Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-v9fdk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-09T13:21:23.540Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Learning to talk Philadelphian: Acquisition of short a by preschool children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Julie Roberts
Affiliation:
University of Vermont
William Labov
Affiliation:
University of Pennsylvania

Abstract

Recent work in the acquisition of variation has shown that children begin to learn patterns of stable variation at a very early age. In fact, it appears that they acquire variable rules at about the same time as they are acquiring related categorical rules. Little is known, however, about the transmission from generation to generation of features undergoing sound change in progress. Therefore, this study examines the acquisition of the Philadelphia short a pattern by 18 3- and 4-year-old children. Even though this pattern of the raising and tensing of short a is a complex one, the children had, for the most part, acquired it. In almost all cases, the children matched the short a distribution both of their parents and a group of adult Philadelphians who were interviewed in the mid 1970s and described in Labov (1989b). These results indicate that even the youngest members of the speech community are actively participating in ongoing sound change.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1995

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bowerman, Melissa. (1982). Starting to talk worse: Clues to language acquisition from children's late errors. In Strauss, S. (ed.), U-shaped behavioral growth. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Ferguson, Charles A. (1975). ‘Short a’ in Philadelphia English. In Smith, M. Estelle (ed.), Studies in linguistics: In honor of George L. Trager. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Guy, Gregory, & Boyd, Sally. (1990). The development of a morphological class. Language Variation and Change 2:118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hindle, Donald. (1980). The social and situational conditioning of phonetic variation. Doctoral dissertation, University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Kuczaj, S. A. (1977). The acquisition of regular and irregular past tense forms. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 15:589600.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labov, William. (1980). The social origins of sound change. In Labov, William (ed.), Locating language in time and space. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Labov, William. (1989a). The child as linguistic historian. Language Variation and Change 1:8598.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labov, William. (1989b). Exact description of the speech community: Short a in Philadelphia. In Fasold, Ralph & Schiffrin, Deborah (eds.), Language change and variation. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Labov, William, Yaeger, M. & Steiner, R. (1972). A quantitative study of sound change in progress. Philadelphia: U.S. Regional Survey.Google Scholar
Marchman, Virginia. (1988). Rules and regularities in the acquisition of the English past tense. Center for Research in Language Newsletter, 2 04.Google Scholar
Marcus, Gary, Ullman, Michael, Pinker, Steven, Hollander, Michelle, Rosen, T. John & Xu, Fei. (1990). Overregularization. (Occasional paper #41.) Cambridge, MA: The Center for Cognitive Science, MIT.Google Scholar
Payne, Arvilla. (1980). Factors controlling the acquisition of the Philadelphia dialect by out-of-state children. In Labov, William (ed.), Locating language in time and space. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Roberts, Julie. (1994). Acquisition of variable rules: (-t, d) deletion and (ing) production in preschool children. Doctoral disseration, University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Trager, George L. (1930). The pronunciation of ‘short a’ in American Standard English. American Speech 5:396400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar