We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
Online ordering will be unavailable from 17:00 GMT on Friday, April 25 until 17:00 GMT on Sunday, April 27 due to maintenance. We apologise for the inconvenience.
Hostname: page-component-669899f699-7tmb6
Total loading time: 0
Render date: 2025-04-26T02:03:12.813Z
Has data issue: false
hasContentIssue false
An abstract is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)
Article purchase
Temporarily unavailable
References
REFERENCES
Blount, B. G. (1977). Ethnography and caretaker-child interaction. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cross, T. G. (1977). Mothers' speech adjustments: The contribution of selected child listener variables. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hood, L., & Schieffelin, B. (1978). Elicited imitation in two cultural contexts. Quarterly Newsletter of the Institute for Comparative Human Development, 2(1). 4–12.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, (1–2, Serial No. 149) 38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. (1981). Individual differences in language development: Implications for development and language. Developmental Psychology17: 170–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar