedness restrictions.

Therefore, it can be considered that the postaspirated and affricate allophones that result from the resyllabification process of /-s⋅C/ sequences constitute an optimal output in maintaining the phonological representation of the underlying forms while also favoring open-syllable structure through a group of sounds that show a natural realization from the articulatory perspective. It should not be forgotten that postaspirated plosives and affricate sounds can be found as phonological categories in several languages, such as Thai, Korean, Lithuanian, Italian, or Catalan (Jiménez, Reference Jiménez1995; Ladefoged & Maddieson, Reference Ladefoged and Maddieson1996:55-56; Maddieson, Reference Maddieson1984:12, 28, 38; Rogers & d’Arcangeli, Reference Rogers and d'Arcangeli2004).

Long-term sound shifts: diachronic evidence of lenition

Linguistic evolution of different language families provides clear evidence of the vitality of the aforementioned linguistic constraints. It is well known that word-final /-s/ had a weak articulation among many ancient Indo-European languages, which led to its aspiration and even deletion in, among others, Sanskrit, Armenian, ancient Slavic, or Celtic (Alvar, Reference Alvar1955; Meringer, Reference Meringer1923; Politzer, Reference Politzer1947; Väänänem, Reference Väänänem1950). It is also known that deletion of the final /-s/ plural marker divided Romance languages into the two main traditional—eastern and western—linguistic groups (von Wartburg, Reference von Wartburg1952) and that the same process has continued in the western group in French, where the historical deletion of final /p, t, k, s/ took place during the Middle Ages (Meyer-Lübke, Reference Meyer-Lübke1934).

Another well-documented linguistic change is the development of affricates and fricatives out of voiceless plosives registered among Indo-European languages. A first example can be found in the First Germanic Sound Shift (Grimm's and Verner's Law), where Proto-Indo-European voiceless stops changed into voiceless fricatives, and in the Second (or “High German”) Sound Shift (Gloning & Young, Reference Gloning and Young2003:13-15, 32-36), which involved a series of consonant changes that made the three Germanic voiceless stops become affricates or fricatives depending on the phonetic environments (i.e., Modern Standard German /p/ > /p͡f/ in Apfel (apple) [ˈap͡fəl] and /t/ > /t͡s/ in Zwei (two) [t͡svai]). Currently, fricative and even affricate variants of /t/ are very frequent in several varieties of modern English (Kallen, Reference Kallen, Auer, Hinskens and Kerswill2005; West, Reference West, Auer, Caro-Reina and Kaufmann2013).

A second example can be found in Romance languages. In the evolution from Latin to Old Spanish, dental and velar voiceless plosives also evolved, under a process of palatalization in different phonetic environments, into a series of affricate and fricative (new) consonants (Penny, Reference Penny2002:62-65, 101). Closer to resyllabification of /-s⋅t/ is the process of palatalization of syllable-final plosive velars, which produced both fricative /ʃ/, /x/, and affricate /t͡ʃ/ outcomes (e.g., Penny, Reference Penny2002:69-71). This last case, the Latin group K⋅T > /t͡ʃ/ (NOCTE [ˈnokte] (night) > noche [ˈnot͡ʃe]), is very similar to the current Andalusian metathesis in words such as coste /ˈkoste/ (cost, price) to [ˈkotse]. Additionally, although we cannot find a direct shift from the Latin group /S⋅T/ to an affricate outcome in Spanish, there are many examples of that transformation in words that come from Arabic (e.g., Ar. ost̟awán (hall) > old Sp. açaguán [atsaˈwan]) (Alonso, Reference Alonso1967:106-24).

Finally, a postaspirated realization of /-s⋅t/ can be documented, at least by the late 1950s, in the Linguistic Atlas of Andalusia (ALEA) (Alvar, Llorente, & Salvador, Reference Alvar, Llorente and Salvador1973, VI: map 1602; Moya-Corral & Tejada-Giráldez, Reference Moya-Corral and de la Sierra Tejada-Giráldez2020:205; Villena-Ponsoda, Reference Villena-Ponsoda2018:25-31).

Sociolinguistic studies on final /-s/ variation in Spanish

The lenition process of postnuclear /-s/ in Latin American and European innovative varieties of Spanish has been widely studied from a variationist perspective. Studies on Latin American varieties over the last fifty years include those of Donni de Mirande (Reference Donni de Mirande1968, Reference Donni de Mirande1989) in Rosario (Argentina), Cedergren (Reference Cedergren1972) in Panama, Terrell (Reference Terrell1978) in Buenos Aires (Argentina), Poplack (Reference Poplack1979) on Spanish speakers in Philadelphia (USA), López-Morales (Reference López-Morales1983) in Puerto Rico, Alba (Reference Alba1990) in Santiago de los Caballeros (Dominican Republic), or Brown and Torres Cacoullos (Reference Brown, Torres-Cacoullos, Nuñez-Cedeño, López and Cameron2003) in Chihuahua (Mexico), where the authors explored the interrelation between processes of syllable-final and syllable-initial /s/. As to the European Spanish varieties, the most relevant studies were those by Moya-Corral (Reference Moya-Corral1979) in Jaen, Samper-Padilla (Reference Samper-Padilla1990) in Las Palmas de Gran Canaria, Martín-Butragueño (Reference Martín-Butragueño1991) in Getafe, Gómez-Serrano (Reference Gómez-Serrano1993) in Linares, Molina-Martos (Reference Molina-Martos1998, Reference Molina-Martos, Cestero-Mancera, Molina-Martos and Paredes-García2015) in Toledo and Vallecas, Blanco-Canales (Reference Blanco-Canales2004) in Alcalá de Henares, Kapović (Reference Kapović2014) in Ciudad Real, or Fernández de Molina-Ortés in Mérida (Reference Fernández de Molina-Ortés2016).

As traditionally described, the lenition process of post-nuclear /-s/ consists of two basic successive steps, that is, the aspiration and the total deletion of the segment: [s]>[h]>[∅]. Thus, most of the aforementioned studies rely on the perceptual categorization of the traditional allophonic realizations, mainly sibilant [s], aspiration [h], and deletion [∅]. However, acoustic approximations to this topic have also been accomplished in different speech communities, for example, by Erker (Reference Erker2010) in Santo Domingo (Dominican Republic) or File-Muriel and Brown (Reference File-Muriel and Brown2011) in Cali (Colombia).

In southern Spain, studies conducted by Vida-Castro (Reference Vida-Castro2004, Reference Vida-Castro2016) in Malaga, Ruch (Reference Ruch2008, Reference Ruch2013, Reference Ruch2018) in Seville, and Tejada-Giráldez (Reference Tejada-Giráldez2015) in Granada that address the acoustic characterization of the different variants of /-s/ revealed the presence of postaspirated allophones whenever postnuclear /-s/ is followed by a voiceless plosive consonant. The acoustic characterization of such aspirated plosive sounds in Western Andalusian Spanish has specifically been explored by Gylfadottir (Reference Gylfadottir2019), O'Neill (Reference O'Neill2010), Parrel (Reference Parrell2012), and Torreira (Reference Torreira, Prieto, Mascaró and Solé2007, Reference Torreira2012), who established a solid basis for the experimental approach to this set of aspirated variants of /-s/ in coda position. Furthermore, Ruch and Harrington (Reference Ruch and Harrington2014) provided phonetic evidence that postaspiration is a result of a sound change in progress in Western Andalusian Spanish. In a perception experiment, they furthermore found out that postaspiration was a relevant cue for distinguishing the minimal pair /ˈpata/-/ˈpasta/ (‘leg’ and ‘dough’ respectively), not only among Andalusian speakers (Ruch & Peters, Reference Ruch and Peters2016) but also by listeners of Argentinian Spanish, a nonpostaspirating variety.

As to the special case of the affricate allophone of /-s⋅t/ in Andalusian Spanish, Moya-Corral (Reference Moya-Corral2007) compared the realizations of /-s⋅t/ in two samples of speakers from Seville (the historical and administrative head city of Andalusia) and Antequera (a town in the province of Malaga, in fact, in central Andalusia), and found that the affricate realizations were favored by educated young informants. Ruch (Reference Ruch2008) thoroughly analyzed the acoustic characteristics and the social distribution of this specific sound and the linguistic constraints that favor its realization among a stratified group of fifty-four speakers in Seville. Not only did she study the case of word-internal /-s⋅t/, but she also analyzed word-final /-s# t/ in sequences such as los tomates ‘the tomatoes,’ which can be realized as [lotsoˈmate]. Her results indicate that the affricate sound is favored in word-internal position, especially in frequent words (Bybee & Hopper, Reference Bybee and Hopper2001:3), and it is promoted by young speakers. Similarly, in Malaga city (Vida-Castro, Reference Vida-Castro and Ortega Arjonilla2018), where only word-internal /-s⋅t/ occurrences have been analyzed, the affricate sound is favored by young speakers. Regarding the Eastern Andalusian Spanish variety, Tejada-Giráldez (Reference Tejada-Giráldez2015:82, 164) found that, in Granada, this variant is not frequent at all (1.2%), but the emergence of this allophone is interpreted as an incipient change.

The indexicality of a new feature in a blended new variety

It should be considered that the appearance of the allegedly new affricate allophone of /-s⋅t/ in Malaga takes place in the context of the emergence of a koinesed variety, which would have developed (since approximately 1950) in southern Spain, between, on the one hand, the Andalusian vernacular varieties and, on the other, the national standard in Spain (Villena-Ponsoda, Reference Villena-Ponsoda1996; Villena-Ponsoda & Vida-Castro, Reference Villena-Ponsoda, Matilde Vida-Castro, Buchstaller and Siebenhaar2017, Reference Villena-Ponsoda, Vida-Castro, Cerruti and Tsiplakou2020). The linguistic features that shape this intermediate variety are a combination of northern conservative standard-like realizations of consonants in syllable-onset position (of which most salient is the acquisition of the phonemic contrast between dentoalveolar fricatives /s/ and /θ/) with southern innovative vernacular deletion of syllable-final consonants, especially syllable-final /-s/, which is an unmarked phonetic trait in southern varieties. However, as has been observed elsewhere (Cerruti & Tsiplakou, Reference Cerruti and Tsiplakou2020), not only is the presence of vernacular unmarked features frequent in the formation of regional varieties, but such varieties may also accept and develop linguistic innovations with no basis in either the standard or in local dialects (Auer, Reference Auer, Delbecque, van der Auwera and Geeraerts2005:25). This could probably be the case of the affricate allophone [ts] of /-s⋅t/, that is, the result of a natural shift that finds, in an emerging variety, a fertile environment in which to spread across the speech community.

Recent research on the intermediate southern Spanish variety shows evidence for a perceptually coherent sociolect that indexes a blended identity as it is used by urban, modern, and standard-orientated speakers who are, at the same time, loyal to both the local community norms and the southern traditional values (Villena-Ponsoda & Vida-Castro, Reference Villena-Ponsoda, Matilde Vida-Castro, Buchstaller and Siebenhaar2017, Reference Villena-Ponsoda, Vida-Castro, Cerruti and Tsiplakou2020).Footnote 2 Bearing in mind that the affricate realization of /-s⋅t/ is frequent among young educated speakers, it could be hypothesized that it is one of the features forming the intermediate southern variety. However, as it has not traditionally been associated either with the vernacular or with the standard, its social meaning is yet to be assigned, and its indexical values can easily be transformed and enregistered among the speech community members (on these general notions, see Eckert, Reference Eckert2012:94-97; Silverstein, Reference Silverstein2003:216-20; Sneller, Reference Sneller2020).

Hypotheses

Based on the background discussed above, the analysis presented in this paper examines the following hypotheses:

Related to the hypotheses, three research questions (RQs) can be posed:

  • RQ1. Has the frequency of the affricate allophone of /-s⋅t/ increased over the last twenty years in Malaga city? If so, has the social distribution of the affricate allophone changed? In order to test this question, a trend study compares data from two samples separated by a twenty-year gap.

  • RQ2. Which acoustic characteristics contrast the different perceptive categories of /-s⋅t/? The acoustic analysis of data from the most recent sample responds to this question.

  • RQ3. To what extent are the ‘resyllabified’ (postaspirated [th] and affricate [ts]) allophones of /-s⋅t/ reliably perceived as an /-s⋅t/ sequence (and not as a singleton stop or palatal affricate)? Two perception experiments address this point.

Trend Study

Method

Data were obtained through the recording of partially guided interviews with two representative samples of speakers designed to obtain social and stylistic variation. Both samples were stratified according to three social variables: age, gender, and education (Table 1). It should be noted that this particular study follows the theoretical and methodological framework of the international sociolinguistic research project on Latin American and Castilian Spanish (PRESEEA project) (Moreno-Fernández, Reference Moreno-Fernández1996).

Table 1. Tableau of parameters for the two Malaga sample surveys (1995 = 72: 4 speakers per cell; 2015 = 54: 3 speakers per cell)

1 Age is based on year of recording

The first sample was recorded in 1995 using a tape recorder (Sanyo®, TRC 1250), and it is composed of a total of seventy-two interviews (four speakers per cell in Table 1) which were digitized afterward using Audacity® (Vida-Castro, Ávila-Muñoz, Lasarte-Cervantes, Villena-Ponsoda, & Sánchez-Sáez, Reference Vida-Castro, Ávila-Muñoz, Cruz Lasarte-Cervantes, Villena-Ponsoda and Sánchez-Sáez2008). The second sample was recorded in 2015, and it consists of a set of fifty-four interviews (three speakers per cell in Table 1) taken with a digital recorder (Roland® R-05). The internal microphone of each recorder was used for the two samples. All the participants come from Malaga city, and the interviews last, approximately, forty-five minutes each. Apart from the partially guided interview, the informants were also asked to fill out a sociological and attitudinal questionnaire to complete the speaker's background and sociological information. In order to test the idea that the affricate allophone of /-s⋅t/ is an allegedly new variant in the speech community with an undetermined indexicality (H1), the social distribution of the variants of /-s⋅t/ in each sample was compared. With that purpose, a set of 4,894 tokens of /-s⋅t/ (2,750 taken from the 1995 sample and 2,144 tokens from the 2015 oneFootnote 3), were auditorily codified by the author of this paper into five perceptive allophonic categories:Footnote 4 sibilant [s⋅t], preaspiration [h⋅t], elision [ø], postaspiration [th], and affricate [ts]. The tokens were listened to in context and in order of appearance. Pearson's chi-squared and Cramer's V tests were used to measure associations between categorical variables, such as the distribution of allophonic categories across the two corpora (Levshina, Reference Levshina2015:199-222). Additionally, a mixed effects logistic regression model (Bates, Maechler, Bolker, & Walker, Reference Bates, Maechler, Bolker and Walker2015) was fitted in order to measure the effect size of the social variables. All the statistical analyses presented in the paper were conducted in R (R Core Team, 2019).

Results

As displayed in Figure 1, the affricate variant of /-s⋅t/ was not frequent in the 1995 sample (12.80%), where the postaspirated realization [th] (54.87%) and complete deletion (25.16%) were the two main allophones. However, during the last twenty years, the frequency of the affricate realization has sharply soared (34.19%) at the expense of both the deleted and the postaspirated allophone.

Figure 1. Effect of time in the allophonic variation of /-s⋅t/. Pearson's Chi-squared: 371.94 (4). Sig.: <.001. Cramer's V: .276. (1995 Corpus n = 2750; 2015 Corpus n = 2144).

Zooming in on the affricate variant, Figure 2 displays the individual probabilities of occurrence of this allophone. As it is apparent, older speakers were hardly using this allophone twenty years ago, and they seldom use it now, while the frequency of using this variant gradually rises among the youngest speakers of both samples.

Figure 2. By-speaker probability of affrication [ts] in apparent time.

However, the most striking shift can be found among the youngest female speakers. As readily observable in Table 2, in 1995 only young secondary- and university-level educated women tended to produce this allophone (0.31 and 0.40), while young primary-level educated women hardly used it (0.11). On the contrary, in 2015 a completely different pattern appears: although the use of the affricate variant has increased among all young women, those with the lowest levels of formal education are the ones who are currently leading the change (0.79). The use of the affricate allophone has also increased sharply among the primary-level educated youngest group of men (0.68), but not so much among the university-educated young men in 2015.

Table 2. Mean probability of affrication [ts] in every group of speakers in 1995 and in 2015

Table 3 presents the results of the mixed effect logistic regression analysis. The independent variables selected as fixed factors were ageFootnote 5 (continuous) and, as data come from two different samples, the interactions of corpus (1995 and 2015) with (1) gender (men and women) and (2) education (primary, secondary, and university).

Table 3. Summary of mixed effects logistic regression for [ts] realizations (versus [0+h+th+s]), speaker as random factor. Signif. codes: ‘***’ < .001 ‘**’ < .01 ‘*’< .05 ‘⋅’ < 0.1

The results suggest that age and corpus are the most significant factors that predict the realization of the affricate allophone, which is more likely to appear in the 2015 sample and always among the younger speakers (the odds of producing the affricate variant decrease as the age of the speaker rises). Although the tested interaction between gender and corpus is not statistically significant, the coefficients are consistent with the results displayed in Figure 2 and Table 2: the affricate realizations are more likely among women in the 2015 sample than they were in 1995. The main effect of education is superseded by the interaction with corpus: in 1995, speakers were more likely to produce the affricate allophone as their level of education increased while the opposite tendency is found in 2015: the odds of realizing the affricate variant decrease as the level of education rises. The R-squared values reveal that the 39% of the variation explained by the whole model (60%) is due to the fixed predictors, while the remaining 21% is due to individual variation (speaker).

Acoustic Analysis

Method

Materials and procedure. In order to answer RQ2, an acoustic analysis based on the 2015 sample was conducted. The 2015 sample was specifically designed to carry out this acoustic analysis of the /-s⋅t/ allophones, thus, along with the semistructured interview, every participant was asked to read aloud a reading passage, a list of sentences, and a wordlist. The main purpose of this reading task was to obtain a relevant number of the sibilant realization of /-s⋅t/, the standard variant, which is infrequent in casual styles (Vida-Castro, Reference Vida-Castro2004:117).

A set of 3,430 tokens from the 2015 sample (2,144 from the interviews—see previous section—and 1,286 from the reading tasks) were auditorily classified into the five perceptive allophonic categories described in the previous section (sibilant [s⋅t], preaspiration [h], elision [ø], postaspiration [th] and affricate [ts]) and segmented using Praat (Boersma & Weenink, Reference Boersma and Weenink2012). Every word under study was isolated in a separate sound file, and the boundaries for acoustic measure were placed by hand. The labeled segments were: (1) the sibilant [s] or aspiration [h] fricative noise (if any); and (2) the voice onset time (VOT) of the dental stop /t/. A Praat script (Elvira-García, Reference Elvira-García2019) was used to measure different acoustic parameters that are described in the following paragraphs and represented in Figure 3: (1) duration values; (2) spectral properties (center of gravity and spectral peak location); and (3) zero crossing rates.

Figure 3. Relevant acoustic features related to basic stages in the word-medial /-s + t/ resyllabification process.

Duration values are related to the duration (milliseconds, ms.) of the fricative noise. Coda Frication Time (CFT) refers to the fricative noise that appears before the stop closure (see spectrograms 1 and 2 in Figure 3). CFT is expected to be shorter in preaspiration [h] than in the sibilant allophone [s] (File-Muriel & Brown, Reference File-Muriel and Brown2011:230). Voice Onset Time (VOT) refers to the length of time between the release of the stop and the onset of the voicing. Plosive [t] is expected to present a shorter VOT than the postaspirated realization [th] (Ladefoged & Maddieson, Reference Ladefoged and Maddieson1996:66-73; Torreira, Reference Torreira, Prieto, Mascaró and Solé2007:72-77) and the affricate allophone is expected to display the longest values of VOT (Martínez-Celdrán & Fernández-Planas, Reference Martínez-Celdrán and Fernández-Planas2013:46-50).

Spectral properties are values that refer to the spectrum of frequencies (hertz, Hz.) in which fricative noise is produced. Center of Gravity (CoG)Footnote 6 is a measure of how high the frequencies in a spectrum are on average and the spectral peak location (Peak) is the period of the maximum (peak) energy density (intensity) in the spectrum. Fronted variants ([s] and [ts]) are expected to present a higher CoG and Peak than aspirated allophones [h] and [th] (Gordon, Barthmaier, & Sands, Reference Gordon, Barthmaier and Sands2002:166-70; Jongman, Wayland, & Wong, Reference Jongman, Wayland and Wong2000:1253-5).

A zero crossing is a point where the waveform crosses the horizontal axis. The zero crossing rate (ZCR) measurement considers the number of zero crossings along the whole interval of the fricative noise multiplied by ten and divided by the whole duration of the interval. ZCR is useful for quantifying the noise level that is present in consonants (Martínez-Celdrán, Reference Martínez-Celdrán2015:121-4). Thus, aspirated [h] and postaspirated variants [th] are expected to present lower rates of zero crossings than the sibilant [s] and affricate [ts] allophones.

It should be noted that spectral properties and zero crossing rates are subject to biological sex effects. As the sample is balanced for gender, the overall results are given in section 3.2, but values separated by gender can be found online in Appendices 1 and 2.

The main purposes of the acoustical scrutiny are related to H2: (1) to check to what extent the idea of the emergence of an allegedly new affricate allophone of /-s⋅t/ has a physical basis; and (2) to confirm the acoustical contrast among the different perceptive categories, especially the preaspirated versus the postaspirated ones.

Statistical analysis. In order to compare the acoustic features across the perceptual categories it was necessary to check the normality of the samples and their homoscedasticity first, so the Shapiro-Wilk test of normality and the Levene's test for the homogeneity of the variances were used to compare the mean values of every parameter across the five categories. As the results suggested that the assumptions of normality and homogeneity across the groups were not met, nonparametric tests were chosen to contrast the different measures: the Wilcoxon test was used to compare the acoustic features across two groups while the Asymptotic K-Sample Fisher-Pitman Permutation Test, a nonparametric ANOVA based on permutation, was used to contrast all the groups. Additionally, the post hoc Nonparametric Multiple Comparison Test for relative contrast effects (Tukey) was performed to find out which groups differ significantly. Additionally, Pearson's chi-squared and Cramer's V tests were also used to examine the relationship between categorical variables (Levshina, Reference Levshina2015:87-113, 171-222).

Results

According to the data shown in Table 4, where the distribution of the five allophonic categories of word-internal /-s⋅t/ is observed across two different styles (interview style and reading tasks), reading styles favor the sibilant allophone, while the interview style increases the occurrence of innovative postaspirated [th] (45.76%) and affricate [ts] (34.19%) variants. Deletion is not very frequent (10.82% of the total cases) and preaspiration is almost absent in this particular context.Footnote 7

Table 4. Effect of style on the allophonic variation of /-s⋅t/. Absolute and relative frequencies and statistical significance. Source: 2015 sample

As shown above (Figure 3), the location of fricative noise, either before the closure of the plosive (CFT) or immediately after its release (VOT), is the first and evident considerable difference among two main groups of allophones, that is, the postnuclear realizations, [s] and [h], and the postaspirated ones, [th] and [ts].

Sibilant [s] and preaspirated [h] are the only allophones that show fricative noise in postnuclear position. In Table 5, the values of the main acoustic features of coda fricative noise are shown: sibilant noise is longer, its center of gravity and spectral peak are located at higher frequencies, and it reveals a higher rate of zero crossings than the preaspirated variant. The Wilcoxon test shows significant statistical differences across the two contrasting categories.

Table 5. Acoustic correlates of coda fricative noise. Means, standard deviation and statistical significance (see Online Appendix 1 for values separated by gender)

When the acoustic features of the plosive [t] are compared across the categories (Table 6), statistically significant differences are also found for most of them. The results of the Fisher-Pitman and the post hoc Tukey tests revealed a set of findings that support H2.

Table 6. Acoustic correlates of onset fricative noise in /-s⋅t/ variants. Means, standard deviation and statistical significance (Online Appendix 2 for values separated by gender)

First, the affricate allophone [ts] specifically differs from each one of the /-s⋅t/ variants ([ts] ≠ [s⋅t]; [ts] ≠ [h⋅t]; [ts] ≠ [th]; [ts] ≠ ∅) for every acoustic feature (posthoc Tukey p-values < 0.001, including the contrast with the adjacent postaspirated category [th]). In fact, [ts] consistently shows longer VOT, higher frequency of both CoG and spectral peak of the fricative noise, as well as a bigger rate of zero crossing. This suggests that the existence of an affricate category, distinct from the traditional postaspirated variant, has a physical basis (H2).

Second, when the variants involved in the postaspiration process are observed, it is apparent that as the resyllabification process advances, the VOT of the following plosive /t/ becomes longer as well. Post hoc tests reveal that VOT length consistently contrasts “adjacent” categories: [s⋅t] < [h⋅t] < [th] < [ts] (p-values < 0.003 in every contrast).

Third, spectral measures and zero crossing rates do not contrast “adjacent” categories such as the sibilant and the preaspirated realizations: [s] = [h] (p-values > 0.23), nor the “pre” and “post” aspirated allophones [h] = [th] (p-values > 0.37). The only significant difference appears in the contrast of any of these groups with the affricate allophone [ts] (see first point above). The fricative noise found after the release of the plosive is produced at the highest average frequencies (CoG = 5029; Peak = 5478), which, in articulatory terms, means that it is produced in a relatively more fronted position than the rest of the groups (Jongman et al., Reference Jongman, Wayland and Wong2000:1253; Martínez-Celdrán & Fernández-Planas, Reference Martínez-Celdrán and Fernández-Planas2013:107).

Perception Experiments

The idea that the postaspirated [th] and affricate [ts] sounds are the result of a natural and optimal resyllabification process can also be explored from the listener's perspective. Two different perception experiments were conducted in order to show to what extent listeners from the speech community can recognize the postaspirated sounds as allophones of /-s⋅t/ segments, such that the articulation of a sequence such as [ˈmo⋅tho] or [ˈmo⋅tso] is associated to the word mosto /ˈmos⋅to/ ‘grape juice,’ and not to the word moto /ˈmo⋅to/ ‘motorbike’ or mocho /ˈmo⋅t͡ʃo/, ‘mop’ (H3).

Perception experiment 1

Method. The first test was a pilot experiment. A digital recorder (Roland® 05) was used to record the stimuli, a set of ten words pertaining to four minimal pairs or triplets (Table 7) produced by a female speaker from Malaga who realized the segment /-s⋅t/ with a postaspirated allophone ([ts]). Together with the ten target stimuli, a group of ten distractors was included. Each word was realized inside the carrier sentence Digo la palabra… (‘I say the word…’) which was recorded for each item and played unaltered to the listeners. The order was randomized once, so that members of the triplets/pairs were never adjacent to each other in the trial list and all the informants listened to the words in the same order.

Table 7. List of stimuli for perception experiment 1: target stimuli, distractors and order of presentation to listeners

The listeners were the same fifty-four informants who composed the 2015 sample (see Trend Study section). All of them were tested individually just after their interviews. They were asked to write the word they heard on an answer sheet (where no choices were presented) immediately after listening to each sentence only once. An external speaker (Mini Monster Beatbox®) connected to the recorder was used to play the sentences.

Results

The results of the first perception experiment (Table 8) showed a high level of accuracy (90%) in associating the stimuli to their corresponding words. This is not surprising as the stimuli were not manipulated, and the listeners could find phonetic cues in the previous vowel that may facilitate the proper association. But there was an interesting exception related to the minimal pair castillo ‘castle’/cachillo ‘little piece.’ In this case, the word cachillo /ka⋅ˈʧi⋅ ʝo/ (realized with a dentoalveolar articulation) was interpreted as castillo /kas⋅ˈti⋅ ʝo/ by 29.63% of the listeners (see Ruch [Reference Ruch2008:62] for very similar results). This result suggests that a dentoalveolar affricate sound may be associated with the /-s⋅t/ segment, facilitating the path to the resyllabification process.

Table 8. Answers to perception experiment 1. Total n of responses per word = 54. The accurate answers are shadowed

Perception experiment 2

Stimuli, subjects and procedure. The second perception test was prepared after measuring the acoustic correlates of the different variants of /-s⋅t/ (see Acoustic Analysis section) and once the data from the pilot experiment had been analyzed. The main goal was to find out which acoustic cues, if any, enable the implicit association of an affricate sound with either the Spanish palatal phoneme /t͡ʃ/—whose occurrence is limited to the syllable-onset position–or the sequence /-s⋅t/, in which the alveolar fricative phoneme /s/ belongs to the coda of the previous syllable.

It should be noted that statistically significant acoustic differences exist between the most frequent realization of the palatal affricate phoneme /t͡ʃ/ and the affricate variant [ts] of the /-s⋅t/ sequence (Table 9). Therefore, the main hypothesis here is that listeners are able to recognize those differences and, hence, associate each sound to the corresponding category (H3).

Table 9. Acoustic correlates of affricate sounds. Means, standard deviation, and statistical significance. Source: 2015 sample (see Trend Study section)

As the main differences between the two affricate sounds in contrast (Table 9) are the spectral measures (CoG and Peak) and VOT duration, a set of nine affricate stimuli was prepared (Table 10). First, a student of Spanish philology, a male speaker from Malaga (who also realized the /-s⋅t/ segment with an affricate pronunciation [ts]) was trained to read a total of twenty-eight words and nonce-words in the frame sentence Digo la palabra (‘I say the word’) with different phonetic realizations (e.g.: [ˈmata] - [ˈmasta] - [ˈmatsa] - [ˈmaʧa]).Footnote 8 The sounds were taken with a digital-recorder (Roland® 05) and then acoustically analyzed with Praat using the same procedure explained in the Acoustic Analysis section. After the analysis, three fricative segments were selected based on their respective CoG and Peak values to synthesize the manipulated stimuli. Their duration was altered using the Manipulation function to generate three new sound objects out of each one (three natural speech sounds x three manipulations = nine altered stimuli) with different duration values. The manipulated sounds were analyzed again to test the CoG and Peak mean values.

Table 10. Groups of stimuli generated for the perception test

As a result, the affricate stimuli in the perception experiment can be organized in three groups respectively labeled as apical, dentoalveolar, and palatal. The sounds of the apical group show very high frequencies of the correlates of fricative noise (CoG. 6500 Hz., Peak 6000 Hz.) while the dentoalveolar and the palatal group of sounds feature fricative noise at relatively lower frequencies. As shown in Table 10, nine affricate sounds with different VOT durations were created.

The Edit function was used to insert each stimulus after the plosive sound in two different sequences: [ˈmat_a] and [ˈpat_a]. The phonetic context was always the same recorded sequence of sounds that was always included in the same recorded frame sentence, so any other phonetic cues apart from the stimuli were blocked. The resulting sequence masta corresponds to the minimal pair masta/macha (which are nonce-words in southern Spanish), but the second series (pasta/pacha) contains an available and very frequent unit in Malaga variety (pasta ‘dough’) opposed to pacha, which is not a word (Ávila-Muñoz & Villena-Ponsoda, Reference Ávila-Muñoz and Villena-Ponsoda2010).

Subsequently, an online questionnaire was prepared using LimeSurvey®. The survey included twelve questions about the origin and sociological background of the participant, eighteen queries with the target stimuli and twelve fillers.Footnote 8 The thirty sounds were randomized once so all the informants listened to the stimuli in the same order (online Appendix 3).

A total of seventy-one subjects from Malaga participated (forty-seven women and twenty-four men; thirty-nine younger than thirty-five and thirty-two older than thirty-five). All used their own computer to complete the test. In the instructions they were informed about the task: click on a sound icon, listen to the sentence and then click on the sequence they had perceived among three options they were presented with.Footnote 9 They were informed that some of the sounds could be “meaningless words.”

Statistical Analysis. The association between the listeners’ responses and the stimuli was first measured using Pearson's chi-squared and Cramer's V tests. Then, as the participants could choose among three options (a stop, a /-s⋅t/ sequence or a palatal affricate, that is, pata/pasta/pacha), a multinomial logistic regression model was fitted (Levshina, Reference Levshina2015:277-89). Palatal response /ʧ/ was taken as the reference level, so the model tests how the predictors influence the selection of a /-s⋅t/ sequence versus the palatal affricate sound and, separately, the choice of a plosive /t/ versus the affricate option. The independent variables selected as predictors were duration (treated here as a continuous variable), stimulus type (apical, dental, or palatal), lexical frame ([pa_a] or [ma_a]) and subject age group. Additionally, interactions were tested between independent variables.

Results

According to Table 11, which shows the distribution of the participant responses across the different stimuli, palatal sounds are mostly related to the Spanish palatal phoneme /ʧ/, whereas those with the highest spectral values (apical group) are usually associated to the /-s⋅t/ sequence, especially the two longest stimuli of this group. Therefore, fricative noise duration seems to be the factor that determines phoneme discrimination among the dentoalveolar group of stimuli: sounds with longer fricative noise are usually categorized as allophones of the palatal phoneme /ʧ/ while those with the shortest noise are identified as occurrences of the /-s⋅t/ phonological segment.

Table 11. Effect of noise duration and spectral values on the phoneme discrimination. Absolute and relative frequencies and statistical significance

The results of the multinomial regression analysis (Table 12) suggest that whether the participants listened to a fronted stimulus (apical or dental) instead of a palatal one, the chances of associating it to an /-s⋅t/ sequence or even to a single plosive /t/ significantly increased. On the contrary, as the duration of the stimulus increases, it is less likely to get an /-s⋅t/ or a /t/ response instead of a /ʧ/ one. However, this effect is superseded by the interaction with the stimulus type: the association of longer sounds to an /-s⋅t/ response increases whenever the participants listen to an apical stimulus instead of a palatal one. Lexical frame does not significantly predict the participant responses, but when the stimulus type changes from palatal to dental, the biased context results in more /-s⋅t/ sequences associations. Finally, although the participants’ age group does not predict the responses, longer stimuli are associated to /-s⋅t/ sequences when they are presented to the youngest group of participants.

Table 12. Summary of multinomial regression for /-st/ (left) and /t/ (right) responses versus /ʧ/ responses (reference level: /ʧ/ responses)

Frequencies of alternative responses: /ʧ/ = 644 (50%); /—s⋅t/ = 405 (31%); /t/ = 229 (18%); Total n: 1278

Log-Likelihood: -993.28 McFadden R2: 0.23624 Likelihood ratio test : chisq = 614.46 p.value = < .001

Signif. codes: ‘***’ < .001 ‘**’ < .01 ‘*’< .05 ‘⋅’ < 0.1

Discussion

H1 A new allophone with an undetermined indexicality

Although the affricate outcome of the resyllabification process seems to have historical precedents in Indo-European languages, it had not been documented as an independent allophone in the landscape of the innovative varieties of Spanish, where the lenition process of /-s/ takes place. Recent studies indicate that the affricate variant appears in western Andalusian Spanish during the last decades (Moya-Corral, Reference Moya-Corral2007:461; Ruch, Reference Ruch2008:101-102; Ruch & Peters, Reference Ruch and Peters2016:13). The results of the trend study in Malaga city provide confirmation: the affricate allophone, which rarely appeared in 1995 (12.8% of the total cases) and only among the youngest, mainly university-educated speakers, has increased sharply during the last twenty years, and it is now the preferred realization of young less-educated speakers. This could support an interpretation of [ts] as a change from below (Labov, Reference Labov2006:203-209).

The shift leading to the resyllabification process could have originated in the context of the regional koine, the intermediate variety emerging among middle-class young speakers in southern Spanish, which can be–as is the case with similar varieties in different contexts–an enabling environment to spread and develop linguistic innovations (Auer, Reference Auer, Delbecque, van der Auwera and Geeraerts2005:25). In 2015, noneducated speakers were the ones leading the change, but as the resyllabification process is also frequent among young educated speakers and middle-aged men, it seems reasonable that competing indexicalities are related to this brand-new allophone (Eckert, Reference Eckert2012:90).

When exploring the social meanings of several features of the Malaga variety, Chariatte (Reference Chariatte2015:133, 137, 144, 155) observes that the affricate variant of /-s⋅t/ is recognized by the speech community as one of its most salient and widespread characteristics and, at the same time, as a salient feature of the Malaga middle-class variety (see also Ruch, Reference Ruch2018). According to all of this, it may be assumed that, although speakers are nowadays quite aware of the existence of this feature in the Malaga variety, its indexicality is not yet well-defined. Although further perceptual (Chariatte, Reference Chariatte2015), stylistic (Eckert, Reference Eckert2012), and ethnographic (Sneller, Reference Sneller2020) investigation about this trait's indexicality is needed, one likely explanation would follow the ensuing line of reasoning:

  1. (1) Resyllabified affricate /ts/ would be intended as a natural, unmarked solution to balance, on the one hand, the southern tendency to delete consonants in coda position and, on the other, the educated urban middle-class trend toward standardization, that is, maintenance of the underlying form of the word, close to the mainstream norm of pronunciation. This urban, modern, standard-like indexicality would likely be sustained originally by [ts] and expanded, especially, by young speakers.

  2. (2) Once [ts] was established, a competing indexicality would have started among lower-class speakers, particularly prone to use unmarked phonetic outcomes. Regardless of its alleged faithfulness to the underlying form of the word, [ts] seems to currently be acquiring a quite different indexicality, probably associated with covert prestige and the local speech community.

H2 The resyllabification process

The acoustic analysis reinforces the idea that the affricate variant is the optimal result of a resyllabification process. As shown in Table 6, the affricate allophone's acoustic correlates significantly differ from the rest of /-s⋅t/ realizations, even from the very similar postaspirated [th] variant. Furthermore, its structure and physical characteristics support the idea that postaspirated variants are the outcome of a natural shift in the phasing relationship of the articulatory gestures (Parrell, Reference Parrell2012), from a sequential production in the preaspirated variants (the aspiration precedes the oral closure of the plosive [h⋅t]) to a simultaneous articulation in the postaspirated allophones (where both gestures begin at the same time). Additionally, it could be assumed that /-s⋅t/ word-medial articulation is undergoing an assimilatory process (Martínez-Celdrán & Fernández-Planas, Reference Martínez-Celdrán and Fernández-Planas2013:113), where the place of articulation of the first consonant (the sibilant [s] or its lenited variant [h]) blends with the adjacent one during its entire production. The final outcome is a natural and unmarked result from a gradual assimilatory process that maintains on the surface the phonological representation of the underlying form (sibilant fricative /-s/) in an open-syllable structure.

H3 Categorizing affricate sounds

According to the results of the two perception experiments, in Malaga coronal affricate sounds with particular acoustic characteristics (mainly, high rates of spectral values–CoG and Peak) were perceived as an /-s⋅t/ sequence, which contains a segment that belongs to a postnuclear syllabic position in the underlying representation. These results, in line with those reported by Ruch and Harrington (Reference Ruch and Harrington2014) and Ruch and Peters (Reference Ruch and Peters2016), reinforce the idea that the resyllabified allophones constitute an optimal output, not only from an articulatory perspective, but also from a perceptual one, as they maintain the phonological representation of the underlying forms.

It seems expected that listeners will be more likely to identify the /-s⋅t/ sequence (against any other possible candidate with /ʧ/) whenever a dental stimulus occurs in a frequent and recognizable /-s⋅t/ word, the opposite being also true, that is, the unlikeliness of successful identification of rare and infrequent words (Ganong, Reference Ganong1980). It may be assumed that speakers associate certain affricate sounds with /-s⋅t/ sequences in the right circumstances. Furthermore, the relationship between age of subject and response is a straightforward reflection of the change in progress.

Conclusion

This paper explored the internal and external factors that, according to the available data, are triggering the resyllabification process of word-internal /-s/ when followed by the voiceless dental plosive /t/. The results obtained so far allow us to conclude that the development of a resyllabified affricate allophone is based on the convergence of, at least, two complementary conditions: (1) it is a natural articulatory outcome (Parrell, Reference Parrell2012) (2) that finds, in a context of koineization, a favorable environment to spread (Auer, Reference Auer, Delbecque, van der Auwera and Geeraerts2005).

In addition, the results obtained in the perception test indicate that the acoustical cues defining the new affricate sound [ts] lead speakers to perceive an /-s⋅t/ sequence, especially by the youngest listeners. It can thus be argued that the affricate allophone is an optimal output regarding syllabic restrictions, since the sound that stems from the resyllabification process maintains all the phonological information pertaining to the underlying form (faithfulness constraints in Optimality Theory terms). The fact that this information is analyzed as part of the onset of the following syllable makes it possible to create an open-syllable structure (well-formedness restrictions) without losing any relevant semantic information. The assimilatory shift is not only natural from the production perspective but also when linguistic universal constraints are regarded. Finally, the results of the trend study indicate that the indexicality of this new allophone, which can be related neither to the standard nor to local vernacular use, is undetermined.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954394522000084

Acknowledgments

Data and results discussed in this article are based on the Spanish Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovación research projects El Español de Málaga: Procesos de Variación y Cambio Espaciales y Sociales (VARES-AGENDA 50, PID2019-104982GB-C52) and Sociolinguistic Patterns of Castilian Spanish (ECOPASOS, FFI2015-68171-C5-1). Funding for open access charge: Universidad de Málaga / CBUA. I am truly indebted to Juan Andrés Villena-Ponsoda for all his generosity, continuous feedback, and statistical guidance. I am also grateful to three anonymous reviewers for their invaluable input and to all the informants from Malaga who so kindly participated in this project. All errors remain my own.

Competing interests

The author declares none

Footnotes

1. It should be noted that word-final /-s/ can have a grammatical function as a plural marker in a noun (libro/libros, ‘book/books’) or a second person singular marker in a verb (amas/ama, ‘you love/he loves’).

2. The development of intermediate coherent varieties in Europe is a well-known and thoroughly studied process (Cerruti & Tsiplakou, Reference Cerruti and Tsiplakou2020). See also Chela-Flores (Reference Chela-Flores2017) for a different perspective on these issues.

3. A minimum of thirty tokens per participant was established for the codification. 1995 sample: mean = 38.19 tokens per participant, sd = 6.35; 2015 sample: mean = 39.70 tokens per participant, sd = 9.92.

4. 5.4% of the sample was codified again by another researcher. The intercoder percent agreement was 94.1%.

5. Age of the speaker at the time of recording.

6. CoG is one of the so-called spectral moments (Forrest, Weismer, Milenkovic, & Dougall, Reference Forrest, Weismer, Milenkovic and Dougall1988). The other three, Standard deviation, Skewness, and Kurtosis, were also measured for this analysis, and the statistical differences found among the perceptual variants turned out to be similar when any of the spectral moments is considered. However, since CoG showed the most highly significant differences between the categories, it is the only spectral moment reported in this paper.

7. It should be noted that no other linguistic factors (previous phonetic environment, stress, etc.) proved to be related to allophonic variation in this particular context.

8. List of words and nonwords (fillers are in bold): [ˈmata] [ˈmasta] [ˈmatsa] [ˈmaʧa]; [ˈmoto] [ˈmosto] [ˈmotso] [ˈmoʧo]; [ˈcato] [ˈcasto] [ˈcatso] [ˈcaʧo]; [ˈteto] [ˈtesto] [ˈtetso] [ˈtecho]; [piˈton] [pisˈton] [piˈtson] [piˈʧon]; [ˈpata] [ˈpasta] [ˈpatsa] [ˈpaʧa]; [ˈmuto] [ˈmusto] [ˈmutso] [ˈmuʧo].

9. Both pairs are actually minimal triplets (pata / pasta / pacha and mata / masta / macha), mata (‘bush’) and pata (‘paw’) are Spanish words.

References

Alba, Orlando. (1990). Variación fonética y diversidad social en el español dominicano de Santiago. Santiago de los Caballeros: Universidad de Santiago de los Caballeros.Google Scholar
Alonso, Amado. (1967). Estudios lingüísticos. Temas españoles. Madrid: Gredos.Google Scholar
Alvar, Manuel. (1955). Las Hablas Meridionales. Revista de Filología Española XXXIX:284313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alvar, Manuel, Llorente, Antonio, & Salvador, Gregorio. (1973). Atlas lingüístico y etnográfico de Andalucía. Granada: Universidad de Granada.Google Scholar
Auer, Peter. (2005). Europe's sociolinguistic unity, or: A typology of European dialect / standard constellations. In Delbecque, N., van der Auwera, J., & Geeraerts, D. (eds.), Perspectives on variation. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. 742.Google Scholar
Ávila-Muñoz, Antonio, & Villena-Ponsoda, Juan-Andrés (eds.). (2010). Variación social del léxico disponible en la ciudad de Málaga. Diccionario y análisis. Málaga: Sarriá.Google Scholar
Bates, Douglas, Maechler, Martin, Bolker, Ben, & Walker, Steve. (2015). Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 67(1):148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blanco-Canales, Ana. (2004). Estudio sociolingüístico de Alcalá de Henares. Alcalá de Henares: Universidad de Alcalá.Google Scholar
Boersma, Paul, Dekkers, Joost, & van de Weijer, Jeroen. (2000). Optimality theory. Phonology, syntax and acquisition. In Dekkers, J., van der Leeuw, F., & van de Weijer, J. (eds.), Optimality theory. Phonology, sintax and acquisition. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 144.Google Scholar
Boersma, Paul, & Weenink, David. (2012). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (5.3.04) [Computer Software]. http://www.praat.org.Google Scholar
Brown, Esther L., & Torres-Cacoullos, Rena. (2003). Spanish /s/: A different story from beginning (initial) to end (final). In Nuñez-Cedeño, R., López, L., & Cameron, R. (eds.), A Romance perspective on language knowledge and use: Selected papers from the 31st Linguistic Symposium on Romance Languages (LRSL). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 2138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan, & Hopper, Paul (eds). (2001). Frequency and the emergence of linguistic structure. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cedergren, Henrietta J. (1972). Interplay of social and linguistic factors in Panama. Doctoral dissertation, Cornell University.Google Scholar
Cerruti, Maximo, & Tsiplakou, Stavroula (eds). (2020). Intermediate language varieties. Koinai and regional standards in Europe. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chariatte, Nadine. (2015). El malagueño real, mental y virtual. Doctoral dissertation, University of Bern.Google Scholar
Chela-Flores, Godsuno. (2017). Language ideologies, intervarietal conflict and their repercussions on language and society: the case of the Hispanic dialect complex. Dialectologia et Geolinguistica 25 (1):123–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clements, George N. (1990). The role of the sonority cycle in core syllabification. In Kingston, J., & Beckman, M. E. (eds.), Papers in Laboratory Phonology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 283333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donni de Mirande, Nélida E. (1968). El español hablado en Rosario. Rosario: Universidad del Litoral, Instituto de Lingüística y Filología.Google Scholar
Donni de Mirande, Nélida E. (1989). El segmento fonológico “s” en el español de Rosario (Argentina). Lingüística Española Actual 11 (1):89116.Google Scholar
Eckert, Penelope. (2012). Three Waves of Variation Study: The Emergence of Meaning in the Study of Sociolinguistic Variation. Annual Review of Anthropology 41 (1):87100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elvira-García, Wendy. (2019). Zero-crossing-and-spectral-moments, v.1.3. [Praat script] https://www.ub.edu/phoneticslaboratory/praatscripts/zero-crossing-and-spectral-moments.praat.Google Scholar
Erker, Daniel (2010). A subsegmental approach to coda /s/ weakening in Dominican Spanish. International Journal of the Sociology of Language 203:926.Google Scholar
Fernández de Molina-Ortés, Elena. (2016). El nivel social como indicador de la variación de -/s/ en el habla de Mérida (Badajoz). Dialectologia 16:93116.Google Scholar
File-Muriel, Richard, & Brown, Earl K. (2011). The gradient nature of s-lenition in Caleño Spanish. Language Variation and Change 23:223–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forrest, Karen, Weismer, Gary, Milenkovic, Paul, & Dougall, Ronald N.. (1988). Statistical analysis of word-initial voiceless obstruents: Preliminary data. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 84:115–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ganong, William F. (1980). Phonetic categorization in auditory word perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 6 (1):110–25.Google ScholarPubMed
Gilles, Peter, & Hinskens, Frans. (2019). Two processes of word-final n-deletion in Central Franconian dialects, their distribution, their conditioning and what keeps them together, underground and at the surface. 10 International Conference on Language Variation in Europe (ICLAVE 10). Leeuwarden.Google Scholar
Gloning, Thomas, & Young, Christopher. (2003). A History of the German Language Through Texts. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Goldsmith, John A. (1990). Autosegmental and metrical phonology. London: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Gómez-Serrano, Antonio. (1993). Aspectos del habla de Linares (Jaén). Málaga: Universidad de Málaga.Google Scholar
Gordon, Matthew, Barthmaier, Paul, & Sands, Kathy. (2002). A cross-linguistic acoustic study of voiceless fricatives. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 32 (2):141–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guy, Gregory R. (1980). Variation in the group and the individual: the case of final stop deletion. In Labov, W. (ed.) Locating language in time and space. New York: Academic Press. 136.Google Scholar
Gylfadottir, Duna. (2019). A simplification analysis of Andalusian aspirated stop clusters. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 146 (4):3009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hinskens, Frans. (2009). The erosion of a variable process. The case of n-deletion in Ripuarian and Limburg dialects of Dutch. In Kügler, F., Féry, C., & van de Vijver, R. (eds.), Variation and Gradience in Phonetics and Phonology. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. 311–50.Google Scholar
Hinskens, Frans, & van Hout, Roeland. (1994). Testing theoretical phonological aspects of word-final (t) deletion. In Viereck, W. (ed.), Verhandlungen des internationalen Dialektologenkongresses. Bamberg, 1990, vol. 3, ZDL 76. Stuttgart: Steiner. 297310.Google Scholar
Jiménez, Jesús. (1995). El aplecs ts i ds en valencià. Una anàlisi des de la teoria de la optimitat. Capilletra. Revista Internacional de Filologia 19:81112.Google Scholar
Jiménez, Jesús, & Lloret, Maria-Rosa. (2011). Sonicidad y tipología de los márgenes silábicos. In Val-Álvaro, J. F. (ed.), De la unidad del lenguaje a la diversidad de las lenguas. Actas del 10o Congreso Internacional de Lingüística General. Zaragoza: Universidad de Zaragoza. 440–55.Google Scholar
Jongman, Allard, Wayland, Ratree, & Wong, Serena. (2000). Acoustic characteristics of English fricatives. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 108 (3):1252–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kallen, Jeffrey L. (2005). Internal and external factors in phonological convergence: the case of English /t/ lenition. In Auer, P., Hinskens, F. & Kerswill, P. (eds.), Dialect change. Convergence and Divergence in European Languages. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 5180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kapović, Marco. (2014). Análisis sociolingüístico de la variable (s) en el habla de Ciudad Real. Doctoral dissertation, Zadar.Google Scholar
Labov, William. (1972). Sociolinguistic pattterns. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Labov, William. (2006). The social stratification of English in New York City. 2nd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ladefoged, Peter, & Maddieson, Ian. (1996). The sounds of the world languages. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Levshina, Natalia. (2015). How to do linguistics with R. Data exploration and statistical analysis. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
López-Morales, Humberto. (1983). Estratificación social del español de San Juan de Puerto Rico. Mexico City: Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México.Google Scholar
Maddieson, Ian. (1984). Patterns of sounds. New Haven: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martín-Butragueño, Pedro. (1991). Desarrollos sociolingüísticos en una comunidad de habla. Doctoral dissertation, Universidad Complutense.Google Scholar
Martínez-Celdrán, Eugenio. (2015). Naturaleza fonética de la consonante ‘ye’ en español. Normas 5:117–31.Google Scholar
Martínez-Celdrán, Eugenio & Fernández-Planas, Ana-Maria. (2013). Manual de fonética española. Articulaciones y sonidos del español. Barcelona: Ariel.Google Scholar
Meringer, Rudolf. (1923). Lingüística Indoeuropea. Madrid: Victoriano Suárez.Google Scholar
Meyer-Lübke, Wilhelm. (1934). Historische Grammatik der französischen Sprache. Heidelberg: C. Winter.Google Scholar
Molina-Martos, Isabel. (1998). La fonética de Toledo. Contexto geográfico y social. Alcalá de Henares: Universidad de Alcalá.Google Scholar
Molina-Martos, Isabel. (2015). La variable sociolingüística /-s/ en el distrito de Vallecas (Madrid). In Cestero-Mancera, A. M., Molina-Martos, I., & Paredes-García, F. (eds.), Patrones sociolingüísticos de Madrid. Bern: Peter Lang. 91116.Google Scholar
Moreno-Fernández, Francisco. (1996). Metodología del “Proyecto para el Estudio Sociolingüístico del Español de España y de América”. Lingüística, 8:257–87.Google Scholar
Moya-Corral, Juan-Antonio. (1979). La pronunciación del español en Jaén. Granada: Universidad de Granada.Google Scholar
Moya-Corral, Juan-Antonio. (2007). Noticia de un sonido emergente: la africada dental procedente del grupo -st- en Andalucía. Revista de Filología 25:457–65.Google Scholar
Moya-Corral, Juan-Antonio, & de la Sierra Tejada-Giráldez, María. (2020). Patterns of the linguistic change in Andalusia. Spanish in Context 17 (2):200–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Naro, Anthony J. (1981). The social and structural dimensions of a syntactic change. Language 57/1:6398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neu, Helene. (1980). Ranking of constraints on /t, d/ deletion in American English. A statistical analysis. In Labov, W. (ed.), Locating language in time and space. New York: Academic Press. 3754.Google Scholar
O'Neill, Paul. (2010). Variación y cambio en las consonantes oclusivas del español de Andalucía. Estudios de fonética experimental 19:1141.Google Scholar
Parrell, Benjamin. (2012). The role of gestural phasing in Western Andalusian Spanish aspiration. Journal of Phonetics 40 (1):3745.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penny, Ralph. (2002). A History of the Spanish Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Politzer, Robert L. (1947). Final /-s/ in the Romania. Romanic Review XXXVIII:156–66.Google Scholar
Poplack, Shana. (1979). Function and process in a variable phonology. Doctoral dissertation, University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Prince, Alan & Smolensky, Paul. (2004). Optimality Theory Constraint Interaction in Generative Grammar. Oxford: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raymond, William D., Dautricourt, Robin, & Hume, Elizabeth. (2006). Word-internal /t,d/ deletion in spontaneous speech: Modeling the effects of extra-linguistic, lexical, and phonological factors. Language Variation and Change 18 (1):5597.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
R Core Team. (2019). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
Rogers, Derek & d'Arcangeli, Luciana. (2004). Italian. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 34 (1):117–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruch, Hanna. (2008). La variante [ts] en el español de la ciudad de Sevilla. Aspectos fonético-fonológicos y sociolingüísticos de un sonido innovador. Doctoral dissertation, Universidad de Zürich.Google Scholar
Ruch, Hanna. (2013). Investigating a gradual metathesis: Production and perception of /s/ aspiration in Andalusian Spanish. Penn Working Papers in Linguistics 19 (2):171–80.Google Scholar
Ruch, Hanna. (2018). Perception of speaker age and speaker origin in a sound change in progress: The case of /s/-aspiration in Andalusian Spanish. Journal of Linguistic Geography (2018) 6. 4055.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruch, Hanna, & Harrington, Jonathan. (2014). Synchronic and diachronic factors in the change from pre-aspiration to post-aspiration in Andalusian Spanish. Journal of Phonetics 45 (1):1225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruch, Hanna, & Peters, Sandra. (2016). On the Origin of Postaspirated Stops: Production and Perception of /s/ + Voiceless Stop Sequences in Andalusian Spanish. Laboratory Phonology 7 (1):136.Google Scholar
Samper-Padilla, José-Antonio. (1990). Estudio sociolingüístico del español de Las Palmas de Gran Canaria. Las Palmas de Gran Canaria: La Caja de Canarias.Google Scholar
Sankoff, David. (1986). Ordenamiento de reglas variables: /r/ implosiva en un dialecto puertorriqueño. In Núñez-Cedeño, R. A., Páez-Urdaneta, I., & Guitart, J.M. (eds.), Estudios sobre la fonología del español del Caribe. Caracas: La Casa de Bello. 109–15.Google Scholar
Scherre, Maria Marta Pereira, & Naro, Anthony J.. (1991). Marking in discourse: “Birds of a feather.” Language Variation and Change 3:2332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silverstein, Michael. (2003). Indexical order and the dialectics of sociolinguistic life. Language and Communication 23 (3–4):193229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sneller, Betsy. (2020). Phonological rule spreading across hostile lines: (TH)-fronting in Philadelphia. Language Variation and Change, 32:2547.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tejada-Giráldez, María de la Sierra. (2015). Convergencia y divergencia entre comunidades de habla: a propósito de la /s/ implosiva. Contribución al estudio de los patrones sociolingüísticos del español de Granada. Doctoral dissertation, Universidad de Granada.Google Scholar
Terrell, Tracy D. (1978). Sobre la aspiración y elisión de /s/ en el español porteño. Anuario de Letras XVI:4166.Google Scholar
Terrell, Tracy D. (1979). Problemas de los estudios cuantitativos de procesos fonológicos variables: datos del Caribe hispánico. Boletín de la Academia Puertorriqueña de la Lengua Española VII:145–65.Google Scholar
Torreira, Francisco. (2006). Coarticulation between aspirated-s and voiceless stops in Spanish: An interdialectal comparison. In Sagarra, N., & Toribio, A. J. (eds.) Selected Proceedings of the 9th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium. 113–20.Google Scholar
Torreira, Francisco. (2007). Pre- and postaspirated stops in andalusian Spanish. In Prieto, P., Mascaró, J., & Solé, M. J. (eds.), Prosodic and segmental issues in Romance. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 6782.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torreira, Francisco. (2012). Investigating the nature of aspirated stops in Western Andalusian Spanish. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 42 (1):4963.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Väänänem, Veikko. (1950). A propos de l's final das les langues romances. Boletín de Filología XI:3140.Google Scholar
Vida-Castro, Matilde. (2004). Estudio sociofonológico del español hablado en la ciudad de Málaga. Condicionamientos sobre la variación de /-s/ en la distensión silábica. Alicante: Universidad de Alicante.Google Scholar
Vida-Castro, Matilde. (2016). Correlatos acústicos y factores sociales en la aspiración de /s/ preoclusiva en la variedad de Málaga (España). Análisis de un cambio en curso. Lingua Americana, 38:1536Google Scholar
Vida-Castro, Matilde. (2018). Variación y cambio fonético en la aspiración de /s/ preoclusiva. Caracterización acústica y distribución social en la ciudad de Málaga. In Ortega Arjonilla, E. (ed.), El paisaje. Percepciones interdisciplinares desde las Humanidades. Granada: Comares. 101–09.Google Scholar
Vida-Castro, Matilde, Ávila-Muñoz, Antonio, Cruz Lasarte-Cervantes, M., Villena-Ponsoda, Juan-Andrés, & Sánchez-Sáez, José-María (eds.). (2008). El español hablado en Málaga. Corpus oral para su estudio sociolingüístico I, II, III. Málaga: Sarriá.Google Scholar
Villena-Ponsoda, Juan-Andrés. (1996). Convergence and divergence in a standard dialect-continuum: networks and individuals in Malaga. Sociolinguistica 10:112–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Villena-Ponsoda, Juan-Andrés. (2018). The dilemma of the reliability of geolinguistic and dialectological data for sociolinguistic research. The case of the Andalusian demerger of /θ/. Acta Linguistica Lithuanica LXXIX:936.Google Scholar
Villena-Ponsoda, Juan Andrés, & Matilde Vida-Castro, Matilde. (2017). Between local and standard varieties. Horizontal and vertical convergence and divergence of dialects in Southern Spain. In Buchstaller, I., & Siebenhaar, B. (eds.), Language Variation - European Perspectives VI. Selected papers from the Eighth International Conference on Language Variation in Europe (ICLaVE 8). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 125–40.Google Scholar
Villena-Ponsoda, Juan-Andrés, & Vida-Castro, Matilde. (2020). Variation, identity and indexicality in southern Spanish. On the emergence of a new variety in urban Andalusia. In Cerruti, M., & Tsiplakou, S. (eds.), Intermediate language varieties. Koinai and regional standards in Europe. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 150–83.Google Scholar
von Wartburg, Walter. (1952). La fragmentación lingüística de la Romania. Madrid: Gredos.Google Scholar
West, Helen-Faye. (2013). A town between dialects, psycho-social orientation and identity in Merseyside, UK. In Auer, P., Caro-Reina, J., & Kaufmann, G. (ed.), Language Variation-European Perspectives IV. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 247–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolfram, Walter A. (1969). A sociolinguistic description of Detroit Negro speech. Washington DC: Center for Applied Linguistics.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Tableau of parameters for the two Malaga sample surveys (1995 = 72: 4 speakers per cell; 2015 = 54: 3 speakers per cell)

Figure 1

Figure 1. Effect of time in the allophonic variation of /-s⋅t/. Pearson's Chi-squared: 371.94 (4). Sig.: <.001. Cramer's V: .276. (1995 Corpus n = 2750; 2015 Corpus n = 2144).

Figure 2

Figure 2. By-speaker probability of affrication [ts] in apparent time.

Figure 3

Table 2. Mean probability of affrication [ts] in every group of speakers in 1995 and in 2015

Figure 4

Table 3. Summary of mixed effects logistic regression for [ts] realizations (versus [0+h+th+s]), speaker as random factor. Signif. codes: ‘***’ < .001 ‘**’ < .01 ‘*’< .05 ‘⋅’ < 0.1

Figure 5

Figure 3. Relevant acoustic features related to basic stages in the word-medial /-s + t/ resyllabification process.

Figure 6

Table 4. Effect of style on the allophonic variation of /-s⋅t/. Absolute and relative frequencies and statistical significance. Source: 2015 sample

Figure 7

Table 5. Acoustic correlates of coda fricative noise. Means, standard deviation and statistical significance (see Online Appendix 1 for values separated by gender)

Figure 8

Table 6. Acoustic correlates of onset fricative noise in /-s⋅t/ variants. Means, standard deviation and statistical significance (Online Appendix 2 for values separated by gender)

Figure 9

Table 7. List of stimuli for perception experiment 1: target stimuli, distractors and order of presentation to listeners

Figure 10

Table 8. Answers to perception experiment 1. Total n of responses per word = 54. The accurate answers are shadowed

Figure 11

Table 9. Acoustic correlates of affricate sounds. Means, standard deviation, and statistical significance. Source: 2015 sample (see Trend Study section)

Figure 12

Table 10. Groups of stimuli generated for the perception test

Figure 13

Table 11. Effect of noise duration and spectral values on the phoneme discrimination. Absolute and relative frequencies and statistical significance

Figure 14

Table 12. Summary of multinomial regression for /-st/ (left) and /t/ (right) responses versus /ʧ/ responses (reference level: /ʧ/ responses)

Supplementary material: File

Vida-Castro supplementary material

Appendix

Download Vida-Castro supplementary material(File)
File 149.3 KB