Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-18T11:22:24.744Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Some features of early child–adult dialogues1

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 December 2008

Susan Ervin-Tripp
Affiliation:
University of California, Berkeley

Extract

Adaptations to a partner perceived to be conversationally incompetent can be heard in speech to children, the hard-of-hearing, foreigners, speakers with certain speech defects, the aged, and hospital patients. These adaptations may be controlled partially by the actual performance of partners in conversations, through influences on perceived ability. The first half of this paper describes specific changes in children's conversational abilities in early childhood, which in turn may serve to alter how their partners judge their abilities to understand. The second half of the paper addresses the evidence regarding the level and types of changes in adult speech to children as the child's ability changes.

Type
Articles: Sequencing in Children's Discourse
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1978

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Andersen, E. S. & Johnson, C. E. (1973). Modifications in the speech of an eight-year-old as a reflection of age of listener. Stanford Occasional Papers in Linguistics 3, 149–60.Google Scholar
Berko, Gleason J. (1973). Code switching in children's language. In Moore, T. E. (ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. (1975) One word at a time. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Bloom, L.Lightbown, P. & Hood, L.Structure and variation in child language. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, No. 160, Vol. 40, No. 2.Google Scholar
Bloom, L.Rocissano, L. & Hood, L. (1977). Adult–child discourse: Developmental interaction between information processing and linguistic knowledge. Cognitive Psychology 9. 521–52.Google Scholar
Blount, B. (1972). Parental speech and language acquisition: Some Luo and Samoan examples. Anthropological Linguistics 14. 119–30.Google Scholar
Bohannon, J. N. & Marquis, A. L. (1977). Children's control of adult speech. Child Development 48. 1002–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R. (1973) Afirst language: The early stages. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cathcart, R. (1977). Answering ‘yes-no’ questions: Do as we do, not as we teach. Unpublished paper for Psychology 298. University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Corsaro, W. (1977). The clarification request as a feature of adult interactive styles with young children. LinS 6, 183208.Google Scholar
Drach, K., Kobashigawa, B., Pfuderer, C. & Slobin, D. (1969). The structure of linguistic input to children. Working paper No. 14, Berkeley, Calif.: Language-Behavior Research Laboratory.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. M. (1970). Discourse agreement: How children answer questions. In Hayes, R. (ed.), Cognition and language learning. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. M. (1976) Is Sybil there? The structure of some American-English directives. LinS 5. 2566.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. M. (1977). Wait for me, roller-skate. In Mitchell-Kernan, C. & Ervin-Tripp, S. (eds.), Child discourse. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. M. & Miller, W. (1977). Early discourse: Some questions about questions. In Lewis, M. & Rosenblum, L. (eds.), The origins of behavior: Interaction, conversation, and the development of language. New York: Wiley. 925.Google Scholar
Ferguson, C. A. (1964). Baby talk in six languages. American Anthropologist 66, 210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fillmore, L. W. (1976). The second time around: Cognitive and social strategies in second language acquisition. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Gelman, R. & Shatz, M. (1977). Appropriate speech adjustments: The operation of conversational constraints on talk to two-year-olds. In Lewis, M. & Rosenblum, L. (eds.), The origins of behavior: Interaction, conversation, and the development of language. New York: Wiley, 2762.Google Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K. & Hasan, R. (1976). Cohesion in English. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Harkness, S. (1977). Aspects of social environment and first language acquisition in rural Africa. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds.), Maternal input and language acquisition New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hatch, E., Shapira, R. & Gough, J. (in press). ‘Foreigner-talk’ discourse. ITL: Review of Applied Linguistics.Google Scholar
Holzman, M. (1972). The use of interrogative forms in the verbal interaction of three mothers and their children. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research I. 311–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howes, D. (1957). On the relation between the probability of a word as an association and in general linguistic usage. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 54. 7585.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieven, E. V. M. (1977a). Conversations between mothers and young children: Individual differences and their possible implication for the study of language learning. In Waterson, N. & Snow, C. (eds.), The development of communication: Social and pragmatic factors in language acquisition. London: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Lieven, E. V. M. (1977b). Turn-taking and pragmatics: Two issues in early child language. In Campbell, R. & Smith, P. T. (eds.), The Stirling Psychology of Language Conference. New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Newport, E. (1976) Motherese: The speech of mothers to young children. In Castellan, N. J., Pisoni, D. B. & Potts, G. R. (eds.), Cognitive theory, Vol. II. Hilisdale, N.J.: Lawrence Earlbaum.Google Scholar
Sachs, J. & Devin, J. (1976). Young children's use of age-appropriate speech styles in social interaction and role-playing. Journal of Child Language 3, 8198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schachter, F. F., Fosha, D., Stemp, S., Brotman, H. & Ganger, S. (1976). Everyday caretaker talk to toddlers vs. threes and fours. Journal of Child Language 3. 225–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shatz, M. (1976). The comprehension of indirect directives: Can two-year-olds shut the door? Paper at Linguistic Society of America, Amherst, Mass.Google Scholar
Shatz, M. & Gelman, R. (1973). The development of communication skills: Modifications in the speech of young children as a function of listener. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. No. 152, vol. 38, No. 5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snow, C. E. (1977). The development of conversation between mothers and babies. Journal of Child Language 4. 122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, C. E., Arlman-Rupp, A., Hassing, Y., Jobse, J., Joosten, J., & Voster, J. (1977). Mothers' speech in three social classes. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds.), Maternal input and language acquisition. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Van, der Geest T., Snow, C. E. & Drews-Drubbel, A. (in press). Developmental aspects of mother-child conversation.Google Scholar