Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-t5tsf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-16T15:12:44.525Z Has data issue: true hasContentIssue false

When and how patients' self-claims are challenged in psychotherapy

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 September 2024

Anssi Peräkylä*
Affiliation:
University of Helsinki, Finland
Liisa Voutilainen
Affiliation:
University of Helsinki and University of Eastern Finland, Finland
Mariel Wuolio
Affiliation:
University of Helsinki, Finland
Arnulf Deppermann
Affiliation:
University of Helsinki, Finland and Leibniz-Institut für Deutsche Sprache, Mannheim, Germany
*
Address for correspondence: Anssi Peräkylä Faculty of Social Sciences PO Box 18 00014 University of Helsinki Finland [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The article describes the practices through which patients’ self-presentations are challenged in psychotherapy. Based on the analysis of thirty-eight instances from psychodynamic psychotherapy and psychoanalysis, analyzed with methods of conversation analysis, narrative analysis, and coding, this article reports on how therapists challenge patients’ self-conceptualizations in response to patients’ self-presentations. Challenges mostly follow patients’ descriptive, narrative, or evaluative accounts that include a strong claim about their self. Challenges to the self pertain to core issues of the therapeutic projects. They are mostly built in ways that show its sensitivity to probable rejection by the patient. Overwhelmingly, the challenge is accounted for by reference to shared knowledge built in the participants’ shared interactional history. Arguably, psychotherapy is a particular setting where the organization of face-work is modified, as occasional challenging of the co-interactant's self-presentation is part of the institutional task of the professional participant. Data are in Finnish and German. (Self, psychotherapy, Goffman, conversation analysis)*

Type
Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

While much of the interactional work that psychotherapists do is seemingly geared to maintain or re-establish affiliation (see e.g. Muntigl, Knight, Watkins, Horvath, & Angus Reference Muntigl, Knight, Watkins, Horvath; and Angus2013; Muntigl & Horvath Reference Muntigl and Horvath2014), it is also evident that disaffiliative actions, such as challenging (Voutilainen, Henttonen, Kahri, Ravaja, Sams, & Peräkylä Reference Voutilainen, Henttonen, Kahri, Ravaja, Sams and Peräkylä2018), are an equally important part of therapy. In this article, we focus on a particular kind of challenge: one where the therapist questions the description of ‘self’ that the patient has put forward in their talk. Extract (1) below is an example of such a challenge. In lines 1–9, the patient tells the therapist that she feels guilty for bringing her dreams to the therapy sessions only rarely. Rather than affiliating with the patient, the therapist challenges the patient's self-description.

  1. (1) Fragment of extract (3) below; line numbers not corresponding (PA: patient, TH: therapist)

In lines 10–13 the therapist interprets what the patient has just said: the patient's talk shows that she lives too much for others, forgetting herself. Thereby, the therapist treats as problematic the self that the patient's prior talk implied. In line 15, the patient responds to the challenge, with pro-forma agreement that is clearly marked as being less than wholehearted.

In what follows, we ask when and how the therapists issue such challenges. Before turning to our data, we present our theoretical perspective to self in interaction and in psychotherapy.

Self in interaction research

Self has been a cardinal theme of social psychology. Alongside James (Reference James1891), classical authors such as Cooley (Reference Cooley1922) and Mead (Reference Mead1934) emphasized the social determination of the self: what the person considers themself arises from their interactions with others and calls for recognition and validation from others. More recently, social psychologists and philosophers have also clarified the concept of self: self is not part of the person, but rather ‘the whole person considered from a particular point of view’ (Zahavi Reference Zahavi2014:3). In sociology, Goffman considered the self as a key node in social organization. He pointed out that the maintenance of positive images of the selves of participants—their ‘faces’—is an omnipresent concern in interaction (Goffman Reference Goffman1955). Yet, despite the skillful practices of self-presentation (Goffman Reference Goffman1959), self is never secured (see also Rawls Reference Rawls1987). Social interaction brings about an inevitable vulnerability of the self, due to inconsistencies in self-presentation, discrediting information, and physical or psychological intrusions by others.

The reception of Goffman's conceptualization of the self has been ambivalent. Brown & Levinson's (Reference Brown and Levinson1987) influential work on politeness took up Goffman's theory of face, suggesting linguistic strategies of face-work are associated with various social actions. The reception in conversation analysis (CA) has been more critical. Schegloff (Reference Schegloff, Drew and Wootton1988) argues that Goffman's insistence on concerns about face as the driver of all interaction ‘psychologizes’ the study of interaction in a harmful way. In a text that focuses on the notion of identity, Schegloff (Reference Schegloff, Boden and Zimmerman1991) proposes that for an identity category to be relevant in interaction, it needs to be locally made relevant and consequential through the participants’ actions.

As theoretical notions, ‘self’ and ‘identity’ are close but not identical. In line with Goffman, we consider ‘self’ as something that is reflected upon and experienced, often emotionally, while we see ‘identity’ more as a social category defining a person. Generally, ‘face’ and ‘self’ only rarely figure in conversation analytic studies (see however Heritage & Raymond Reference Heritage and Raymond2005). Conversation analysts tend to reject the analysis of self, because it tends to explain interactional events with assumed inner processes. Instead, conversation analysts call for an analysis of observable practices constituting identities: exploration of the ways in which status, rights, duties, and properties of persons are manifestly treated in social interaction (Antaki & Widdicombe Reference Antaki and Widdicombe1998). This has been done mainly in studies of membership categorization, originating in Sacks's lectures (Reference Sacks1992).

Membership categorization is explicitly performed by assigning social category labels to persons, thereby treating them as members of a certain social group. Schegloff (Reference Schegloff1997), Stokoe (Reference Stokoe2012) and others insist on a rigorous sequential approach which shows that participants actually use categories in a certain interactional moment to index some property, action (disposition), or evaluation of their incumbents (or the category as such) and vice versa.

While membership categorization captures many facets in which identities become relevant in everyday talk, a more complex approach is needed especially for narratives in which the biographical dimension plays a role and in which several temporal layers of the past, present, and sometimes also future self are related to each other (Deppermann Reference Deppermann2013, Reference Deppermann, Fina and Georgakopoulou2015). This point is particularly important for the analysis of therapeutic discourse, in which narratives about the patient's past experience play a primary role. The positioning approach developed by Bamberg (Reference Bamberg1997; Bamberg & Georgakopoulou Reference Bamberg and Georgakopoulou2008) tries to reconcile conversation analytic insights concerning the displayed relevance of self and identity with insights from narrative analysis.

We focus on one interactional environment where issues of self are made manifest and treated in a consequential way: psychotherapy. Even though our data and analysis does not offer evidence for or against the idea of omnipresence of self, it touches upon ‘Goffmanian’ themes. We zoom in on moments where the patients explicitly or implicitly make claims about who they are, and the therapists call into question these claims. The claims involve emotionally relevant self-attributions. Yet our approach also attends to the ‘Schegloffian’ requirement regarding the observable relevancy of categorization. By challenging patients’ self-claims, therapists display their orientation to the patient having made such claims. In our data, participants put ‘identity’ in service of ‘self’: when presenting their (experiential and emotional) selves, participants also employ identity-categories. Yet, selves transcend these identity-categories by, for example, psychological, evaluative, attitude, and action -related descriptions. Finally, our work is informed by narrative analysis, as we examine the participants’ ways of relating the patients’ past and present selves.

Psychotherapy as social interaction

Psychotherapy stands out among medical and therapeutic practices, as it operates almost entirely through language and social interaction. Perhaps for this reason, it has become one key topic of social scientific and linguistic interaction research, and especially of CA. Typically, CA studies have taken up therapists’ actions, such as formulations, interpretations, and questions, seeking to specify their composition, sequential environments, and interpersonal functions. Furthermore, the studies have explored the interactional management of the therapeutic relation, for example, in terms of affiliation and resistance (for overviews of CA studies on therapeutic interaction, see Peräkylä Reference Peräkylä2019). In spite of the centrality of the experience of self in psychotherapeutic process (see the section below), the interactional management of self-experience and self-presentation have only rarely been addressed in CA studies. Focusing on therapists’ and clients’ choices of person reference forms in Finnish psychotherapies (active, passive, or zero person), Wahlström (Reference Wahlström2022) explored how the client's positioning of themselves as agentic or non-agentic was negotiated in therapeutic talk. In a similar vein, the recent study of Deppermann, Scheidt, & Stukenbrock (Reference Deppermann, Scheidt and Stukenbrock2020) on narrative and performative self in psychotherapy shows how the therapists, in receiving the patients’ stories, typically shift from responses through which they affiliate with the self conveyed through the content of the narrative, to comments inviting the patient to reflect upon the ‘performative’ self that is presented through the action of telling the story and through embodied affective displays. We continue this line of research by investigating systematically the ways in which the therapists challenge the patients’ self-presentations.

Self in clinical theories of psychotherapy

According to Mahoney (Reference Mahoney1991:235), ‘all psychotherapies are psychotherapies of the self’. If a therapy is successful, the patient's understanding of themself undergoes a process of change (Purkey & Stanley Reference Purkey, Stanley and Cain2002:484). The psychoanalyst Donald Winnicott described the process of psychotherapy as one in which ‘the patient will find his or her self, and will be able to exist and to feel real’ (Winnicott Reference Winnicott1971:5).

It is assumed in clinical theories that acceptance—be it called ‘positive regard’, ‘validation’, or ‘empathy’—is a key facilitator of the client's self-related processes (see e.g. Rogers Reference Rogers1961; Kohut Reference Kohut1971; Linehan Reference Linehan, Bohart and Greenberg1997). With an empathizing therapist, ‘clients feel safe to explore the nature of the self’. Thereby, it becomes possible for the clients to accept themselves, their ‘self-definition becomes more crystallized, and the discrepancy between self and self-ideal tends to be reduced’ (Purkey & Stanley Reference Purkey, Stanley and Cain2002:484).

While clinical discussions about self tend to revolve around empathy, it is acknowledged that for psychotherapy to be successful, challenge is needed alongside empathy (Bänninger-Huber & Widmer Reference Bänninger-Huber and Widmer1999; Voutilainen et al. Reference Voutilainen, Henttonen, Kahri, Ravaja, Sams and Peräkylä2018). Challenge involves that ‘the therapist, sometimes overtly but often discreetly, questions the client's beliefs about self, the world and his or her ways of being with others’ (Voutilainen et al. Reference Voutilainen, Henttonen, Kahri, Ravaja, Sams and Peräkylä2018:1). The ways in which the therapist might challenge clients’ self-experiences and beliefs about the self has not been much described in the clinical literature. Discussing narcissistic patients, Kohut (Reference Kohut1971:192) describes a process where the therapist gradually confronts the client's grandiose fantasies ‘with a realistic conception of the self’. How such confrontation happens interactionally is not known. In this article, we present an analysis of therapists’ ways of challenging patients’ self-presentation.

It is distinctive for psychotherapy that patients’ accounts and more generally their conduct in the therapeutic session are treated by the therapist not in terms of their validity, social evaluation, similarity to own experiences, and so on, as is usual in other social encounters, but as a window to how the patient conceives of their (emotional, cognitive, social, normative) self. In our article, we set as our research question the following: when, how, and with what consequences do the therapists challenge patients’ self-presentations in psychotherapy? We answer the ‘when’ question by examining the interactional environments of the challenges, the ‘how’ question by examining the design features of the challenging turns, and the ‘with what consequences’ question by examining the patients’ responses to the challenges. By answering these questions, our article is the first to describe interactionally the challenges of self-descriptions in psychotherapy.

Data and method

Three sets of data were used in this study. One set consists of German psychodynamic therapies with three therapist-patient dyads with twenty-five sessions each (recorded 2017–2018), another of Finnish psychodynamic therapy with five therapy-patient dyads with six sessions each (recorded in 2012–2013), and the third of Finnish psychoanalytic therapy (classical psychoanalysis) with three therapist-patient dyads with eighteen to twenty-one sessions each (recorded in 1999–2000). All patients were adults (seven female and four male). Two therapists were in their advanced training, the others had long work experience; four therapists were female and six were male. The patients’ problems involved mostly depression and anxiety. The German data collection was accepted by the Ethical Board of the University of Freiburg. As for the Finnish data, at the time of its collection, there was no relevant ethical board for this type of research. All participants in all datasets gave their written informed consent for the use of the data in scientific research and publications. The data are stored in data storage platforms and in hard disks protected by passwords. In transcripts, all identity-related information has been anonymised. In our study, we use conversation analysis (see e.g. Sidnell & Stivers Reference Sidnell and Stivers2013) as the primary methodology, amended by narrative analysis and coding. The transcripts of thirty-eight instances of challenges to the self in our collection were translated into English and analyzed sequentially and narratologically in joint data sessions by all authors. In our initial analysis, we outlined for each case the aspect of the self that is being challenged, the steps by which the challenge is brought about, the resources used to create the challenge, and the patient's response to it. In the second phase of the analysis, we analyzed the talk prior to the challenge, relevant aspects of the interactional history of the therapy that provide a backdrop for the challenge, the ways in which therapists attend to patients’ ‘face’ in challenging, and how the patient engages with the challenge. After systematizing our case analyses according to recurrent phenomena and analytic categories, we devised a coding scheme for checking the generality of what struck us as being recurrent observations. The coding scheme consisted of fourteen categories. The categories ‘strong self-related statement of the patient before the challenge’, ‘challenge refers to shared knowledge’, ‘challenge contributes to pursuing therapeutic core project’, ‘orientation to sensitivity in the design of the challenge’, and ‘patient's response to challenge’ proved to be particularly relevant for describing constitutive features of the challenges and their sequential context (see next section). Each case of our sample was coded first by one of the authors and then reviewed by the other authors. Disagreements were resolved in discussions involving all authors.

Analysis

Overall view of challenges

Relying on existing transcripts of parts of the corpus amounting to 123 hours of audio and video-recorded therapy sessions overall, we identified thirty-eight instances, where the patient's self is challenged by the therapist. Being convinced that a more systematic reading of our data would yield more instances, we consider thirty-eight cases enough as representation of key characteristics of the phenomenon. The challenges in our data occur in the context of a three-part sequence. It roughly consists of (i) the patient's claim about themselves (ii) the therapist's challenge of the patient's self-claim, and (iii) the patient's response to the challenge. Importantly, the self-claims, challenges, and responses are conveyed by variety of actions. In other words, these other actions, which we specify below, serve as ‘vehicles’ (see Schegloff Reference Schegloff2007:73–78) of self-claims, challenges, and responses.

The three-part sequence starts with patients’ turns that, in broad terms, involve a narration, a description of the self or the world, or an assessment of the self or the world. Often patients’ turns preceding the challenge consist of more than one action type, for example, a narration followed by an assessment of the self.

The challenges in our data are produced through various actions that include formulations (e.g. Antaki Reference Antaki, Peräkylä, Antaki, Vehviläinen and Leudar2008), interpretations (e.g. Peräkylä Reference Peräkylä2004), or evaluations (therapists disclosing their own point of view towards something in the patient or in their world; cf. Voutilainen, Peräkylä, & Ruusuvuori Reference Voutilainen, Peräkylä and Ruusuvuori2010a). In the challenges, the therapists address directly or indirectly the patient's self-presentation and convey that the patient is not what they claim to be, or that they are something else, or that what they claim to be is a problem. Challenges in our data are not implemented by wh-questions (which Koshik Reference Koshik2003 associates with challenges), while (when being formulations) they can adopt the shape of a request for confirmation (see also Küttner & Ehmer Reference Küttner and Ehmer2023).

Finally, patients’ responses to challenges can involve agreement or disagreement. The patients sometimes also avoid responding to the challenge by focusing their talk elsewhere. The three-part sequence of self-challenges is depicted in Figure 1 below.

Figure 1. Actions that convey self-claims, challenges, and responses to challenges. The vehicular actions (shown in boldface) convey the self-claims, challenges, and responses to the challenge.

We identified some key characteristics in the sequences of self-claims, challenges, and responses. The characteristics include (i) strength of the patient's self-presentation, (ii) the challenge's embeddedness in the therapeutic project, (iii) orientation to sensitivity in the design of the challenge, (iv) references to shared knowledge, and (v) lack of agreement in the patients’ responses. The case analyses presented below are organized around these characteristics. First, we present a quantitative overview of them.

Challenges typically occur after strong self-presentations (in 87% of the cases). By strong we mean that the patient displays strong epistemic certainty and a clear stance about the self that they are presenting. These are unmitigated, categorical statements by which the patient attributes to themselves certain personal characteristics by using a categorical predicate, usually a noun, an adjective, or a self-attribution of habitual ways of acting by a verbal phrase, by contrasting their own stance or behavior with others, or by telling a story designed to give evidence of certain personal characteristics.

Challenges draw on shared knowledge about the patient (in 79% of cases), which has been built in the earlier interactions between the participants.

Challenges concern issues that are at the heart of the therapeutic project (in 76% of the cases). By therapeutic project (see Peräkylä Reference Peräkylä2019), we mean the main topical thread (Schegloff Reference Schegloff2007) in the therapeutic talk that extends over several sessions. Such topical thread can be understood to reflect the aims that the participants (often especially the therapist) strive towards through their interactions.

Therapists attend to the sensitivity of the challenge, that is, the invalidation of the patient's self-presentation (in 71% of the cases). Sensitivity is indexed by displaying empathy and (partial) agreement with the patient's account before producing the challenge (Vehviläinen Reference Vehviläinen2003; Voutilainen, Peräkylä, & Ruusuvuori Reference Voutilainen, Peräkylä and Ruusuvuori2010b; Deppermann et al. Reference Deppermann, Scheidt and Stukenbrock2020), and by a dispreferred turn-design (Pomerantz Reference Pomerantz, Maxwell Atkinson and Heritage1984), for example, by hesitation, mitigation, self-repairs, cut-offs, and/or additional accounts for the challenge. Orientation to sensitivity is absent only in particular environments, for example, in challenges in response to self-deprecations, in challenges in the context of an ongoing argument, or in challenges repeating the patient's own words.

In response to the challenges, in about half of the cases, patients agree with the therapist; however, equally often (in 47% of the cases) the patient's response is other than agreement (disagreement, irony, or avoidance).

In our data, there are cases where the self-claims and self-challenges are readily observable in the design of the manifest actions; in others, self-claims and challenges are more implicit. We present four cases from our data, exploring the characteristics of self-claims and challenges. We start from a case in which the patient's self-presentation and the therapist's challenge are straightforward and explicit, observable in the design of the manifest actions. Afterwards, we move towards cases in which the self-claims and challenges are more implicit, and where the sequence ‘self-claim—challenge—response’ is also more complex and extended.

Case analyses

Extract (2) is from a psychodynamic focal therapy with a female patient in her twenties suffering from psychogenic seizures. In this case, the patient's self-claim is straightforward and the therapist's challenge is explicit and overtly part of the manifest actions that their turns accomplish.

Before the extract, the patient described the ways in which she experiences and handles her seizures in the social context, saying that she talks about them with her boyfriend, but hides them from her relatives. The patient then claims that her seizures are not part of her real self (lines 7–35). The therapist challenges her view by suggesting that the seizures belong to a part of her that usually does not surface (lines 38–47). The patient rejects this interpretation and reinforces the position she has already taken before (lines 49–73).

  1. (2) ‘Seizures’ (Therapy_C9, 29:05) (PA: patient; TH: therapist)

Strong self-presentation

In lines 1–35 the patient is engaged in narration of her experiences of the attacks. In and through this narration, she conveys what she considers herself to be. The patient produces a categorical, generalizing self-presentation, making a sharp distinction between her nature (wesen; lines 7, 34), that is, her real, core self, and the experiences associated with her seizures. She uses a list of dispositional predicates (strong, funny, cheerful, obliging; lines 9, 15–16) to characterize the former, contrasting them with the seizure experience, which she categorizes as the opposite (lines 18, 20) of how she feels ordinarily (line 22). The feelings of helplessness and insecurity (line 27), in contrast, are not attributed to herself as traits, but as state predicates (cf. e.g. Mischel Reference Mischel1968), which only apply in the context of the seizures (lines 24–30). Again, she mentions that she wants to avoid others perceiving her exhibiting these (temporary) properties (lines 24–25). Thus, the patient takes care that only those aspects of herself that she considers to be part of her true self and that are positively valued become socially visible, while the seizure-related, negatively valued impressions are excluded from her social self.

The therapist's challenge

The therapist challenges the patient's self-conceptualization by stating that in the seizures, an aspect of the patient's self becomes visible that usually does not belong to the patient's life (lines 39–43). This view explicitly contradicts the account emphasizing the positive aspects of her self that the patient gave in lines 9–16 and 34–35. In his turn-continuation (lines 44–45), the therapist (in an ungrammatical statement) rephrases the patient's dislike of this side of her self, which he then reformulates as ‘helplessness and dependency’ (line 47), echoing the words which the patient used herself in line 27. The therapist thus makes clear that his challenge concerns the issue that the seizure experiences, which the patient treats as unrelated to her self, are to be integrated into the patient's self-concept.

Shared knowledge and sensitivity

Before his challenge, the therapist seemingly affiliates with the patient by first highlighting the importance of the patient's account (line 38). The challenge is prefaced by a mitigation projecting a hypothetical statement (“perhaps y'know one could say”; line 39), which, by using the modal particle ja, at the same time indexes that the upcoming statement builds on shared knowledge. While the content of the therapist's turn squarely challenges the patient's self-conceptualization, its linguistic design displays sensitivity to the invalidation of the patient's self-presentation and the possible rejection of the challenge by the patient: He presents his challenge as an insight to be inferred from the patient's own account. It is thus built counterfactually on shared knowledge, as if it confirmed the patient's account, mitigating thus the oppositional quality of the challenge.

Core therapeutic project

The challenge in extract (2) pertains to key issues dealt with in this therapy. The seizures are a major problem for the patient, and much of the therapeutic work is focused on understanding them.

Patient's response

The patient rejects the therapist's interpretation. Prefacing her turn with ja: obwohl (line 49), she indexes disagreement (Betz Reference Betz, Blühdorn, Deppermann, Helmer and Spranz-Fogasy2017); she insists that the seizure experiences “do not belong” to her (lines 49–50), because they developed only after her high school diploma (lines 51–52). The patient thus draws on the notion of an eternal, essential self, which is fixed from the very beginning as personality. In addition, she states that the seizure experiences feel alien to herself (lines 57–59) like a second, disjunct personality (lines 62–64).

In what follows, we go through three further examples of sequences in which the therapist challenges the patient's self-description. The cases exhibit largely similar characteristics as extract (2). We continue with another case (extract 3) where the self-claim and self-challenges are readily observable in the design of the manifest actions, and the sequence ‘self-claim—challenge—response’ is compact and transparent. Before extract (3), the participants had a long discussion about dreams of the patient. She adds that she actually had two dreams, but that she cannot remember the other one. She guesses that “perhaps there will be them [i.e. dreams] again at some point in time” and laughs (data not shown). The therapist comments, in a smiley voice, that the patient has a somewhat pessimistic attitude (lines 1–2). The patient joins in the humor by laughing (line 3), but then shifts to a more serious talk, saying that she feels guilty because she so seldom brings dreams to the therapist, while others regularly do (lines 4–5, 11–13, 15).

  1. (3) ‘Dreams as gifts’ (KA1, 39:55)

Strong self-presentation

The patient's talk in lines 4–13 involves a self-deprecation: an explicit negative evaluation of the self through disclosure of guilt and unfavorable comparison to others. The self-deprecation conveys a pronounced, yet negative presentation of the self. The therapist receives the patient's self-deprecation with an increment (line 7) and formulation (line 10), which rephrase the ‘object’ that the patient does not bring to the therapist as ‘gift’ (cf. Weiste & Peräkylä Reference Weiste and Peräkylä2013). After the formulation, the patient elaborates her self-deprecation (lines 11–13, 15) by the unfavorable comparison to others.

The therapist's challenge

In lines 16–20, the therapist produces an interpretation (cf. Vehviläinen Reference Vehviläinen2003; Peräkylä Reference Peräkylä2004). It challenges the patient's prior self-attribution of being guilty for not bringing dreams (lines 4–5) as being an instantiation of her general tendency to live her life for others and not for herself.

Shared knowledge

By the clitic siinäkin (which could be translated ‘also there’) and taas ‘again’ (line 16), the therapist indicates that the pattern he asserts is similar to something that the participants already know. This allusion to repetitiveness indexes shared knowledge, but also adds a quality of reproach to the therapist's interpretation.

Sensitivity

Unlike in many other cases in our collection, the therapist's challenge does not overtly orient to the sensitivity of the invalidation of the patient's self-presentation. His initial formulations (lines 7, 10) before the actual challenge convey collaboration and understanding. They, however, also prepare the challenge, by depicting the patient's ‘altruistic’ attitude. The actual challenge is upgraded, framing it as an “extremely important thing” (lines 16–17). Possibly the fact that the challenge follows the patients self-deprecatory talk makes this directness interactionally possible.

Core therapeutic project

Through the nineteen sessions that we recorded from this dyad, the therapist works to draw the patient's attention to her tendency to deny her own needs. Thus, challenging the patient's relation to dream-telling is one variation of a key theme—non-adaptive altruism—that the participants work with through the material that we have at hand. The therapist seems to emphasize the importance of this theme in lines 16–17 by characterizing it as “the extremely important thing”.

Patient's response

The patient receives the interpretation by a pro-forma agreement which is clearly marked as being less than wholehearted (“That's (.) how I guess it must be seen th°en°”; line 22). After the patient's initial response, the therapist pursues further his challenge in lines 24–26 and 28. He reformulates again the patient's general attitude as ‘sacrificing’ (line 24), and once more points out that her relation to dreams not having dreams for herself, but for the analyst is an instantiation of it. In her response (lines 32–37), the patient defends her relation to dreams, by pointing out that she cannot interpret them herself without the therapist. She does not, however, address the more general self-related attitude—sacrificing herself and living her life for others—that the therapist attributed to her.

While in extracts (2) and (3) the self-claims and self-challenges were readily observable in the design of the manifest actions, in extract (4) below, the patient's self-presentation is met by a challenge that engenders a more complex negotiation regarding the patient's self. While we can find the core sequence (self-presentation—challenge—response) here as well, the negotiation involves several expansions of it. Like in the previous extract, the therapist's intervention in extract (4) amounts to an interpretation.

The participants talk about the consequences that the patient's partner's recent death has in the patient's life. Just prior to the extract, the patient expressed her worry that she might become ‘stuck’ in her grief and would not be able to return to normal living.

  1. (4) ‘To dance and to sing’ (KA 18, 4:29)

Strong self-presentation

The patient first depicts the prospect of her becoming as miserable as her ‘old relative’ (lines 1, 4–5), and then expresses her disapproval of such a way of being (lines 7, 9). By contrasting herself with the relative, the patient, somewhat indirectly, presents herself as willing to live fully and fighting against getting stuck in her grief.

The therapist's challenge

While the fighter self depicted by the patient implies a positive way forward in her life situation, the therapist puts it into a problematic light. The therapist designs a description of the patient's childhood experiences in such a way that the resistance to grief is depicted as an externally given obligation (note the modalities of obligation in lines 17, 18 and prohibition in line 21); yet he also points out that rejection of grief served the patient's self-regulation (“and you too would feel better”; line 27). The therapist closes his intervention by challenging the adequacy of the patient's attitude: much of the grief “goes completely ungrieved” (line 30).

Shared knowledge

In his challenge starting in line 11, the therapist links the patient's concerns to her ‘childhood situations’ that involved ‘great sorrows’ (lines 13–14). By the demonstrative articles ‘this’ and ‘these’, he presents her childhood experiences as known in common. He points out that in her childhood, the patient felt that she was not allowed to grieve (lines 17–18, 21), thereby drawing on a shared interactional history. The therapist relocates current attitudes with respect to past experiences (Weiste & Peräkylä Reference Weiste and Peräkylä2013): the problematic current attitude towards grief is seen as being rooted in her childhood.

Sensitivity

The therapist shows empathetic understanding for the patient. In lines 14, he refers to “these great sorrows” that the patient experienced in her childhood. In line 27, in elaborating further these childhood scenes, he displays understanding of the patient's motivation to get rid of the grief, as then “you too would feel better”. In these descriptions of the patient's adverse experiences, he treats them as factual, not mitigating or relativizing them.

Core therapeutic project

Through the nineteen consecutive sessions of therapy that we have recordings of, grief is recurrently addressed. The therapist suggests that the patient is avoiding grief, and in different ways encourages her to face the loss that she is undergoing. The challenge in extract (4) contributes to this therapeutic project.

Patient's response

Her response to the therapist's challenge is first affiliative and then turns ambivalent. After the therapist has made the first linkage between the patient's current situation and her ‘childhood sorrows’, the patient produces a minimal agreement (nii; line 15; cf. Sorjonen Reference Sorjonen2001). After the therapist's interpretation regarding the patient's felt childhood obligation to leave behind grief (lines 17–18, 21), the patient produces an elaboration (cf. Peräkylä Reference Peräkylä2005) in agreement with the therapist. The elaboration is delivered in three parts (“yes just (…) to dance and to sing (…) so that others would be happy and pleased with me”; lines 20, 22, 25). Yet after the therapist has explicated his view regarding the problematic implications of rejecting grief (lines 27, 29–30), the patient contradicts ironically the therapist's assertion, pointing out that thus far, there's “not any (…) great danger” (lines 31–33) that she would get rid of the grief. Eventually she depicts a scene where she will be putting bulbs of amaryllis into the soil (lines 37–38), which may be understood as implicit reference of processing death.

Even more than in extract (4), in extract (5) the self-claim and challenge are implicit, and the basic sequence ‘self-presentation—challenge—response’ is extended and embedded in other activities. The patient in this case is a man in his late twenties, who is making a career as a Lied singer. Through a narrative, he indirectly presents himself as an artistic ‘snob’ (line 3) that his family members criticize, but also admire.

  1. (5) ‘Snob’ (T1_06)

Before the extract, the patient told about his godfather and other family members, who live in a small town (while the patient lives in a big city). At the beginning of the extract, he tells about having heard a hearsay that the family members consider him as a ‘snob’ (lines 1–3). Prompted by the therapist's request for clarification (line 4), the patient elaborates the family members’ reasons for considering him in this way. After some further talk, the patients’ story takes a humoristic turn as he tells how his aunt was laughed at by her siblings, which made her stop taking singing lessons (lines 28–30). In lines 32–38 the participants collaboratively evaluate the little story. The patient points out that his mother denies having laughed at her sister's singing lessons, and then expresses a doubt: the mother would not remember (line 34). The therapist agrees (line 35), whereafter the patient in line 36 starts a more general statement, apparently working towards an assertion about the better memory of the person who has been teased. In overlap with the patient's assertion, the therapist repeats twice (with ‘no’ tokens; line 37) his agreement to the patient's prior assumption about the mother not remembering the events. But before the patient completes his more generalized assertion about memory (lines 36–38), the therapist cuts in by saying “the target remembers” (line 39), thereby actively displaying his agreement with what the patient apparently was going to assert.

Strong self-presentation

The patient presents himself as someone whom the small-town family members consider as a ‘snob’ (line 3). While the patient is telling about the way in which others categorize him, he also conveys a self-claim: he is someone who others see as a snob. The self-ascription is here done from the perspective of a third person (a cousin told what the family members think about the patient), which gives credibility to the report. The categorization is followed by laughter (line 3), which may indicate an affective stance towards being seen as a snob. While this self-attribution seems to be ambivalent (involving both criticism and admiration from the side of the family; yet in both cases showing how the patient stands out from the others), in lines 22–26 the patient continues by telling about a particular family member (his mother's sister), who more unequivocally appreciates his artistic pursuits, because she wanted to be a singer in her youth, too.

The therapist's challenge

In lines 39–42, the therapist challenges the patient's self-presentation. Latched to his general statement about the target remembering, the therapist points out: “you know this (…) also that maybe these school bullies of yours (…) you remember it better than them” (lines 39, 41–42). Thereby, the therapist shifts the focus of talk away from the patient's family members’ histories and their relation to arts. He draws an analogy to the patient's own painful history of having been bullied at school. In terms of construction of the patient's self, there is a shift from the successful artistic self of the present that the patient has portrayed, to his bullied self of the past. Yet, the therapist does the shift under the auspices of agreement with the patient's just prior talk. Thereby, the challenge of the patient's self-claim is not marked, but is rather embedded in a turn where the therapist agrees with and elaborates the patient's assessment of his own telling about the aunt and her sisters.

Sensitivity

It is noteworthy that the therapist designs his utterance that challenges the patient's self-presentation as a direct continuation to his display of agreement with the patient's prior assertions (see lines 35, 37, 39), thereby making this potentially disaffiliative move in a strongly affiliative environment. He also mitigates his statement epistemically with ‘maybe’ (line 41) and ‘possibly’ (line 42).

Shared knowledge

When referring to the “school bullies of yours” in the challenge, the therapist uses the demonstrative article ‘these’, showing an orientation to the salience and psychological presence of the characters he is referring to (line 41). The preface “you know this” (line 39) formulates the expectation of shared knowledge. He also invokes the school-time events by the simple pronoun ‘it’ (line 42), thus presupposing shared knowledge about them.

Core therapeutic project

The topic shift that the challenge involves appears to come ‘out of the blue’, through the therapist's association prompted by the patient's story. It becomes understandable in the light of the patient's psychological problems: the experiences of having been bullied were a key part of the problems that brought him to therapy. It appears that the therapist is making use of the response space after the patient's story to redirect his attention to these problems. He seems to treat the patient's self-ascription as ‘snob’ or artist as less relevant at this moment, redirecting the talk to therapeutically more significant matters.

The patient's response

In lines 43–44, the patient agrees with the therapist's turn about the school bullies even before its completion. The short juu ‘sure’, followed by ‘most likely’, claims independent knowledge (Heritage Reference Heritage2012), according to which what the therapist has suggested is clear or self-evident for the patient. Through these utterances, the patient also seemingly ratifies the shift in the construction of self (from the current artistic self to the past bullied self); yet he does not treat it as emotionally significant or a surprising perspective. As he continues his turn (lines 43–44, 46–47), the patient elaborates, in agreement with the therapist, his proposal about remembering, by suggesting a parallel between the art critiques of his first concerts and having been bullied at school: in both, he has been the target of potentially frightening attention. Yet, the shift to the memory of the critiques also accomplishes another shift in self-presentation: the patient returns to the identity of an artist, leaving behind the identity of the bullied schoolboy that the therapist promoted. Through his agreements (lines 45 and 48), the therapist not only accepts the parallel that the patient suggested, but also ratifies his shift back to the artistic identity.

Conclusion

In this article, we investigated moments in psychotherapy where the therapists problematize the patients’ presentations of themselves. In problematizing the patient's self-presentation, the therapists can convey that:

  • the patient's self-conceptualization is too narrow or too rigid (‘You are (also) something else than you claim to be’),

  • the patient's self-conceptualization is inaccurate (‘You are not what you claim to be’),

  • the patient's self-conceptualization is an impediment to therapeutic change (‘What you claim to be is a problem’),

  • the patient's self-presentation is in need of being reflected upon, however without stating a problem or an alternative (‘Look at what you are claiming to be’).

As we have shown through our four examples, sometimes the patients’ self-claims and the therapist's challenges are explicit and conveyed in a compact sequence of actions, whereas in other cases they are more implicit or indirect, and embedded in a more complex or extended sequence of actions.

Challenging a person's self-presentation is a highly sensitive matter. The self-presentation ‘automatically exerts a moral demand upon the others’ to treat the person as the kind of person they presented themselves as (Goffman Reference Goffman1959:13; Heritage Reference Heritage, Stivers, Mondada and Steensig2011:160). According to Goffman, questioning a self-presentation is always possible in interaction but it is usually avoided. Psychotherapy, however, is one of the few interactional settings in which challenges to the (patient's) self are licensed and occur regularly. Challenges can elicit the patients’ self-exploration and make them reflect on themselves. Therefore, challenges to the self are an integral part of the interactional work through which psychotherapy takes place and which is informed by the goal that ‘the patient will find his or her own self, and will be able to exist and to feel real’ (Winnicott Reference Winnicott1971:5).

Arguably, for Goffman, the self is ‘entirely’ in the presentation: there is no other, more authentic ego hiding behind the presentation and its social recognition. This conceptualization is different from psychotherapeutic theories of the self. Winnicott's (Reference Winnicott1965) distinction between false and true self is a case in point. The true self involves experience that is closely connected to physical, motion-related, and erotic aspects of the individual. The true self needs to be protected by the false self that is anchored in social conventions. Yet, the false self can entirely hide the true self, so that the individual, as well as the others, understands the false self as the whole person. In psychotherapy, the true self can be rediscovered.

Our theory and methods do not allow assessing any of the proposed versions of the self as indexes of a false or a true self. We investigated the technique of challenging, without committing ourselves to the metatheory of true self, which might be motivating the challenges.

The working alliance of psychotherapy (Greenson Reference Greenson1967:203–15) requires that therapist and patient disclose and discuss the patient's psychological processes and motives sincerely, even if this means disregarding considerations and taboos of social etiquette. However, even in this ‘amoral’ context, challenging the other's self-presentation still is a sensitive action. This is reflected by the design of the therapists’ challenges: Therapists mostly mitigate their challenging statements (Stukenbrock, Deppermann, & Scheidt Reference Stukenbrock, Deppermann and Scheidt2021), and they almost always take care to root the challenge in shared knowledge arising from the shared interactional history (Deppermann & Pekarek Doehler Reference Deppermann and Pekarek Doehler2021), thus making it accountable. In particular, they refer back to the shared interactional history of the therapeutic process. Admittedly, epistemic sources of the challenge might lie as well in the psychotherapist's clinical theory. Yet, in our data, the therapists never account for the challenges by directly appealing to theoretical assumptions. Instead, they always draw on the patient's prior talk and/or shared biographical knowledge.

Challenges to the self in most cases respond to a strong self-presentation of the patient. The self-presentation often involves a more or less pronounced assessment of the self, which can be either positive or self-deprecatory. These are kinds of actions that in other contexts would make affiliation relevant (however, in a complex way in the case of self-deprecation, see Pomerantz Reference Pomerantz and Schenkein1978). While therapists often first affiliate with the patient's account or at least show empathy before delivering an interpretation (Vehviläinen Reference Vehviläinen2003; Voutilainen et al. Reference Voutilainen, Peräkylä and Ruusuvuori2010b; Stukenbrock et al. Reference Stukenbrock, Deppermann and Scheidt2021), in this article we have analyzed sequences in which therapists do not produce the projected affiliative response, but rather a straightforward challenge. It seems that, in particular, strong self-related statements seem to provoke challenges, probably because such self-presentations are seen by therapists as symptoms of conflicts or as inherently problematic by virtue of their tendency to reify an unchangeable self.

Rather than simply stating a competing aspect of the patient's self, the therapists’ challenges are tailored to the patient's prior accounts and lines of interpretation. The therapeutic challenges treat patient's accounts, which often refer to single actions, events, or situated thoughts and feelings, in terms of enduring, general facets of the patient's conceptions of their self (i.e. personal tendencies to act, motives, habits, conflicts, etc.)—and in response make facets of the patient's self a topic of discussion and exploration. Challenges to the self in this way are a primary instance of the general tendency of therapeutic action to constantly put the spotlight on the patient's habitual feelings, thoughts, relational patterns—and self-conceptualizations, taking situated report and events as a point of departure. Although therapists’ challenges often respond to pronounced self-presentation, they sometimes also follow patients’ descriptions and narratives, which to an outsider (and often to the patient as well) do not seem to have a definite self-presentational value, but which are interpreted by the therapist as indexing more general self-conceptualizations of the patient in need of being problematized.

In our data, patients’ responses to therapists’ challenges are very diverse. They include, for example, agreement, taking up some of the challenge while ignoring other (often more fundamental) aspects of it, pro-forma agreement and resuming the patient's prior storyline, irony, justification of their self-related claims, and straightforward rejection of the challenge. Therefore, when considering only immediate responses, the therapeutic effectiveness of challenges seems dubious. We can understand patients’ tendency to disaffiliate with the challenges in different yet overlapping ways. On one hand, challenges can be at odds with deeply entrenched ways that patients conceive of themselves. Thus, their response can indicate that they are not (yet) ready to adopt the new perspective to themselves. However, the resistance can also be more locally anchored. The challenge questions the just prior presentation of self. Yielding to the challenge would amount to an inconsistency of self-presentation—which is something that we generally avoid (Goffman Reference Goffman1959). Local inconsistency of self-presentation may be as problematic as the acceptance of new understandings of what one ‘really’ is.

Since challenges to the self address core issues of the therapy, we can expect that, in spite of the lack of immediate acceptance, they may nevertheless unfold some impact in later sequences and sessions, for example, by therapists pursuing them further, or by patients who reinvoke the challenge by their own initiative and use them for more in-depth self-exploration. Thus, a next step in the study of challenges to the self in psychotherapy would be analyzing them within a broader interactional histories’ perspective. In this way, we may understand better if and how they are taken up and become consequential for the further course of the therapeutic process.

Footnotes

*

This study has been supported by the Academy of Finland (grants 319113 and 320248, AP) and the University of Helsinki Humanities Program (AD). We thank Prof. Carl E. Scheidt (University of Freiburg) for access to the German data.

References

Antaki, Charles (2008). Formulations in psychotherapy. In Peräkylä, Anssi, Antaki, Charles, Vehviläinen, Sanna, & Leudar, Ivan (eds.), Conversation analysis and psychotherapy, 2642. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Antaki, Charles, & Widdicombe, Sue (eds.) (1998). Identities in talk. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.Google Scholar
Bamberg, Michael (1997). Positioning between structure and performance. Journal of Narrative and Life History 7(1–4):335–42.Google Scholar
Bamberg, Michael, & Georgakopoulou, Alexandra (2008). Small stories as a new perspective in narrative and identity analysis. Text & Talk 28(3):377–96.Google Scholar
Betz, Emma (2017). Diskursmarker aus konversationsanalytischer Sicht: Prosodisch integriertes ja am Beginn von responsiven Turns. In Blühdorn, Hardarik, Deppermann, Arnulf, Helmer, Henrike, & Spranz-Fogasy, Thomas (eds.), Diskursmarker im Deutschen: Reflexionen und Analysen 183–205. Mannheim: Verlag für Gesprächsforschung.Google Scholar
Brown, Penelope, & Levinson, Stephen C. (1987). Politeness: Some universals in language usage. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bänninger-Huber, Eva, & Widmer, Christine (1999). Affective relationship patterns and psychotherapeutic change. Psychotherapy Research 9:7487.Google Scholar
Cooley, Charles Horton (1922). Human mature and the social order. Revised edn. New York: Charles Scribner's Sons.Google Scholar
Deppermann, Arnulf (2013). How to get a grip on identitites-in-interaction: What does ‘positioning’ offer more than ‘membership categorization’? Evidence from a mock story. Narrative Inquiry 23(1):6288.Google Scholar
Deppermann, Arnulf (2015). Positioning. In Fina, Anna De & Georgakopoulou, Alexandra (eds.), Handbook of narrative analysis, 369–87. Malden, MA: Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Deppermann, Arnulf, & Pekarek Doehler, Simona (2021). Longitudinal conversation analysis: Introduction to the special issue. Research on Language and Social Interaction 54(2):127–41.Google Scholar
Deppermann, Arnulf; Scheidt, Carl; & Stukenbrock, Anja (2020). Positioning shifts from told self to performative self in psychotherapy. Frontiers in Psychology 11:572436. Online: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.572436.Google Scholar
Goffman, Erving (1955). On face-work: An analysis of ritual elements in social interaction. Psychiatry 18(3):213–31.Google Scholar
Goffman, Erving (1959). Presentation of self in everyday life. New York: Doubleday Anchor Books.Google Scholar
Greenson, Ralph R. (1967). The technique and practice of psychoanalysis, vol. 1. New York: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
Heritage, John (2011). Territories of knowledge, territories of experience: Empathic moments in interaction. In Stivers, Tanya, Mondada, Lorenza, & Steensig, Jacob (eds.), The morality of knowledge in conversation, 159–83. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Heritage, John (2012). Epistemics in action: Action formation and territories of knowledge. Research on Language and Social Interaction 45(1):129.Google Scholar
Heritage, John, & Raymond, Geoffrey (2005). The terms of agreement: Indexing epistemic authority and subordination in talk-in-interaction. Social Psychology Quarterly 68(1):1538.Google Scholar
James, William (1891). Principles of psychology, vol. 1. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Kohut, Heinz (1971). The analysis of the self. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Koshik, Irene (2003). Wh-questions used as challenges. Discourse Studies 5(1):5177.Google Scholar
Küttner, Uwe, & Ehmer, Oliver (2023). Affordances and actions: Requests for confirmation as devices for implementing challenging and other disagreement-implicative actions. Contrastive Pragmatics 1:145. Online: https://doi.org/10.1163/26660393-bja10089.Google Scholar
Linehan, Marsha (1997). Validation and psychotherapy. In Bohart, Arthur & Greenberg, Leslie (eds.), Empathy reconsidered: New directions in psychotherapy, 353–92. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Mahoney, Michael (1991). Human change processes. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Mead, George Herbert (1934). Mind, self and society. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Mischel, Walter (1968). Personality and assessment. New York: Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Muntigl, Peter, & Horvath, Adam O. (2014). The therapeutic relationship in action: How therapists and clients co-manage relational disaffiliation. Psychotherapy Research 24(3):327–45.Google Scholar
Muntigl, Peter; Knight, Naomi; Watkins, Ashley; Horvath;, Adam O. & Angus, Lynne (2013). Active retreating: Person-centered practices to repair disaffiliation in therapy. Journal of Pragmatics 53:120.Google Scholar
Peräkylä, Anssi (2004). Making links in psychoanalytic interpretations: A conversation analytic view. Psychotherapy Research 14(3):289307.Google Scholar
Peräkylä, Anssi (2005). Patients’ responses to interpretations: A dialogue between conversation analysis and psychoanalytic theory. Psychotherapy Research 12(3):289307.Google Scholar
Peräkylä, Anssi (2019). Conversation analysis and psychotherapy: Identifying transformative sequences. Research on Language and Social Interaction 52(3):257–80.Google Scholar
Pomerantz, Anita (1978). Compliment responses: Notes on the cooperation of multiple constraints. In Schenkein, Jim (ed.), Studies in the organization of conversational interaction, 79112. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Pomerantz, Anita (1984). Agreeing and disagreeing with assessments: Some features of preferred/dispreferred turn shaped. In Maxwell Atkinson, J. & Heritage, John (eds.), Structures of social action, 57101. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Purkey, William, & Stanley, Paula (2002). The self in psychotherapy. In Cain, David (ed.), Humanistic psychotherapies: Handbook of research and practice, 473–98. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Rawls, Anne W. (1987). The interaction order sui generis: Goffman's contribution to social theory. Sociological Theory 5(2):136–49.Google Scholar
Rogers, Carl (1961). The process equation of psychotherapy. American Journal of Psychotherapy 15(1):2745.Google Scholar
Sacks, Harvey (1992). Lectures on conversation, vol. 1. Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Schegloff, Emmanuel (1988). Goffman and the analysis of conversation. In Drew, Paul & Wootton, Anthony (eds.), Erving Goffman: Exploring the interaction order, 89135. Cambridge: Polity.Google Scholar
Schegloff, Emmanuel (1991). Reflections on talk and social structure. In Boden, Deirdre & Zimmerman, Don (eds.), Talk and social structure: Studies in ethnomethodology and conversation analysis, 4470. Cambridge: Polity.Google Scholar
Schegloff, Emmanuel (1997). Whose text? Whose context? Whose context. Discourse & Society 8(2):165–87.Google Scholar
Schegloff, Emmanuel (2007). Sequence organization in interaction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sidnell, Jack, & Stivers, Tanya (eds.) (2013). Handbook of conversation analysis. New York: Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Sorjonen, Marja-Leena (2001). Responding in conversation. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Stokoe, Elizabeth (2012). Moving forward with membership categorization analysis: Methods for systematic analysis. Discourse Studies 14(3):277303.Google Scholar
Stukenbrock, Anja; Deppermann, Arnulf; & Scheidt, Carl (2021). The art of tentativity: Delivering interpretations in psychodynamic psychotherapy. Journal of Pragmatics 176:7696.Google Scholar
Vehviläinen, Sanna (2003). Preparing and delivering interpretations in psychoanalytic interaction. Text & Talk 23(4):573606.Google Scholar
Voutilainen, Liisa; Henttonen, Pentti; Kahri, Mikko; Ravaja, Niklas; Sams, Mikko; & Peräkylä, Anssi (2018). Empathy, challenge, and psychophysiological activation in therapist–client interaction. Frontiers in Psychology 9, article530. Online: https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00530.Google Scholar
Voutilainen, Liisa; Peräkylä, Anssi; & Ruusuvuori, Johanna (2010a). Professional non-neutrality: Criticising the third party in psychotherapy. Sociology of Health & Illness 32(5):798816.Google Scholar
Voutilainen, Liisa; Peräkylä, Anssi; & Ruusuvuori, Johanna (2010b). Recognition and interpretation: Responding to emotional experience in psychotherapy. Research on Language and Social Interaction 43(1):85107.Google Scholar
Wahlström, Jarl (2022). Person references, change in footing, and agency positioning in psychotherapeutic conversations. Frontiers in Psychology 14:1206327. Online: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1206327.Google Scholar
Weiste, Elina, & Peräkylä, Anssi (2013). A comparative conversation analytic study of formulations in psychoanalysis and cognitive psychotherapy. Research on Language and Social Interaction 46(4):299321.Google Scholar
Winnicott, Donald (1965). The maturational processes and the facilitating environment: Studies in the theory of emotional development. New York: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
Winnicott, Donald (1971). Playing and reality. London: Tavistock.Google Scholar
Zahavi, Dan (2014). Self and other: Exploring subjectivity, empathy, and shame. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Actions that convey self-claims, challenges, and responses to challenges. The vehicular actions (shown in boldface) convey the self-claims, challenges, and responses to the challenge.