Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-lj6df Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T11:05:53.666Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A critical review of research relating to the learning, use and effects of additional and multiple languages in later life

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 July 2019

Simone E. Pfenninger*
Affiliation:
Department of English and American Studies, University of Salzburg, Austria
David Singleton
Affiliation:
Trinity College, University of Dublin, Ireland
*
*Corresponding author. Email: [email protected]

Abstract

While there is a growing body of research on second language acquisition (SLA) in children, adolescents, young and more mature adults, much remains to be explored about how adults in later life learn a new language and how good additional language learning is for them. Our goal in this article is to survey and evaluate what is known about the linguistic, socio-affective, neurobiological and cognitive underpinnings of the second language (L2) learning process in older individuals, the extent to which L2 acquisition may be seen as contributing to healthy and active ageing, and how these phenomena are to be approached scientifically, methodologically and pedagogically. Our view is that a developmental enterprise as complex as L2 learning in senior adulthood and its effects in later life cannot be explained by a single theory or set of principles. Furthermore, we take it that L2 acquisition in the third age needs to be regarded not just as a goal in itself but as a means of promoting social interaction and integration, and that it is partly through the stimulation of social well-being that its cognitive effects may potentially be observed.

Type
State-of-the-Art Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abu-Rabia, S., & Sanitsky, E. (2010). Advantages of bilinguals over monolinguals in learning a third language. Bilingual Research Journal, 33(2), 173199.Google Scholar
Abutalebi, J. (2008). Neural aspects of second language representation and language control. Acta Psychologica, 128(3), 466478. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.03.014Google Scholar
Abutalebi, J., Canini, M., Della Rosa, P. A., Sheung, L. P., Green, D. W., & Weekes, B. S. (2014). Bilingualism protects anterior temporal lobe integrity in aging. Neurobiology of Aging, 35(9), 21262133.Google Scholar
Abutalebi, J., Guidi, L., Borsa, V., Canini, M., Della Rosa, P. A., Parris, B. A., & Weekes, B. S. (2015). Bilingualism provides a neural reserve for aging populations. Neuropsychologia, 69, 201210.Google Scholar
Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality. AHRQ Publication No 07-0012. Rockville, MD: US Department of Health and Human Services; 2006. National healthcare disparities report.Google Scholar
Alladi, S., Bak, T. H., Duggirala, V., Surampudi, B., Shailaja, M., Shukla, A. K., … Kaul, S. (2013). Bilingualism delays age at onset of dementia, independent of education and immigration status. Neurology, 81, 19381944.Google Scholar
Amer, T., Campbell, K. L., & Hasher, L. (2016). Cognitive control as a double-edged sword. Trends in Cognitive Science, 20(12), 905915. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2016.10.002Google Scholar
Andersen, S. L., Thompson, A. T., Rutstein, M., Hostetter, J. C., & Teicher, M. H. (2000). Dopamine receptor pruning in prefrontal cortex during the periadolescent period in rats. Synapse, 37(2), 167169.Google Scholar
Andrew, P. (2012). The social construction of age: Adult foreign language learners. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Antón, E., Duñabeitia, J. A., Estévez, A., Hernández, J. A., Castillo, A., Fuentes, L. J., & Carreiras, M. (2014). Is there a bilingual advantage in the ANT task? Evidence from children. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(398), 112.Google Scholar
Antoniou, M., Gunasekera, G. M., & Wong, P. C. M. (2013). Foreign language training as cognitive therapy for age-related cognitive decline: A hypothesis for future research. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(10), 26892698.Google Scholar
Antoniou, M., Liang, E., Ettlinger, M., & Wong, P. C. M. (2015). The bilingual advantage in phonetic learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(4), 683695. doi:10.1017/S1366728914000777Google Scholar
Antoniou, M., & Wright, S. M. (2017). Uncovering the mechanisms responsible for why language learning may promote healthy cognitive aging. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 2217.Google Scholar
Arxer, S. L., del Puy Ciriza, M., & Shappeck, M. (2017). Aging in a second language: A case study of aging, immigration, and an English learner speech community. Cham: Springer.Google Scholar
Ashaie, S., & Obler, L. K. (2014). Effect of age, education, and bilingualism on confrontation naming in older illiterate and low-educated populations. Behavioural Neurology, 2014, Article ID 970520.Google Scholar
August, D., & Hakuta, K. (Eds.) (1997). Improving schooling for language-minority children: A research agenda. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.Google Scholar
Baars, J. (2010). Philosophy of aging, time, and finitude. In Cole, T. R., Ray, R., & Kastenbaum, R. (Eds.), A guide to humanistic studies in aging (pp. 105120). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Baayen, R. H. (2001). Word frequency distributions. Amsterdam: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Baayen, R. H., Tomaschek, F., Gahl, S., & Ramscar, M. (2017). The Ecclesiastes principle in language change. In Hundt, M., Mollin, S., & Pfenninger, S. E. (Eds.), The changing English language: Psycholinguistic perspectives (pp. 2148). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bak, T. H., Long, M. R., Vega-Mendoza, M., & Sorace, A. (2016). Novelty, challenge, and practice: The impact of intensive language learning on attentional functions. PLoS One, 11, 111. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0153485Google Scholar
Bak, T. H., Nissan, J. J., Allerhand, M. M., & Deary, I. J. (2014). Does bilingualism influence cognitive aging? Annals of Neurology, 75(6), 959963.Google Scholar
Ballesteros, S., Kraft, E., Santana, S., & Tziraki, C. (2015). Maintaining older brain functionality: A targeted review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev., 55, 453477. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.06.008Google Scholar
Barnes, S. (2011). Third age: Golden years of adulthood. San Diego State University Interwork Institute, San Diego, CA. See http://calbooming.sdsu.edu/documents/TheThirdAge.pdfGoogle Scholar
Bartolotti, J., & Marian, V. (2012). Language learning and control in monolinguals and bilinguals. Cognitive Science, 36(6), 11291147.Google Scholar
Bartolotti, J., Marian, V., Schroeder, S. R., & Shook, A. (2011). Bilingualism and inhibitory control influence statistical learning of novel word forms. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 324.Google Scholar
Basak, C., Boot, W. R., Voss, M. W., & Kramer, A. F. (2008). Can training in a real-time strategy video game attenuate cognitive decline in older adults? Psychology and Aging, 23(4), 765777.Google Scholar
Beder, H. (1989). Purposes and philosophies of adult education. In Merriam, S. B. & Cunningham, P. M. (Eds.), Handbook of adult and continuing education (1st ed., pp. 3750). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass Publishers.Google Scholar
Bellander, M., Berggren, R., Mårtensson, J., Brehmer, Y., Wenger, E., Li, T.-Q., … Lövdén, M. (2016). Behavioral correlates of changes in hippocampal gray matter structure during acquisition of foreign vocabulary. NeuroImage, 131, 205213.Google Scholar
Benisowitsch, N. (in prep.). The dynamic nature of language learning later in life: A longitudinal intervention study of the multifactorial interaction of age, affect and the bilingual advantage.Google Scholar
Berggren, R., Nilsson, J., Brehmer, Y., Schmiedek, F., & Lövdén, M. (2018). No evidence that foreign language learning in old age improves cognitive function: A randomized controlled study. https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/8y4gaGoogle Scholar
Bernal Castañeda, S. (2017). Lifelong learning and limiting factors in second language acquisition for adult students in post-obligatory education. Cogent Psychology, 4(1). doi:10.1080/23311908.2017.1404699Google Scholar
Berthele, R., & Kaiser, I. (2014). Mehrsprachigkeit und Lebensalter: Einführende Bemerkungen zum Themenheft. Bulletin Suisse de linguistique appliquée, 99, 116.Google Scholar
Berthele, R., & Vanhove, J. (2014). Entre jeunes barbes et vieux de la vieille. Bulletin Suisse de Linguistique Appliquée, 99, 3150.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2007). Acquisition of literacy in bilingual children: A framework for research. Language Learning, 57(1), 4577. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9922.2007.00412.xGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Poarch, G. J. (2014). Language experience changes language and cognitive ability. Zeitschrift Für Erziehungswissenschaft, 17(3), 433446.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Poarch, G. J., Luo, L., & Craik, F. I. M. (2014). Effects of bilingualism and aging on executive function and working memory. Psychology and Aging, 29(3), 696705.Google Scholar
Biggs, S. (2005). Beyond appearances: Perspectives on identity in later life and some implications for method. Journal of Gerontology, 60B(3), 118128.Google Scholar
Birdsong, D. (2006). Age and second language acquisition and processing: A selective overview. Language Learning, 56(1), 949.Google Scholar
Blanco, N. J., Love, B. C., Ramscar, M., Otto, A. R., Smayda, K., & Maddox, W. T. (2016). Exploratory decision-making as a function of lifelong experience, not cognitive decline. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145, 284297.Google Scholar
Bolzman, C. (2013). Ageing immigrants and the question of return: New answers to an old dilemma? In Percival, J. (Ed.), Return migration in later (pp. 6788). Bristol: Policy Press.Google Scholar
Borella, E., Carretti, B., & De Beni, R. (2008). Working memory and inhibition across the adult lifespan. Acta Psychologica, 128(1), 3344. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2007.09.008Google Scholar
Brändle, M. (1986). Language teaching for the ‘young-old’. Babel, 21(1), 1721.Google Scholar
Brehmer, Y., Kalpouzos, G., Wenger, E., & Lövden, M. (2014). Plasticity of brain and cognition in older adults. Psychological Research, 78(6), 749755.Google Scholar
Brill-Schuetz, K. A., & Morgan-Short, K. (2014). The role of procedural memory in adult second language acquisition. In Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (COGSCI 2014). Retrieved from https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Kara_Morgan-Short/publication/264619667_The_Role_of_Procedural_Memory_in_Adult_Second_Language_Acquisition/links/53e928880cf28f342f3f757b.pdfGoogle Scholar
Bryant, E., & Veroff, J. (2012). Savoring. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Burke, D. M., & Shafto, M. A. (2008). Language and aging. The Handbook of Aging and Cognition, 3, 373443.Google Scholar
Butler, R. (1975). Why survive? Being old in America. San Francisco, CA: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Bylund, E., Abrahamsson, N., Hyltenstamb, K., & Norrman, G. (2019). Revisiting the bilingual lexical deficit: The impact of age of acquisition. Cognition, 182, 4549.Google Scholar
Bytheway, B. (1994). Ageism. Buckingham: Open University Press.Google Scholar
Carlson, M. C., Parisi, J. M., Xia, J., Xue, Q.-L., Rebok, G. W., Bandeen-Roche, K., & Fried, L. P. (2012). Lifestyle activities and memory: Variety may be the spice of life. The Women's Health and Aging Study II. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 18, 286294.Google Scholar
Carr, D. C., & Komp, K. S. (2011). Gerontology in the era of the third age: Implications and next steps. New York: Springer Publishing.Google Scholar
Cattan, M., White, M., Bond, J., & Learmouth, A. (2005). Preventing social isolation and loneliness among older people: A systematic review of health promotion interventions. Ageing and Society, 25, 4167.Google Scholar
Cenoz, J. (2013). The influence of bilingualism on third language acquisition: Focus on multilingualism. Language Teaching, 46, 7186.Google Scholar
Cenoz, J., & Valencia, J. F. (1994). Additive trilingualism: Evidence from the Basque Country. Applied Psycholinguistics, 15, 195207.Google Scholar
Chamot, A. U. (2017). Preparing teachers for language learning strategy instruction in diverse classrooms: A program for new teachers. In Oxford, R. L. & Amerstorfer, C. M. (Eds.), Language learning strategies and individual learner characteristics: Situating strategy use in diverse contexts. London: Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H., & Felser, C. (2006). How native-like is non-native language processing? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 564570.Google Scholar
Cohen, G., & Faulkner, D. (1986). Memory for proper names: Age differences in retrieval. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4, 187197.Google Scholar
Cole, T. R. (1992). The journey of life: A cultural history of aging in America. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Coupland, N., Coupland, J., & Giles, H. (1991). Language, society and the elderly: Discourse, identity and ageing. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Cox, J. G. (2013). Older adult learners and SLA: Age in a new light. In Sanz, C. & Lado, B. (Eds.), Individual differences, L2 development & language program administration: From theory to application (pp. 90107). Boston, MA: Cengage Learning.Google Scholar
Cox, J. G. (2017). Explicit instruction, bilingualism, and the older adult learner. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 39, 2958.Google Scholar
Cox, J. G. (2019). Multilingualism in older age: A research agenda from the cognitive perspective. Language Teaching, 52(3).Google Scholar
Cox, J. G., & Sanz, C. (2015). Deconstructing PI for the ages: Explicit instruction v. practice in young and older bilinguals. Language Teaching Research, 53(2), 225248.Google Scholar
Cullum, S., Huppert, F. A., McGee, M., Dening, T., Ahmed, A., Paykel, E. S., & Brayne, C. (2000). Decline across different domains of cognitive function in normal ageing: Results of a longitudinal population-based study using CAMCOG. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 15(9), 853862.Google Scholar
Czaja, S. J., Sharit, J., Ownby, R., Roth, D. L., & Nair, S. (2001). Examining age differences in performance of a complex information search and retrieval task. Psychology and Aging, 16(4), 564579.Google Scholar
Damoiseaux, J. S., Beckmann, C. F., Sanz Arigita, E. J., Barkhof, F., Scheltens, P., Stam, C. J., … Rombouts, S. A. R. B. (2008). Reduced resting-state brain activity in the “default network” in normal aging. Cerebral Cortex, 18(8), 18561864.Google Scholar
Deary, I. J., Johnson, W., & Starr, J. M. (2010). Are processing speed tasks biomarkers of cognitive aging? Psychology and Aging, 25, 219228.Google Scholar
De Angelis, G. (2007). Third or additional language acquisition. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
de Bot, K. (2017). The future of the bilingual advantage. In Pfenninger, S. E. & Navracsics, J. (Eds.), Future research directions for applied linguistics (pp. 1532). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
de Bot, K., Lowie, W., & Verspoor, M. (2007). A dynamic systems theory approach to second language acquisition. Bilingualism, 10(1), 721.Google Scholar
de Bot, K., & Makoni, S. (2005). Language and aging in multilingual contexts. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
de Bot, K., & Larsen-Freeman, D. (2011). Researching second language development from a dynamic systems theory perspective. In Verspoor, M., de Bot, K., & Lowie, W. (Eds.), A dynamic approach to second language development: Methods and techniques (pp. 523). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Degnen, C. (2007). Minding the gap: The construction of old age and oldness amongst peers. Journal of Aging Studies, 21, 6980.Google Scholar
DeKeyser, R. M., Alfi-Shabtay, I., & Ravid, D. (2010). Cross-linguistic evidence for the nature of age effects in second language acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31, 413438.Google Scholar
Del Maschio, N., Sulpizio, S., Gallo, F., Fedeli, D., Weekes, B. S., & Abutalebi, J. (2018). Neuroplasticity across the lifespan and aging effects in bilinguals and monolinguals. Brain and Cognition, 125, 118126.Google Scholar
Derenowski, M. (2018). Identifying the characteristics of foreign language teachers who work with senior learners. In Gabryś-Barker, D. (Ed.), Third age learners of foreign languages (pp. 145160). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Dillon, A. M. (2009). Metalinguistic awareness and evidence of cross-linguistic influence among bilingual learners in Irish primary schools. Language Awareness, 18(2), 182197.Google Scholar
Divita, D. (2014). Multilingualism and later life: A sociolinguistic perspective on age and aging. Journal of Aging Studies, 30, 94103.Google Scholar
Dowens, M. G., Vergara, M., Barber, H. A., & Carreiras, M. (2010). Morphosyntactic processing in late second-language learners. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(8), 18701887.Google Scholar
Duñabeitia, J. A., & Carreiras, M. (2015). The bilingual advantage: Acta est fabula? Cortex, 73, 371372. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2015.06.009Google Scholar
Dweck, C. S., & Molden, D. (2017). Mindsets: Their impact on competence motivation and acquisition. In Elliot, A. J., Dweck, C. S., & Yeager, D. S. (Eds.), Handbook of competence motivation: Theory and application (pp. 135154). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Eckert, P. (1998). Age as a sociolinguistic variable. In Coulmas, F. (Ed.), The handbook of sociolinguistics (pp. 151167). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishers.Google Scholar
Edlinger, A. (2016). LanguAGEING: Fremdsprachen Lernen im Alter. Hamburg: disserta.Google Scholar
Erikson, E. (1982). The life cycle completed: A review. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Featherstone, M., & Hepworth, M. (1983). The midlife style of ‘George and Lynne’. Theory, Culture, and Society, 1, 8592.Google Scholar
Featherstone, M., & Hepworth, M. (1989). Ageing and old age: Reflections on the post-modern life course. In Bytheway, B. (Ed.), Becoming and being old: Sociological approaches to later life (pp. 143157). London: Sage.Google Scholar
Featherstone, M., & Hepworth, M. (1991). The mask of ageing and the postmodern life course. In Featherstone, M., Hepworth, M., & Turner, B. (Eds.), The body, social processes and cultural theory (pp. 371389). London: Sage.Google Scholar
Featherstone, M., & Hepworth, M. (1995). Images of positive ageing. In Featherstone, M. & Wernick, A. (Eds.), Images of ageing (pp. 170196). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Fjell, A. M., Walhovd, K. B., Fennema-Notestine, C., McEvoy, L. K., Hagler, D. J., Holland, D. D., … Dale, A. M. (2009). One-year brain atrophy evident in healthy aging. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(48), 1522315231.Google Scholar
Gabrys-Barker, D. (Ed.) (2018). Third age learners of foreign languages. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
García-Pentón, L., García, Y. F., Costello, B., Duñabeitia, J. A., & Carreiras, M. (2016). ‘Hazy’ or ‘jumbled’? Putting together the pieces of the bilingual puzzle. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31(3), 353360.Google Scholar
Gardner, K. (2002). Age, narrative and migration: The life course and life histories of Bengali elders in London. London: Berg Publishers.Google Scholar
Giedd, J. N., Clasen, L. S., Lenroot, R., Greenstein, D., Wallace, G. L., Ordaz, S., … Chrousos, G. P. (2006). Puberty-related influences on brain development. Molecular and Cellular Endocrinology, 254–255, 154162. doi:10.1016/j.mce.2006.04.016Google Scholar
Gilleard, C., & Higgs, P. (2002). The third age: Class, cohort or generation? Aging and Society, 22(3), 369382.Google Scholar
Gilleard, C., & Higgs, P. (2009). The third age: Field, habitus, or identity. In Rees Jones, I., Higgs, P., & Ekerdt, D. (Eds.), Consumption & generational change: The rise of consumer lifestyles (pp. 2336). New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers.Google Scholar
Gilleard, C., & Higgs, P. (2010). Aging without agency: Theorizing the fourth age. Aging and Mental Health, 14(2), 121128. doi:10.1080/13607860903228762Google Scholar
Gilleard, C., & Higgs, P. (2013). Ageing, corporeality and embodiment. New York: Anthem Press.Google Scholar
Giroud, N., Hirsiger, S., Muri, R., Kegel, A., Dillier, N., & Meyer, M. (2018). Neuroanatomical and resting state EEG power correlates of central hearing loss in older adults. Brain Structure and Function, 223(1), 145163. doi:10.1007/s00429-017-1477-0Google Scholar
Giroud, N., Lemke, U., Reich, P., Matthes, K. L., & Meyer, M. (2016). Longitudinal auditory learning facilitates auditory cognition as revealed by microstate analysis. Biological Psychology, 123, 2526. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.11.007Google Scholar
Giroud, N., Lemke, U., Reich, P., & Meyer, M. (under review). The impact of hearing aids and age-related hearing loss on auditory plasticity across three months – an electrical neuroimaging study.Google Scholar
Gogtay, N., Giedd, J. N., Lusk, L., Hayashi, K. M., Greenstein, D., Vaituzis, A. C., … Thompson, P. M. (2004). Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101(21), 81748179.Google Scholar
Gollan, T. H., & Acenas, L. A. (2004). What is a TOT? Cognate and translation effects on tip-of-the-tongue states in Spanish-English and Tagalog-English bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 246269.Google Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., Cera, C. M., & Sandoval, T. C. (2008). More use almost always means a smaller frequency effect: Aging, bilingualism, and the weaker links hypothesis. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 787814.Google Scholar
Gómez Bedoya, M. (2008). El aprendizaje en la tercera edad. Una aproximación en la clase de ELE: Los aprendientes mayores japoneses en el Instituto Cervantes de Tokio (Memoria del Máster de Español como Lengua Extranjera). Universidad Antonio de Nebrija.Google Scholar
Goral, M. (2019). Language and older bilinguals. In De Houwer, A. & Ortega, L. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of bilingualism (pp. 267286). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Goral, M., Spiro, A., Albert, M., Obler, L., & Connor, L. (2007). Change in lexical retrieval skills in adulthood. Mental Lexicon, 2(2), 215240.Google Scholar
Goriot, C., Denessen, E., Bakker, J., & Droop, M. (2016). Benefits of being bilingual? The relationship between pupils’ perceptions of teachers’ appreciation of their home language and executive functioning. International Journal of Bilingualism, 20(6), 700713.Google Scholar
Granena, G., & Long, M. H. (2013). Age of onset, length of residence, language aptitude, and ultimate L2 attainment in three linguistic domains. Second Language Research, 29(3), 311343. doi:10.1177/0267658312461497Google Scholar
Green, D. (2018). The interactional challenge: L2 learning and use in the third age. In Gabrys-Barker, D. (Ed.), Third age learners of foreign languages (pp. 3147). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Grey, S., Sanz, C., Morgan-Short, K., & Ullman, M. T. (2017). Bilingual and monolingual adults learning an additional language: ERPs reveal differences in syntactic processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(5), 970994. doi:10.1017/S1366728917000426Google Scholar
Grossmann, J. A., Teichmann, B., & Meyer, P. (in press). Effects of second language learning on cognition in healthy older adults: Study protocol for a randomized controlled trial.Google Scholar
Grossmann, J. A., Teichmann, B., & Meyer, P. (in prep.). Effects of second language learning on cognition in healthy older adults: Study protocol for a randomized controlled trial.Google Scholar
Grotek, M. (2018). Student needs and expectations concerning foreign language teachers in universities of the third age. In Gabryś-Barker, D. (Ed.), Third age learners of foreign languages (pp. 127144). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Gullette, M. M. (2003). From life storytelling to age autobiography. Journal of Aging Studies, 17, 101111.Google Scholar
Hargreaves, I., Pexman, P., Zdrazilova, L., & Sargious, P. (2012). How a hobby can shape cognition: Visual word recognition in competitive Scrabble players. Memory & Cognition, 40(1), 17.Google Scholar
Hartshorne, J. K., & Germine, L. T. (2015). When does cognitive functioning peak? The asynchronous rise and fall of different cognitive abilities across the life span. Psychological Science, 26(4), 433443.Google Scholar
Haselsberger, A. (in prep.). (Neuro)cognitive dimensions of language learning in the third age: A longitudinal study with dense time serial measurements.Google Scholar
Hernandez, A. E. (2013). The bilingual brain. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hernandez, A. E., Claussenius-Kalman, H., Ronderos, J., Castilla-Earls, A., Sun, L., Weiss, S., & Young, D. (in press). Neuroemergentism: A framework for studying cognition and the brain.Google Scholar
Hernandez, A. E., & Li, P. (2007). Age of acquisition: Its neural and computational mechanisms. Psychological Bulletin, 133(4), 638650. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.133.4.638Google Scholar
Hernandez, A. E., Li, P., & MacWhinney, B. (2005). The emergence of competing modules in bilingualism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(5), 220225. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.003Google Scholar
Hildreth, K. L., & Church, S. (2015). Evaluation and management of the elderly patient presenting with cognitive complaints. Medical Clinics of North America, 99, 311335.Google Scholar
Hillage, J., Uden, T., Aldridge, F., & Eccles, J. (2000). Adult learning in England: A review. Brighton: Institute for Employment Studies.Google Scholar
Isopahkala-Bouret, U. (2015). Graduation at age 50+: Contested efforts to construct ‘third age’ identities and negotiate cultural age stereotypes. Journal of Aging Studies, 35, 19.Google Scholar
Jäncke, L., Mérillat, S., Liem, F., & Hänggi, J. (2015). Brain size, sex, and the aging brain. Human Brain Mapping, 36, 150169.Google Scholar
Jessner, U. (2008). Teaching third languages: Findings, trends, and challenges. Language Teaching, 41, 1556.Google Scholar
Johnson, K. J., & Mutchler, J. E. (2014). The emergence of a positive gerontology: From disengagement to social involvement. The Gerontologist, 54(1), 93100.Google Scholar
Johnstone, R. (2002). Addressing ‘the age factor’: Some implications for language policy. Strasbourg: Council of Europe.Google Scholar
Joiner, E. G. (1981). The older foreign language learner: A challenge for colleges and universities. Language in Education: Theory and Practice, 34, Center for Applied Linguistics, Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Kabat-Zinn, J. (2013). Full catastrophe living: Using the wisdom of your body and mind to face stress, pain, and illness. New York: Bantam Dell.Google Scholar
Kadyamusuma, M., Higby, E., & Obler, L. (2018). The neurolinguistics of multilingualism. In Singleton, D. & Aronin, L. (Eds.), Twelve lectures in multilingualism (pp. 269295). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Karl, U., & Torres, S. (2015). Ageing in contexts of migration. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Kaushanskaya, M. (2012). Cognitive mechanisms of word learning in bilingual and monolingual adults: The role of phonological memory. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15(3), 470489.Google Scholar
Kaushanskaya, M., & Marian, V. (2009). The bilingual advantage in novel word learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(4), 705710.Google Scholar
Kaushanskaya, M., & Rechtzigel, K. (2012). Concreteness effects in bilingual and monolingual word learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19(5), 935941.Google Scholar
Kavé, G., Knafo, A., & Gilboa, A. (2010). The rise and fall of word retrieval across the lifespan. Psychology and Aging, 25(3), 719724.Google Scholar
Keijzer, M., & de Bot, C. (2019). Unlearning and relearning of languages from childhood to late adulthood. In De Houwer, A. & Ortega, L. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of bilingualism (pp. 267286). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Keijzer, M. C. J., & Schmid, M. (2016). Individual differences in cognitive control advantages of elderly late Dutch-English bilinguals. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 6(1–2), 6485.Google Scholar
Kemp, C. (2007). Strategic processing in grammar learning: Do multilinguals use more strategies? International Journal of Multilingualism, 4, 241261.Google Scholar
Keshavarz, M. H., & Astaneh, H. (2004). The impact of bilinguality on the learning of English vocabulary as a foreign language. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 7, 297302.Google Scholar
Kinsella, C., & Singleton, D. (2014). Much more than age. Applied Linguistics, 35(4), 441462.Google Scholar
Kliesch, M. (in prep.). New languages and old brains: A dynamic approach to language learning in third age.Google Scholar
Kliesch, M., Giroud, N., Pfenninger, S. E., & Meyer, M. (2018). Research on second language acquisition in old adulthood: What we have and what we need. In Gabrys-Barker, D. (Ed.), Third age learners of foreign languages (pp. 4876). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Knowles, M., Holton, E., & Swanson, R. (2011). The adult learner: The definitive classic in adult education and human resource development (7th ed.). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Knowles, M. (1980). The modern practice of adult education: From pedagogy to andragogy. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Cambridge.Google Scholar
Krakenberger, E. (2014). “50 + L2”: Una formula utile dopo il pensionamento? Bulletin Suisse de Linguistique Appliquée, 99, 115130.Google Scholar
Kruschke, J. K. (1996). Base rates in category learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 22, 326.Google Scholar
Kruschke, J. K. (2005). Learning involves attention. In Houghton, G. (Ed.), Connectionist models in cognitive psychology (pp. 113140). Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Larsen-Freeman, D. (2017). Complexity Theory. In Ortega, L. & Han, Z. (Eds.), Complexity Theory and language development: In celebration of Diane Larsen-Freeman (pp. 1150). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Larsen-Freeman, D., & Cameron, L. (2008). Complex systems and AL. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Laslett, P. (1987). The emergence of the third age. Ageing & Society, 7(2), 133160.Google Scholar
Laslett, P. (1989). A fresh map of life: The emergence of the third age. London: Weidenfield and Nicolson.Google Scholar
Lawrence-Lightfoot, S. (2009). The third chapter: Passion, risk, and adventure in the 25 years after 50. New York: Sarah Crichton Books.Google Scholar
Lenet, A., Sanz, C., Lado, B., Howard, J. H., & Howard, D. V. (2011). Aging, pedagogical conditions, and differential success in SLA: An empirical study. In Sanz, C. & Leow, R. P. (Eds.), Implicit and explicit conditions, processes and knowledge in SLA and bilingualism (pp. 7384). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Levy, B. (2003). Mind matters: Cognitive and physical effects of aging self-stereotypes. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 58, 203211.Google Scholar
Levy, B., & Langer, E. (1994). Aging free from negative stereotypes: Successful memory in China and among the American deaf. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 989997.Google Scholar
Levy, B. R., Zonderman, A. B., Slade, M. D., & Ferrucci, L. (2009). Age stereotypes held earlier in life predict cardiovascular events in later life. Psychological Science, 20(3), 296298. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02298.xGoogle Scholar
Li, L., Abutalebi, J., Emmorey, K., Gong, G., Yan, X., Feng, X., … Ding, G. (2017). How bilingualism protects the brain from aging: Evidence from bimodal bilinguals. Human Brain Mapping, 38(8), 41094124.Google Scholar
Li, P., & Grant, A. (2015). Identifying the causal link: Two approaches toward understanding the relationship between bilingualism and cognitive control. Cortex, 73, 358360. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2015.07.013Google Scholar
Li, P., Legault, J., & Litcofsky, K. A. (2014). Neuroplasticity as a function of second language learning: Anatomical changes in the human brain. Cortex, 58, 301324.Google Scholar
Liang, J., & Luo, B. (2012). Toward a discourse shift in social gerontology: From successful aging to harmonious aging. Journal of Aging Studies, 26, 327334.Google Scholar
Lindenberger, U. (2014). Human cognitive aging. Science, 346(6209), 572578.Google Scholar
Linhart-Wegschaider, H. (2010). Foreign language learning and advanced age (Dissertation). University of Vienna.Google Scholar
Lorenzo, F., Moore, P., & Casal, S. (2011). On complexity in bilingual research: The causes, effects, and breadth of content and language integrated learning – a reply to Bruton (2011). Applied Linguistics, 32, 450455.Google Scholar
Lövdén, M., Wenger, E., Mårtensson, J., Lindenberger, U., & Bäckman, L. (2013). Structural brain plasticity in adult learning and development. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(9), 22962310. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.02.014Google Scholar
Lovelace, E. A., & Twohig, P. T. (1990). Healthy older adults’ perceptions of their memory functioning and use of mnemonics. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 28, 115118.Google Scholar
Lowie, W. (2017). Lost in space? Methodological considerations in Complex Dynamic Theory approaches to second language development research. In Ortega, L. & Han, Z. (Eds.), Complexity Theory and language development: In celebration of Diane Larsen-Freeman (pp. 123142). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Luk, G., Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Grady, C. L. (2011). Lifelong bilingualism maintains white matter integrity in older adults. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(46), 1680816813.Google Scholar
Luo, L., Craik, F. I. M., Moreno, S., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Bilingualism interacts with domain in a working memory task: Evidence from aging. Psychology and Aging, 28(1), 2834. doi:10.1037/a0030875Google Scholar
Mackey, A., & Sachs, R. (2012). Older learners in SLA research: A first look at working memory, feedback, and L2 development. Language Learning, 62(3), 704740.Google Scholar
Marcotte, K., & Ansaldo, A. I. (2014). Age-related behavioural and neuro-functional patterns of second language word learning: Different ways of being successful. Brain and Language, 135, 919.Google Scholar
Mårtensson, J., Eriksson, J., Bodammer, N. C., Lindgren, M., Johansson, M., Nyberg, L., & Lövdén, M. (2012). Growth of language-related brain areas after foreign language learning. NeuroImage, 63(1), 240244. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.043Google Scholar
Matsumoto, D. (2019). Exploring third-age foreign language learning from the well-being perspective: Work in progress. Studies in Self-Access Learning, 10(1), 111116.Google Scholar
McDonald, J. L. (2006). Beyond the critical period: Processing-based explanations for poor grammaticality judgment performance by late second language learners. Journal of Memory and Language, 55, 381401.Google Scholar
McDonald, J. L. (2008). Differences in the cognitive demands of word order, plural, and subject–verb agreement constructions. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(5), 980984.Google Scholar
McDonald, J. L., & Roussel, C. C. (2010). Past tense grammaticality judgment and production in non-native and stressed native English speakers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13, 429448.Google Scholar
Moen, P. (2001). A life-course approach to the third age. In Carr, D. C. & Komp, K. S. (Eds.), Gerontology in the era of the third age: Implications and next steps (pp. 1332). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Mortimer, J. T., & Moen, P. (2016). The changing social construction of age and the life course: Precarious identity and enactment of ‘early’ and ‘encore’ stages of adulthood. In Shanahan, M. J., Mortimer, J. T., & Johnson, M. K. (Eds.), Handbook of the life course (vol. 2, pp. 111129). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Moyer, A. (2004). Age, accent and experience in second language acquisition. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Muñoz, C. (2000). Bilingualism and trilingualism in school students in Catalonia. In Jessner, U. & Cenoz, J. (Eds.), English in Europe: The acquisition of a third language (pp. 157178). Buffalo, NY: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Muñoz, C., & Singleton, D. (2011). A critical review of age-related research on L2 ultimate attainment. Language Teaching, 44(1), 135.Google Scholar
Naveh-Benjamin, M., & Old, S. R. (2008). Aging and memory. In Byrne, J. H., Eichenbaum, H., Menzel, R., Roediger, H. L., & Sweatt, D. (Eds.), Learning and memory: A comprehensive reference (pp. 787808). Oxford: Elsevier.Google Scholar
NIH Report: Advisory Committee to the Director. (2013). Interim report: Brain research through advancing innovative neurotechnologies: NIH.Google Scholar
Norton, D. L. (1976). Personal destinies: A philosophy of ethical individualism. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Olsen, R. K., Pangelinan, M. M., Bogulski, C., Chakravarty, M. M., Luk, G., Grady, C. L., & Bialystok, E. (2015). The effect of lifelong bilingualism on regional grey and white matter volume. Brain Research, 1612, 128139.Google Scholar
Ortega, L. (2005). For what and for whom is our research? The ethical as transformative lens in instructed SLA. Modern Language Journal, 89, 427443.Google Scholar
Ortega, L., & Han, Z. (Eds.) (2017) Complexity Theory and language development: In celebration of Diane Larsen-Freeman (pp. 123142). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Oxford, R. (2017). A developmental perspective on third-age learning. In Gabrys-Barker, D. (Ed.), Third age learners of foreign languages (pp. 318). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O. (2015). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex, 69, 265278.Google Scholar
Park, D. C., Lodi-Smith, J., Drew, L., Haber, S., Hebrank, A., Bischof, G. N., & Aamodt, W. (2014). The impact of sustained engagement on cognitive function in older adults: The synapse project. Psychological Science, 25(1), 103112.Google Scholar
Park, D. C., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. (2009). The adaptive brain: Aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 173196.Google Scholar
Pawlak, M., Derenowski, M., & Mystkowska-Wiertelak, A. (2018). The use of indirect language learning strategies by third-age learners: Insights from a questionnaire study. In Gabrys-Barker, D. (Ed.), Third age learners of foreign languages (pp. 7791). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Peltzer-Karpf, A. (2003). The role of neural plasticity in foreign language learning. In Ahrens, R. (Ed.), Europäische Sprachenpolitik – European Language Policy (pp. 369393). Heidelberg: Universitätsverlag Winter.Google Scholar
Perani, D., Paulesu, E., Galles, N. S., Dupoux, E., Dehaene, S., Bettinardi, V., … Mehler, J. (1998). The bilingual brain: Proficiency and age of acquisition of the second language. Brain, 121(10), 18411852.Google Scholar
Perquin, M., Vaillant, M., Schuller, A.-M., Pastore, J., Dartigues, J.-F., Lair, M.-L., & Diederich, N. (2013). Lifelong exposure to multilingualism: New evidence to support cognitive reserve hypothesis. PLoS ONE, 8(4), 620630.Google Scholar
Pfenninger, S. E. (2011). Age effects on the acquisition of nominal and verbal inflections in an instructed setting. Studies in Second Language Learning and Teaching, 1(3), 401420.Google Scholar
Pfenninger, S. E., & Neuser, H. (under review). Inferential statistics in data analysis. In Rose, H. & McKinley, J. (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of research methods in applied linguistics. Routledge.Google Scholar
Pfenninger, S. E., & Polz, S. (2018). Foreign language learning in the third age: A pilot feasibility study on cognitive, socio-affective and linguistic drivers and benefits in relation to previous bilingualism of the learner. Journal of the European Second Language Association (JESLA), 2(1), 113.Google Scholar
Piechurska-Kuciel, E., & Szyszka, M. (2018). Compensatory strategies in senior foreign language students. In Gabrys-Barker, D. (Ed.), Third age learners of foreign languages (pp. 109128). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Pot, A. (in prep.). Aging in multilingual Netherlands: Effects on cognition, wellbeing and health.Google Scholar
Pot, A., Keijzer, M., & de Bot, K. (2018a). Enhancing language awareness in migrants’ third age to promote wellbeing. In Gabrys-Barker, D. (Ed.), Third age learners of foreign languages (pp. 176200). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Pot, A., Keijzer, M., & de Bot, K. (2018b). The language barrier in migrant aging. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism. Pre-print online: 08 Feb 2018. doi:10.1080/13670050.2018.1435627Google Scholar
Prat, C. S., Yamasaki, B. L., Kluender, R. A., & Stocco, A. (2016). Resting-state qEEG predicts rate of second language learning in adults. Brain and Language, 157–158, 4450.Google Scholar
Ramírez Gómez, D. (2015). Critical geragogy and foreign language learning: An exploratory application. Educational Gerontology, 42(2), 136143.Google Scholar
Ramírez Gómez, D. (2016). Language teaching and the older adult: The significance of experience. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Ramos, S., García, Y. F., Casaponsa, A., & Duñabeitia, J. A. (2017). Does learning a language in the elderly enhance switching ability? Journal of Neurolinguistics, 43, 3948.Google Scholar
Ramscar, M., Dye, M., & Klein, J. (2013). Children value informativity over logic in word learning. Psychological Science, 24(6), 10171023.Google Scholar
Ramscar, M., Dye, M., & McCauley, S. M. (2013). Error and expectation in language learning: The curious absence of mouses in adult speech. Language, 89, 760793.Google Scholar
Ramscar, M., Hendrix, P., Shaoul, C., Milin, P., & Baayen, H. (2014). The myth of cognitive decline: Non-linear dynamics of lifelong learning. Topics in Cognitive Science, 6, 542.Google Scholar
Ramscar, M., Sun, C. C., Hendri, P., & Baayen, H. (2017). The mismeasurement of mind: Life-span changes in paired-associate-learning scores reflect the ‘cost’ of learning, not cognitive decline. Psychological Science, 28(8), 11711179.Google Scholar
Ramscar, M., Yarlett, D., Dye, M., Denny, K., & Thorpe, K. (2010). The effects of feature-label-order and their implications for symbolic learning. Cognitive Science, 34, 909957.Google Scholar
Raz, N. (2005). Regional brain changes in aging healthy adults: General trends, individual differences and modifiers. Cerebral Cortex, 15(11), 16761689.Google Scholar
Raz, N., Lindenberger, U., Rodrigue, K. M., Kennedy, K. M., Head, D., Williamson, A., … Acker, J. D. (2005). Regional brain changes in aging healthy adults: General trends, individual differences and modifiers. Cerebral Cortex, 15(11), 16761689.Google Scholar
Raz, N., & Lindenberger, U. (2013). Life-span plasticity of the brain and cognition: From questions to evidence and back. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(9), 21952200.Google Scholar
Resnick, S. M., Pham, D. L., Kraut, M. A., Zonderman, A. B., & Davatzikos, C. (2003). Longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging studies of older adults: A shrinking brain. Journal of Neuroscience, 23(8), 32953301.Google Scholar
Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., & Park, D. C. (2010). Human neuroscience and the aging mind: A new look at old problems. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 65B(4), 405415. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbq035Google Scholar
Ripollés, P., Marco-Pallarés, J., Hielscher, U., Mestres-Missé, A., Tempelmann, C., Heinze, H.-J., … Noesselt, T. (2014). The role of reward in word learning and its implications for language acquisition. Current Biology, 24, 26062611.Google Scholar
Ristin-Kaufmann, N., & Gullberg, M. (2014). The effects of first exposure to an unknown language at different ages. Bulletin Suisse de Linguistique Appliquée, 99, 1729.Google Scholar
Rodriguez Freitas Silva, L. (2008). From old age to third age: The historical course of the identities linked to the process of ageing. História, Ciências, Saúde-Manguinhos, 15(1), 155168.Google Scholar
Rönnlund, M., Bäckman, L., & Nilsson, L. G. (2005). Stability, growth, and decline in adult life span development of declarative memory: Cross-sectional and longitudinal data from a population-based study. Psychology and Aging, 20(1), 318.Google Scholar
Roth, T. N., Hanebuth, D., & Probst, R. (2011). Prevalence of age-related hearing loss in Europe: A review. European Archives of Oto-Rhino-Laryngology: Official Journal of the European Federation of Oto-Rhino-Laryngological Societies (EUFOS): Affiliated with the German Society for Oto-Rhino-Laryngology – Head and Neck Surgery, 268(8), 11011107. doi:10.1007/s00405-011-1597-8Google Scholar
Roumani, J. (1978). Foreign language learning for older learners: Problems and approaches. Washington, DC: The ERIC Clearinghouse on Languages and Linguistics.Google Scholar
Rughiniș, C., & Humă, B. (2015). Who theorizes age? The ‘socio-demographic variables’ device and age–period–cohort analysis in the rhetoric of survey research. Journal of Aging Studies, 35, 144159.Google Scholar
Ryan, S., & Dörnyei, Z. (2013). The long-term evolution of language motivation and the L2 self. In Berndt, A. (Ed.), Fremdsprachen in der Perspektive lebenslangen Lernens (pp. 89100). Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Safont Jorda, M. P. (2003). Metapragmatic awareness and pragmatic production of third language learners of English: A focus on request acts realizations. International Journal of Bilingualism, 7(1), 4368.Google Scholar
Salat, D. H., Buckner, R. L., Snyder, A. Z., Greve, D. N., Desikan, R. S. R., Busa, E., … Fischl, B. (2004). Thinning of the cerebral cortex in aging. Cerebral Cortex (New York, NY: 1991), 14(7), 721730.Google Scholar
Salthouse, T. A. (2000). Aging and measures of processing speed. Biological Psychology, 54(1–3), 3554.Google Scholar
Salthouse, T. A. (2011). Consequences of age-related cognitive declines. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 5.15.26.Google Scholar
Samuel, S., Roehr-Brackin, K., Pak, H., & Kim, H. (2018). Cultural effects rather than a bilingual advantage in cognition: A review and an empirical study. Cognitive Science, 42(7), 23132341.Google Scholar
Sanz, C. (2000). Bilingual education enhances third language acquisition: Evidence from Catalonia. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 2344.Google Scholar
Schachter-Shalomi, Z., & Miller, R. S. (1995). From age-ing to sage-ing: A profound new vision of growing older. New York: Warner.Google Scholar
Schlegel, A. A., Rudelson, J. J., & Tse, P. U. (2012). White matter structure changes as adults learn a second language. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(8), 16641670.Google Scholar
Schleppegrell, M. (1987). The older language learner. Washington, DC: The ERIC Clearinghouse on Languages and Linguistics.Google Scholar
Scholz, J., Klein, M. C., Behrens, T. E. J., & Johansen-Berg, H. (2009). Training induces changes in white-matter architecture. Nature Neuroscience, 12(11), 13701371. doi:10.1038/nn.2412Google Scholar
Schulz, R. A., & Elliott, P. (2000). Learning Spanish as an older adult. Hispania, 83(1), 107119.Google Scholar
Schwab, J. F., Schuler, K. D., Stillman, C. M., Newport, E. L., Howard, J. H., & Howard, D. V. (2016). Aging and the statistical learning of grammatical form classes. Psychology and Aging, 31(5), 171192.Google Scholar
Schwartz, C. E., Dwyer, M. G., Benedict, R. H. B., Ayandeh, A., Ramanathan, M., Weinstock-Guttman, B., & Zivadinov, R. (2016). Reserve-building activities and MRI metrics: A comparison between multiple sclerosis patients and healthy controls. BMC Neurology, 16, 108.Google Scholar
Schwartz, C. E., Zhang, J., Michael, W., Eton, D. T., & Rapkin, B. D. (2018). Reserve-building activities attenuate treatment burden in chronic illness: The mediating role of appraisal and social support. Health Psychology Open. Pre-published on 14 May 2018 at doi:10.1177/2055102918773440Google Scholar
Sevinç, Y., & Backus, A. (2017). Anxiety, language use and linguistic competence in an immigrant context: A vicious circle? International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 3. doi:10.1080/13670050.2017.1306021Google Scholar
Shapira, N., Barak, A., & Gal, I. (2007). Promoting older adults’ well-being through Internet training and use. Aging & Mental Health, 11(5), 477484.Google Scholar
Singleton, D. (2018). Really late learners: Some research contexts and some practical hints. In Gabrys-Barker, D. (Ed.), Third age learners of foreign languages (pp. 1930). Bristol: Multilingual Matters. (CITED., p. 12, line 568).Google Scholar
Singleton, D., & Pfenninger, S. E. (2019). Exploring a poorly understood variable: An agenda for classroom research on the age factor. Language Teaching, 52(1), 111127.Google Scholar
Sigelman, C. K., & Rider, E. A. (2012). Life-span human development. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.Google Scholar
Smith, J. (2000). The fourth age: A period of psychological mortality? (paper). Berlin: Max Planck Institute for Human Development.Google Scholar
Soskey, L., Holcomb, P. J., & Midgley, K. J. (2016). Language effects in second-language learners: A longitudinal electrophysiological study of Spanish classroom learning. Brain Research, 1646, 4452.Google Scholar
Sousa, N. (2016). The dynamics of the stress neuromatrix. Molecular Psychiatry, 21, 302312.Google Scholar
Sowell, E. R., Peterson, B. S., Thompson, P. M., Welcome, S. E., Henkenius, A. L., & Toga, A. W. (2003). Mapping cortical change across the human life span. Nature Neuroscience, 6(3), 309315.Google Scholar
Stein, M., Federspiel, A., Koenig, T., Wirth, M., Strik, W., Wiest, R., … Dierks, T. (2012). Structural plasticity in the language system related to increased second language proficiency. Cortex, 48(4), 458465.Google Scholar
Stern, Y. (2002). What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8(3), 448460. doi:10.1017.S1355617702813248Google Scholar
Stern, Y. (2012). Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer's disease. The Lancet Neurology, 11(11), 10061012. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70191-6Google Scholar
Storsve, A. B., Fjell, A. M., Yendiki, A., & Walhovd, K. B. (2016). Longitudinal changes in white matter tract integrity across the adult lifespan and its relation to cortical thinning. PLoS ONE, 11(6), e0156770. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0156770Google Scholar
Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M. S., & Spreen, O. (2006). A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentary. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Swain, M., & Lapkin, S. (2011). Languaging as agent and constituent of cognitive change in an older adult: An example. Canadian Journal of Applied Linguistics/Revue Canadienne de Linguistique Appliquée, 14(1), 104117.Google Scholar
Teo, A. R., Choi, H., Andrea, S. B., Valenstein, M., Newsom, J. T., Dobscha, S. K., & Zivin, K. (2015). Does mode of contact with different types of social relationships predict depression in older adults? Evidence from a nationally representative survey. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 63(10), 20142022.Google Scholar
Tolle, E. (2004). The power of now: A guide to spiritual enlightenment. Novato, CA: New World Library.Google Scholar
Treas, J., & Mazumdar, S. (2002). Older people in America's immigrant families: Dilemmas of dependence, integration, and isolation. Journal of Aging Studies, 16(3), 243258.Google Scholar
United Nations, Department of Economic and Social Affairs, Population Division. (2015). World Population Ageing 2015. New York: United Nations. (ST/ESA/SER.A/390).Google Scholar
Valenzuela, M. J. (2008). Brain reserve and the prevention of dementia. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 21(3), 296302.Google Scholar
Van Der Meij, M., Cuetos, F., Carreiras, M., & Barber, H. A. (2011). Electrophysiological correlates of language switching in second language learners. Psychophysiology, 48(1), 4454.Google Scholar
van der Ploeg, M. (in prep.). Language learning never gets old: Implicit and explicit language learning in seniors.Google Scholar
Vega-Mendoza, M., Alladi, S., & Bak, T. H. (2019). Dementia and multilingualism. In Schwieter, W. & Paradis, M. (Eds.), The handbook of the neuroscience of multilingualism (pp. 608624). Malden, MA/Oxford, England: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Verhaeghen, P., & Cerella, J. (2008). Everything we know about aging and response times: A meta-analytic integration. In Hofer, S. M. & Alwin, D. F. (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive aging: Interdisciplinary perspectives (pp. 134150). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Wang, H.-X., Karp, A., Winblad, B., & Fratiglioni, L. (2002). Late-life engagement in social and leisure activities is associated with a decreased risk of dementia: A longitudinal study from the Kungsholmen project. Am. J. Epidemiol, 155(12), 10811087.Google Scholar
Wang, T., & Saffran, J. R. (2014). Statistical learning of a tonal language: The influence of bilingualism and previous linguistic experience. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 953.Google Scholar
Ware, C., Damnee, S., Djabelkhir, L., Cristancho, V., Wu, Y.-H., Benovici, J., … Rigaud, A.-S. (2017). Maintaining cognitive functioning in healthy seniors with a technology-based foreign language program: A pilot feasibility study. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 9, 42. doi:10.3389/fnagi.2017.00042Google Scholar
Warnes, A. M., Friedrich, K., Kellaher, L., & Torres, S. (2004). The diversity and welfare of older migrants in Europe. Ageing & Society, 24(3), 307326.Google Scholar
Warnes, P. A. M., & Williams, P. A. (2006). Older migrants in Europe: A new focus for migration studies. Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies, 32(8), 12571281.Google Scholar
Wartenburger, I., Heekeren, H. R., Abutalebi, J., Cappa, S. F., Villringer, A., & Perani, D. (2003). Early setting of grammatical processing in the bilingual brain. Neuron, 37(1), 159170.Google Scholar
Whalley, L. J., Deary, I. J., Appleton, C. L., & Starr, J. M. (2004). Cognitive reserve and the neurobiology of cognitive aging. Ageing Research Reviews, 3(4), 369382. doi:10.1016/j.arr.2004.05.001Google Scholar
Williams, M., & Penman, D. (2012). Mindfulness: An eight-week plan for finding peace in a frantic world. New York: Rodale.Google Scholar
Wimo, A., Jönsson, L., Bond, J., Prince, M., Winblad, B., & Alzheimer Disease International. (2013). The worldwide economic impact of dementia 2010. Alzheimer's & Dementia: The Journal of the Alzheimer's Association, 9(1), 111.e3.Google Scholar
Wingfield, A., & Grossman, M. (2006). Language and the aging brain: Patterns of neural compensation revealed by functional brain imaging. Journal of Neurophysiology, 96(6), 28302839.Google Scholar
Wong, P. C. M. J., Ou, C. W. Y., Pang, L., Zhang, C. S., Tse, L. C. W. Lam, , & Antoniou, M. (2019). Language training leads to global cognitive improvement in older adults: A preliminary study. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 114. https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_JSLHR-L-18-0321Google Scholar
Woumans, E., Santens, P., Sieben, A., Versijpt, J., Stevens, M., & Duyck, W. (2015). Bilingualism delays clinical manifestation of Alzheimer's disease. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(3), 568574.Google Scholar
Ybarra, O., Burnstein, E., Winkielman, P., Keller, M. C., Manis, M., Chan, E., & Rodriguez, J. (2008). Mental exercising through simple socializing: Social interaction promotes general cognitive functioning. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 248259.Google Scholar
Zare, M., & Mobarakeh, S. D. (2013). Effects of bilingualism on L3 vocabulary learning among Iranian EFL learners. GEMA Online Journal of Language Studies, 13(1), 127138.Google Scholar
Ziegler, G., Dahnke, R., Jäncke, L., Yotter, R. A., May, A., & Gaser, C. (2012). Brain structural trajectories over the adult lifespan. Human Brain Mapping, 33, 23772389.Google Scholar
Zielińska, A. (2015). The strategy use of a successful fourth-age learner of English (unpublished M.A. thesis). University of Silesia.Google Scholar