Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-p9bg8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T12:10:26.244Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Masked orthographic neighbor priming effects in Chinese two-character words

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 May 2023

Huilan Yang*
Affiliation:
Department of Foreign Languages, Zhejiang Gongshang University, Hangzhou, Zhejiang, China
Giacomo Spinelli
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Milano-Bicocca, Milan, MI, Italy
Lingling Li
Affiliation:
Department of Applied Foreign Languages, Zhejiang International Studies University, Hangzhou, Zhejiang, China
Stephen J. Lupker
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Western Ontario, London, ON, Canada
*
Corresponding authors: Huilan Yang and Stephen J. Lupker; Emails: [email protected]; [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

In masked priming lexical decision tasks in alphabetic or syllabic script languages, latencies are longer when a word target is primed by a higher frequency neighbor (e.g., blue–BLUR) than when primed by an unrelated word of equivalent frequency (e.g., care–BLUR) – an “inhibitory neighbor priming effect.” In contrast, Zhou et al. (1999) demonstrated facilitatory orthographic neighbor priming for two-character Chinese words (e.g., 华丽–华贵). However, Zhou et al. did not control for relative prime-target frequency, which has been shown to be important in experiments when examining languages with other scripts. In the present Experiment 1 word neighbor primes (e.g., 容易-容貌) produced an inhibitory neighbor priming effect when the prime was higher frequency than the target, paralleling effects in other script languages. In further experiments, paralleling those in other script languages, two-character targets primed by nonword neighbors (容待-容貌) or single constituent characters matching the target in either position (容-容貌) showed significant facilitation. These results suggest that lexical activation/competition processes for two-character Chinese words are reasonably similar to those for words in alphabetic/syllabic script languages.

Type
Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

1. Introduction

Language researchers have used a broad range of experimental paradigms to investigate the mechanisms involved in visual word recognition. The masked priming paradigm (Forster & Davis, Reference Forster and Davis1984) has been a common tool in this research. The masked priming presentation sequence begins with a mask (typically a row of hash marks), then a brief (<60 ms) display of the prime in lowercase, followed by the target in uppercase. Participants are typically unaware of the prime because of its brief presentation and the presence of the mask. As such, in contrast to unmasked priming, this approach is assumed to prevent participants from using processing strategies based on the existence of a relationship between primes and targets (Forster et al., Reference Forster, Davis, Schoknecht and Carter1987; Forster & Davis, Reference Forster and Davis1984).

A variety of data suggests that the display of a word prime activates not just its orthographic representation but also representations of orthographically similar words, in particular, the orthographic neighbors of that word as well as words containing that word (e.g., Adelman et al., Reference Adelman, Johnson, McCormick, McKague, Kinoshita, Bowers, Perry, Lupker, Forster, Cortese, Scaltritti, Aschenbrenner, Coane, White, Yap, Davis, Kim and Davis2014; Peressotti & Grainger, Reference Peressotti and Grainger1999; Stinchcombe et al., Reference Stinchcombe, Lupker and Davis2012). Orthographic neighbors, according to Coltheart et al. (Reference Coltheart, Davelaar, Jonasson, Besner and Dornic1977), are words that can be formed from a target word by substituting one letter for another while preserving letter positions and word length, for instance, “case,” “ease,” and “vast” are all orthographic neighbors of “vase” (see Davis & Taft, Reference Davis and Taft2005, for a similar, but expanded, definition of orthographic neighbors).

The effect of presenting a word’s orthographic neighbor as a prime in a masked priming experiment has been widely investigated in alphabetic script languages such as French (Segui & Grainger, Reference Segui and Grainger1990), Dutch (Brysbaert et al., Reference Brysbaert, Lange and Van Wijnendaele2000), Spanish (Duñabeitia et al., Reference Duñabeitia, Perea and Carreiras2009), and English (Andrews & Hersch, Reference Andrews and Hersch2010; Davis & Lupker, Reference Davis and Lupker2006; Nakayama et al., Reference Nakayama, Sears and Lupker2008). When the prime is more frequent than the target (e.g., blue-BLUR), an inhibitory effect (in comparison to when an unrelated prime is presented, e.g., come-BLUR) is typically obtained. This result, which also emerged in Japanese for words written in syllabic Katakana (Nakayama et al., Reference Nakayama, Sears and Lupker2011) is explained by proposing that, although the brief presentation of the prime activates its orthographic neighbors, it most strongly activates the prime’s lexical representation. That representation then competes with and inhibits activation in the representations of orthographic neighbors, most relevantly, that of the target word. What is important to note, however, is that these inhibition effects typically only emerge (or only emerge strongly) when the prime is a high-frequency word and the target is a low-frequency word (e.g., Davis & Lupker, Reference Davis and Lupker2006). Essentially, in order for clear evidence of inhibition to be observed, the resting activation level of the prime needs to be higher than the resting activation level of the target.

Inhibitory orthographic neighbor priming effects, the core issue in the present Experiment 1, are, therefore, typically explained in terms of the involvement of mechanisms of lexical competition and inhibition with competition/inhibition mechanisms being incorporated into the majority of localist activation-based models of visual word identification, including the interactive-activation (IA) model (McClelland & Rumelhart, Reference McClelland and Rumelhart1981) and the multiple read-out model (Grainger & Jacobs, Reference Grainger and Jacobs1996), as well as other more recent IA-type models (e.g., Davis, Reference Davis2010). These models specifically assume that during the early stages of processing, the lexical representations of a presented word and those of orthographically similar words (e.g., the word’s “neighbors”) are simultaneously activated, and that, once activated, they compete with one another through a process involving mutual inhibition with the more activated representation successfully inhibiting its competitors.

Consistent with these ideas is the fact that when a nonword neighbor prime is used, the result is typically facilitation rather than inhibition (e.g., Andrews & Hersch, Reference Andrews and Hersch2010; Davis & Lupker, Reference Davis and Lupker2006; Forster et al., Reference Forster, Davis, Schoknecht and Carter1987; Forster & Veres, Reference Forster and Veres1998). That is, nonword neighbor primes would partially activate the target’s lexical representation (as well as representations of other orthographically similar words). However, because a nonword lacks a lexical representation there is no competition between its lexical representation and that of the word target. Note finally that there is typically a null priming effect when a word prime is a lower frequency neighbor of the target. Because the prime word would, presumably, have a lower resting activation level than the target, it would have less ability to compete with the target when the target is presented. Essentially, whatever activation is provided by the orthographic similarity of the prime and target is canceled by the lower ability of the prime’s representation to compete, leading to a latency that is often no different than that produced following an unrelated prime.

2. Orthographic neighbor priming effects in logographic scripts

In contrast to letters in alphabetic script languages, characters in logographic script languages are more than simple orthographic symbols. Characters are frequently morphemes as well (morphemes are defined as the smallest meaning-based units in a language). At this point, less is known about the nature of orthographic neighbor priming effects in logographic script languages despite the fact that neighbor priming effects (from both word and nonword primes) are well established in alphabetic script languages. There has, however, been some research on this issue in both Japanese and Chinese. In one example, Nakayama et al. (Reference Nakayama, Sears, Hino and Lupker2014) investigated neighbor priming of two-character Kanji words, applying Coltheart et al.’s (Reference Coltheart, Davelaar, Jonasson, Besner and Dornic1977) definition of orthographic neighbors and treating individual Kanji characters as the basic orthographic units, a method that has often been used in previous Japanese (e.g., Hino et al., Reference Hino, Miyamura and Lupker2011; Kawakami, Reference Kawakami2002) and Chinese (e.g., Huang et al., Reference Huang, Lee, Tsai, Lee, Hung and Tzeng2006; Tsai et al., Reference Tsai, Lee, Lin, Tzeng and Hung2006) studies. (In Japanese, two-character words make up around 80% of all Kanji words (e.g., Hino et al., Reference Hino, Miyamura and Lupker2011; Hino & Lupker, Reference Hino and Lupker1998).) Specifically, Kanji neighbors share a character in the same position. For example, the Kanji words 企業 and 仕業 are considered orthographic neighbors that differ at the first character position, and the Kanji words 会議 and 会話 are considered orthographic neighbors that differ at the second character position. This definition of orthographic neighbors for Kanji two-character words was also used in our investigation of orthographic neighbor priming in Chinese (a language with a logographic script quite similar to that of Kanji).

Using two-character Japanese Kanji words, Nakayama et al. (Reference Nakayama, Sears, Hino and Lupker2014) obtained inhibitory neighbor priming effects. Specifically, inhibitory neighbor priming effects were obtained in four studies for low-frequency targets primed by higher-frequency Kanji word neighbors (情報-情緒, Experiments 1A, 1B, 3A, and 3B). Targets primed by Kanji nonword neighbors, in contrast, showed a noticeable facilitation effect (情門-情緒, Experiments 2 and 3). Further, when targets were primed by individual constituent Kanji characters (情-情緒, Experiment 4), a significant facilitation effect was also found. These findings imply that, as with words in alphabetic script languages, lexical competition influences the identification of Kanji words. The results of Nakayama et al.’s Experiment 4, showing that subset primes also produce facilitation, are also consistent with the results in alphabetic script languages (e.g., Peressotti & Grainger, Reference Peressotti and Grainger1999; Stinchcombe et al., Reference Stinchcombe, Lupker and Davis2012). However, because the primes and targets in their Experiment 4 also shared a morphological unit, it is possible that the priming observed in Experiment 4 was morphologically based, an interpretation that those authors preferred.

3. Morphological considerations

As noted, Kanji (or Chinese) neighbors are not only orthographically related in that they share a character, but they are also typically morphologically related since each character is a morpheme, even though Kanji or Chinese neighbors are not always semantically related due to the multiple meanings of each morpheme (see Gu et al., Reference Gu, Li and Liversedge2015; Yang et al., Reference Yang, Taikh and Lupker2022). This characteristic of Kanji/Chinese neighbors distinguishes them from most orthographic neighbors in alphabetic languages, such as blue-blur. Therefore, masked neighbor priming in Kanji/Chinese involves a possible facilitation element that is not normally present when priming neighbors in alphabetic script languages (Nakayama et al., Reference Nakayama, Sears, Hino and Lupker2014).

It is now clear that morphologically-based priming effects do appear in masked priming experiments in alphabetic script languages (e.g., Crepaldi et al., Reference Crepaldi, Rastle, Coltheart and Nickels2010; Heathcote et al., Reference Heathcote, Nation, Castles and Beyersmann2018; Rastle et al., Reference Rastle, Davis and New2004). Most relevant to the present research, morphological facilitation effects have been found for compound word prime-target pairs. For example, Crepaldi et al. (Reference Crepaldi, Rastle, Davis and Lupker2013) found that transposed morpheme primes (e.g., moonhoney) facilitated the processing of multi-morphemic targets (HONEYMOON), whereas monomorphic primes transposed in a similar way (e.g., ticeprac) did not facilitate target processing (PRACTICE). Therefore, it is possible that Nakayama et al.’s (Reference Nakayama, Sears, Hino and Lupker2014) neighbor priming effect in their Experiment 4 as well as any neighbor priming effect in Chinese may be at least somewhat morphologically driven.

In fact, some models of visual word recognition in alphabetic script languages are based on the idea that morphological representations play an important role in word recognition, hence, predicting priming from morphologically related primes. For example, Taft and Nguyen-Hoan (Reference Taft and Nguyen-Hoan2010) proposed that morphemes are represented at both the form and lemma level, with the lemma level being an abstract level coding consistent form–meaning linkages. Higher-level representations are activated as a result of activating the lemma-level units. Similarly, in the hybrid dual-route model, Diependaele et al. (Reference Diependaele, Sandra and Grainger2009) suggested that morpho-orthographic decomposition exists with the activation of morphological units driving visual word recognition although holistic processing occurs at the same time.

Although these models differ somewhat in how morphological representations are assumed to be activated, they both suggest that morphological representations can be activated automatically early in word recognition in alphabetic script languages, producing morphological priming effects. As the logographic script used in Chinese appears to be even more morphologically based than alphabetic scripts, one would imagine that morphological priming effects would be even more obvious in Chinese language experiments. Indeed, there are experiments that do seem to document facilitation from morphologically related primes (e.g., Tsang & Chen, Reference Tsang and Chen2014; Wu et al., Reference Wu, Duan, Zhao and Tsang2020; Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Marslen-Wilson, Taft and Shu1999). However, the issue is not clear-cut. For example, Yang et al. (Reference Yang, Hino, Chen, Yoshihara, Nakayama, Xue and Lupker2020) reported that facilitatory morphological priming effects were minimal for four-character Chinese words. Nonetheless, the potential impact of morphological priming in Chinese cannot simply be dismissed and, therefore, needs to be considered when evaluating the impact of orthographic neighbor primes.

4. Orthographic neighbor priming effects in Chinese

The general question we addressed concerned the nature of orthographic neighbor priming effects for two-character Chinese words and what impact relative prime-target frequency has on those effects. At present, there does not appear to have been a direct examination of this issue with what appears to be the most extensive examination of Chinese neighbor priming being provided by Zhou et al. (Reference Zhou, Marslen-Wilson, Taft and Shu1999) using a definition of orthographic neighbors similar to that used in Nakayama et al.’s (Reference Nakayama, Sears, Hino and Lupker2014) Kanji experiments. That is, Zhou et al. examined the priming effect between word primes and targets that share one character in the same position (e.g., 华丽-华贵). They found that, with a 57-ms prime duration, there was a significant facilitation effect across four experiments, with targets processed faster when primed by a neighbor, an effect that was independent of shared character position. Further, that facilitation occurred both when the shared character had the same meaning in the prime and target (their MORPH condition: 华丽-华贵, where the character 华 means “splendid”) and when the shared character had different meanings in the prime and target (their CHAR condition: 华侨-华贵, where the character 华 means “Chinese” and “splendid”, respectively) with the MORPH condition consistently producing a larger facilitation effect than the CHAR condition. Based on these results, Zhou et al. concluded that their facilitation effect was at least partially due to morphological overlap, paralleling the morphological priming effects in alphabetic languages (Duñabeitia et al., Reference Duñabeitia, Perea and Carreiras2009; Rastle et al., Reference Rastle, Davis and New2004).

Zhou et al.’s (Reference Zhou, Marslen-Wilson, Taft and Shu1999) experiments, however, were not ideal for testing for lexical competition (i.e., inhibition) effects between orthographic neighbors because they did not attempt to control relative prime-target frequency. Most of their stimulus pairs involved low-frequency primes (e.g., mean frequency = 22 occurrences per million in their Experiment 1) and higher-frequency targets (mean frequency = 47 occurrences per million in their Experiment 1). According to the literature in other languages, lower-frequency neighbor primes typically do not create much lexical competition for their higher-frequency targets (Andrews & Hersch, Reference Andrews and Hersch2010; Davis & Lupker, Reference Davis and Lupker2006; Nakayama et al., Reference Nakayama, Sears and Lupker2008). Therefore, the prime-target frequency relationship would appear to be a factor that must be controlled when investigating the lexical competition process.

One additional issue that is problematic for present purposes is that Zhou et al. (Reference Zhou, Marslen-Wilson, Taft and Shu1999) did not examine the effect of the lexicality of the neighbor primes. Their primes were always words. As previously mentioned, nonword neighbor primes in alphabetic languages do not produce inhibition effects (e.g., Davis & Lupker, Reference Davis and Lupker2006; Forster & Veres, Reference Forster and Veres1998), presumably because nonword primes do not strongly activate any specific neighbor of the target. Everything considered, Zhou et al.’s data would not provide a full view of the nature of orthographic neighbor priming effects in Chinese. Essentially, when examining the nature of orthographically-driven lexical processing in Chinese in order to contrast it with that in other script languages, relative prime-target frequency needs to be manipulated and the distinction between priming from word neighbor primes and priming from nonword neighbor primes needs to be examined.

Two other issues concerning Zhou et al.’s (Reference Zhou, Marslen-Wilson, Taft and Shu1999) experiments should also be mentioned. First, 50% of their prime-target pairs were designed to have a strong whole-word semantic relationship as the shared character is associated with the same morpheme in the prime and target (e.g., 华丽 and 华贵 meaning “magnificent” and “luxurious”, respectively, their MORPH condition). That fact makes that condition potentially problematic for present purposes because Zhou and Marslen-Wilson (Reference Zhou and Marslen-Wilson2000) have shown semantic priming effects with a short SOA (57 ms) for Chinese compound words. Any semantic priming effects would have masked any (inhibitory) effects of lexical competition. In the other 50% of the primes and targets (their CHAR condition), the shared character did not represent the same morpheme. However, it is not entirely clear that all of the pairs were not semantically related. Second, the number of observations in the relevant experiments was considerably smaller than what is currently deemed necessary (see Brysbaert & Stevens, Reference Brysbaert and Stevens2018) with there being 6–10 stimuli per condition and 30–50 participants per experiment.

5. The present research

The goal of the present research was to provide a more comprehensive investigation of orthographic neighbor priming effects for two-character Chinese words by examining potential interactions with prime-target frequency and prime lexicality. In Experiment 1, we used the masked priming lexical decision task and tested whether inhibitory neighbor priming effects obtain for two-character Chinese words when prime-target frequency is varied systematically. Half of the pairings were high-frequency neighbor primes with low-frequency targets, and the other half were low-frequency neighbor primes with high-frequency targets. The expectation is that any inhibition effect should be more evident with high-frequency primes than with low-frequency primes.

In Experiment 2, the influence of prime lexicality on neighbor priming effects was examined in order to determine whether nonword neighbor primes would produce facilitation using those same targets. If so, that result could provide additional evidence that any inhibitory neighbor priming effect obtained in Experiment 1 was due to lexical competition, supporting the idea that the nature of lexical activation in Chinese is similar to that in other script languages.

Experiment 3 tested one more aspect of orthographic priming, whether there is a facilitation for Chinese two-character words using the first-position character or second-position character of the target words as the prime. These primes should produce evidence of facilitation, as is typically reported in alphabetic language experiments (e.g., Peressotti & Grainger, Reference Peressotti and Grainger1999; Stinchcombe et al., Reference Stinchcombe, Lupker and Davis2012).

6. Experiment 1

6.1. Method

6.1.1. Participants

We conducted power calculations based on the significant interaction obtained in a similar priming experiment (partial η 2 = 0.248; Segui & Grainger, Reference Segui and Grainger1990, Experiment 2). Using G*Power 3.1 software, it was determined that a power of 0.80 would be achieved for a similar-size interaction by using 27 participants. We also noted the sample sizes used in Nakayama et al.’s (Reference Nakayama, Sears, Hino and Lupker2014) Experiment 1 and Zhou et al.’s (Reference Zhou, Marslen-Wilson, Taft and Shu1999) Experiment 1, which were 44 and 40, respectively. Ultimately, 46 undergraduate students from Zhejiang Gongshang University participated in this experiment. Each received a small gift for their participation. All were native speakers of Chinese and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision with no reading disorder.

6.1.2. Materials

All the stimuli were two-character Chinese words. One hundred twenty pairs of orthographic neighbors were selected to create the word target pairs. None of the neighbor pairs were semantically related, as judged by the first author (a native Chinese speaker). For each pair, each neighbor could serve as either a prime or a target depending on the frequency condition that the pair was assigned to. One member of the neighbor pair was a high-frequency word and the other was a low-frequency word, according to the SUBTLWF frequency counts in the SUBTLEX-CH database (Cai & Brysbaert, Reference Cai and Brysbaert2010). The frequency for high-frequency members (occurrences per million: M = 151.85, SD = 2.01) was significantly larger than that for low-frequency members (occurrences per million: M = 2.01, SD = 2.18), t = 8.97, p < 0.001. The orthographic neighborhood size for high-frequency members (M = 56.99, SD = 41.06) was similar to that for low-frequency members (M = 59.06, SD = 51.29), t = −0.47, p = 0.641. The orthographic neighborhood size for a two-character Chinese word is the number of other two-character Chinese words that share one of its two characters in the SUBTLEX-CH database in the same position. Sixty neighbor pairs shared a character in the first position, and the other 60 neighbor pairs shared a character in the second position.

The words forming the 120 neighbor pairs were used twice for each participant, once as a target and once as a (related or unrelated) prime. When the high-frequency member of the pair was used as a target, its low-frequency neighbor was used as its related prime (e.g., 容貌 (Appearance) primed 容易 (Easy)) and a low-frequency word from another pair was used as its unrelated prime (e.g., 参差 (Uneven) primed 容易(Easy)). Similarly, when the low-frequency member of the pair was used as a target, its high-frequency neighbor was used as the related prime (e.g., 容易 (Easy) primed 容貌 (Appearance)) and a high-frequency neighbor from another pair was used as the unrelated prime (e.g., 号码 (Number) primed 容貌 (Appearance)). In the experiment, each participant responded to each target word only once, with one member of the pair presented following a related prime and the other member presented following an unrelated prime. Assignment of targets to conditions was counterbalanced across participants. Note that, in this design, all the primes in the related and unrelated conditions were the same across participants. The unrelated primes shared no characters with their targets.

Four hundred and eighty-two-character Chinese nonwords were selected to serve as the nonword primes (240) and targets (240). These nonword stimuli were derived from the nonwords found in the Chinese Lexicon Project (Tse et al., Reference Tse, Yap, Chan, Sze, Shaoul and Lin2017). The manipulation of prime type for the nonword targets was done similarly to that for word targets. However, there was no counterbalancing of lists for nonword targets (only one list of 120 neighbor nonword primes, 120 unrelated primes, and 240 targets were used).

As the assignment of word targets to relatedness conditions was counterbalanced across participants, we created two lists of size 480 (with 120 targets in each condition in each list and with each target being preceded by its neighbor prime in one list and an unrelated prime in the other list and 240 nonword targets). All primes were presented in 25-pt Dengxian light font typeface, whereas the targets were presented in 30-pt SimSun font typeface.

6.1.3. Procedure

The data were collected using Eprime 2.0 software (Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh, PA; see Schneider et al. (Reference Schneider, Eschman and Zuccolotto2002)). The stimulus color was black and the background color was white. We used a standard 60 Hz refresh rate in a 1,068 × 768-pixel CRT screen.

Each trial began with a pattern mask (####) for 500 ms, followed by a 50 ms prime, and then the target appeared for 3,000 ms or until the participant responded. The stimuli were all displayed in the middle of the screen. Participants were asked to decide if each character string was a real Chinese word or not and to press the “J” button if it was a word, and the “F” button if it wasn’t. They were instructed to respond as rapidly and accurately as possible. Each participant’s trial order was randomized. Sixteen practice trials not involving any stimuli from the experiment proper preceded presentation of the experimental stimuli. Participants had a short break after completing every 160 trials.Footnote 1

6.2. Results

For word targets, latencies less than 250 ms or greater than 1,800 ms (2.2% of the data) and incorrect responses (9.7% of the data) were excluded from the latency analyses. The mean RTs and percentage error rates, as a function of Relatedness (neighbor vs. unrelated) and Frequency Type (high-frequency vs. low-frequency targets) based on subject means for the word targets are shown in Table 1. The data from nonword targets were not analyzed due to the fact that the nonword targets were not counterbalanced across prime types. Generalized Linear Mixed-effects Models (GLMM) from the lme4 package (version 1.1.23) in R software (version 3.6.1) were used to analyze the latency and error rate data (Bates et al., Reference Bates, Maechler, Bolker and Walker2015; Lo & Andrews, Reference Lo and Andrews2015; R Core Team, 2015).

Table 1. Mean lexical decision latencies (RTs, in milliseconds) and percentage error rate for words in experiment 1

Abbreviation. RT = reaction time; %E = percentage error rate.

In the GLMM analysis, Relatedness, Frequency Type, and their interaction were entered as fixed factors. Subjects and items were entered as random factors. For the latency analysis of word targets, the model was: RT = glmer (RT ~ Relatedness * Frequency Type + (Frequency Type |subject) + (1 |item), family = Gamma(link = “identity”), control = glmerControl (optimizer = “bobyqa”)). For the error rate analysis of word targets, the model was: Accuracy = glmer (accuracy ~ Relatedness * Frequency Type + (Frequency Type |subject) + (1 |item), family = “binomial”, control = glmerControl(optimizer = “bobyqa”)). More complex models that included all relevant random structures were used in our initial analyses. Ultimately, we had to use the models noted above due to convergence failures with the more complex random slope models (Barr et al., Reference Barr, Levy, Scheepers and Tily2013). Before running the model, R-default treatment contrasts were altered to sum-to-zero contrasts (Levy, Reference Levy2014; Singmann & Kellen, Reference Singmann, Kellen, Spieler and Schumacher2019). Follow-up analyses were conducted using the emmeans package, version 1.5.0 (Russell, Reference Russell2020). The raw data used for the analyses and word stimuli used in all different experiments are publicly available at https://osf.io/8cdnm/.

6.2.1. Latency and error rate analyses

The main effects of Relatedness, ß = 2.48, SE = 1.20, z = 2.07, p = .038, and Frequency Type were significant, ß = 62.57, SE = 3.31, z = 18.93, p < 0.001, indicating that, overall, the related condition latencies were longer than those in the unrelated condition (i.e., an inhibitory priming effect) and low-frequency target latencies were longer than the latencies for high-frequency targets. Importantly, there was an interaction between Relatedness and Frequency Type, ß = 5.01, SE = 1.21, z = 4.13, p < 0.001. In a follow-up analysis, only the inhibitory priming effect from the high-frequency neighbor primes for low-frequency targets was significant (z = 3.99, p < 0.001). The 6-ms facilitation effect from the low-frequency neighbor primes for high-frequency targets was marginal (z = −1.68, p = .094).

In the error rate analysis, a similar pattern emerged. The main effect of Relatedness was significant, ß = −0.15, SE = 0.05, z = −3.16, p = .002, as was the main effect of Frequency Type, ß = −1.14, SE = 0.12, z = −9.42, p < 0.001, indicating that, overall, targets in the related condition and low-frequency targets produced more errors than targets in the unrelated condition and high-frequency targets, respectively. There was, again, an interaction between Relatedness and Frequency Type, ß = −0.11, SE = 0.05, z = −2.36, p = .019. In a follow-up analysis, only the inhibitory effect from high-frequency primes for low-frequency targets was significant (z = −6.19, p < .001).

6.2.2. Bayes Factor analyses

We also conducted a Bayes Factor analysis for word latencies to quantify the statistical evidence in favor of or against the interaction between Relatedness and Frequency Type. The Bayes factor analysis was calculated using the “lmBF” function and “compare” function from the BayesFactor package (version 0.9.12.4.2) with the default JZS type being used to calculate the Bayes factor (Morey et al., Reference Morey, Rouder and Jamil2015). The analysis was based on subject-averaged latencies. Model 1 (the full model with an interaction) was compared with Model 0 (the null model with no interaction). The contrast between these two models produced a BF 10 of 7.59 ± 4.40%, favoring the hypothesis that there was an interaction between Relatedness and Frequency Type in the latency analysis.

6.2.3. Supplementary analysis

Although efforts were made to make sure that the prime-target pairs were not semantically similar, for 10 pairs of neighbors the shared character had the same morphemic meaning in the prime and target words (i.e., those primes and targets were morphologically similar). Therefore, in a supplementary analysis, we removed those 10 pairs of neighbors from the analysis in order to make certain that the effects reported in Experiment 1 did not reflect any meaning similarity. In this analysis, the main effects of Relatedness and Frequency Type were significant (all p’s < .05). Importantly, there was still a significant two-way interaction between Relatedness and Frequency Type, ß = 4.93, SE = 1.25, z = 3.94, p < 0.001. In follow-up analyses, only the 16 ms inhibitory priming effect from the high-frequency neighbor primes for low-frequency targets was significant (z = 3.82, p < 0.001). The 5-ms facilitation effect from the low-frequency neighbor primes for high-frequency targets was not (z = −1.50, p = .133). Essentially, therefore, removing the pairs in which primes and targets had some morphological similarity had no impact on our data pattern.Footnote 2

6.3. Discussion

Experiment 1 explored the orthographic neighbor priming effect for two-character Chinese neighbor word pairs. If lexical competition occurs during Chinese word recognition, one would expect to see indications of inhibition in the latency or error rate data. If lexical competition plays no role in reading Chinese words, however, a facilitatory priming effect for Chinese neighbor pairs would be more likely. Our results were consistent with the first possibility. We found an inhibition effect for pairs involving a high-frequency neighbor prime and a low-frequency target in both the latency and error rate data, with no significant priming effect being obtained for pairs involving low-frequency neighbor primes and high-frequency targets. This pattern is consistent with the results from other languages. High-frequency primes cause inhibition for low-frequency targets, whereas observing inhibition effects with low-frequency primes and high-frequency targets is rare. Our lexical competition effect was slightly smaller than that often obtained in alphabetic script language experiments (e.g., Davis & Lupker, Reference Davis and Lupker2006; Nakayama et al., Reference Nakayama, Sears and Lupker2008; Segui & Grainger, Reference Segui and Grainger1990), but slightly more robust than that obtained in Kanji script (Nakayama et al., Reference Nakayama, Sears, Hino and Lupker2014).

7. Experiment 2

A prime lexicality effect in masked form priming is an important prediction of the localist models such as the IA model (McClelland & Rumelhart, Reference McClelland and Rumelhart1981). According to these IA-type models, facilitatory and inhibitory priming effects combine to produce form priming effects. All primes provide activation to the representations of all words that are orthographically similar. However, in the case of word primes, the prime is a competitor of the target. As such, because a high-frequency word prime like “able” will most powerfully activate the lexical representation of itself, a representation that has a high level of activation in general because of its high frequency, that representation will become a powerful competitor. Hence, when the low-frequency target word AXLE is presented, its processing will be slowed due to the competition from the highly activated representation for able. Such is not the case when the prime-target relationship is reversed due to the fact that the representation for axle has a low resting activation level and the representation for able has a high resting activation level. The result is a priming effect that is typically around 0. What is important here, however, is that the facilitatory component of form priming is assumed to be comparable for orthographically similar word and nonword primes. Accordingly, response latencies to the target AXLE would be facilitated by nonword neighbor primes (e.g., azle), even though they are slowed down by high-frequency word neighbor primes (e.g., able).

This assumed impact of the lexicality of orthographic neighbor primes was the main issue in Experiment 2. Numerous studies using alphabetic script languages have consistently demonstrated facilitatory form priming from nonword primes and inhibitory form priming from related word primes (Andrews & Hersch, Reference Andrews and Hersch2010; Davis & Lupker, Reference Davis and Lupker2006; Forster et al., Reference Forster, Davis, Schoknecht and Carter1987; Forster & Veres, Reference Forster and Veres1998). As well, Kanji experiments (Nakayama et al., Reference Nakayama, Sears, Hino and Lupker2014) have shown a significant facilitation effect when targets are primed by Kanji nonword neighbors. Zhou et al.’s (Reference Zhou, Marslen-Wilson, Taft and Shu1999) experiments, as noted, did not include nonword primes (nor does it appear that this issue has been examined elsewhere in the literature on Chinese word recognition). From a theoretical standpoint, it would be important to test the effect of lexicality on neighbor priming using the set of targets from Experiment 1 in order to determine whether Chinese produces that same pattern as produced in experiments in other languages.

Essentially, if IA-type models are valid for Chinese, one would expect that when using nonword neighbor primes (e.g., two-character Chinese nonword primes that share one character in the same position as in the two-character Chinese word targets), there should be a facilitation effect for low- (as well as high-) frequency targets since nonword primes do not have lexical representations. This is the question addressed in Experiment 2 using the same target words as used in Experiment 1.

7.1. Method

7.1.1. Participants

We conducted power calculations based on the significant interaction obtained in a similar priming experiment (partial η 2 = 0.137; Nakayama et al., Reference Nakayama, Sears, Hino and Lupker2014, Experiment 2). Using G*Power 3.1 software, it was determined that a power of 0.80 would be achieved for a similar-size interaction by using 53 participants. Ultimately, 58 undergraduate students from Zhejiang Gongshang University participated in this experiment. Each received a small gift for their participation. All were native speakers of Chinese and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision with no reading disorder.

7.1.2. Materials

The same 120 pairs of orthographic neighbors used in Experiment 1 were used as word targets. As noted, one member of the neighbor pair was a high-frequency target and the other was a low-frequency target, according to the SUBTLWF frequency counts in the SUBTLEX-CH database (Cai & Brysbaert, Reference Cai and Brysbaert2010).

One hundred twenty nonword neighbors that shared one character with a target in the same position as in the word neighbor were generated to use as nonword primes. The same nonword neighbor primed both members of each neighbor pair when that target appeared in the related condition. As in Experiment 1, both members of the pair were shown to each participant as targets, once in the related condition, and once in the unrelated condition in a counterbalanced fashion. The unrelated prime-target pairs were created by re-pairing the primes and targets used in the unrelated condition for that participant group.

The nonword targets and primes were the same as in Experiment 1. The other details were the same as those in Experiment 1.

7.1.3. Procedure

The basic procedure was the same as that in Experiment 1.

7.2. Results

For word targets, latencies less than 250 ms or greater than 1,800 ms (2.3% of the data) and incorrect responses (8.5% of the data) were excluded from the latency analyses. The mean RTs and percentage error rates, as a function of Relatedness (neighbor vs. unrelated) and Frequency Type (high frequency vs. low frequency) based on the subject means for the word targets are shown in Table 2. For the latency analysis, the model was: RT = glmer (RT ~ Relatedness * Frequency Type + (Frequency Type |subject) + (1 |item), family = Gamma(link = “identity”), control = glmerControl (optimizer = “bobyqa”)). For the error rate analysis, the model was: Accuracy = glmer (accuracy ~ Relatedness * Frequency Type + (Frequency Type |subject) + (1 |item), family = “binomial”, control = glmerControl(optimizer = “bobyqa”)). The other details were the same as those in Experiment 1.

Table 2. Mean lexical decision latencies (RTs, in milliseconds) and percentage error rate for words in experiment 2

Abbreviation. RT = reaction time; %E = percentage error rate.

7.2.1. Latency and error rate analyses

The main effect of Relatedness was significant, ß = −2.64, SE = 1.05, z = −2.51, p = .012, as was the main effect of Frequency Type, ß = −54.64, SE = 3.56, z = −15.37, p < 0.001, indicating that there was a facilitatory priming effect (i.e., latencies were faster following related primes) and that there was a standard target frequency effect (low-frequency target latencies were longer than high-frequency target latencies). There was no interaction between Relatedness and Frequency Type, ß = −0.14, SE = 1.07, z = −0.13, p = .897.

In the error rate analysis, the main effect of Relatedness was significant, ß = −0.11, SE = 0.04, z = −2.55, p = .011, indicating a small (1.2%) inhibitory effect (the related condition produced slightly more errors than the unrelated condition). The main effect of Frequency Type (in the expected direction) was also significant, ß = 1.01, SE = 0.11, z = 9.21, p < 0.001. Again, there was no suggestion of an interaction between Relatedness and Frequency type, ß = −0.02, SE = 0.04, z = −0.48, p = 0.634.

7.2.2. Bayes Factor analyses

We also conducted a Bayes Factor analysis for word latencies to quantify the statistical evidence in favor of or against the interaction between Relatedness and Frequency Type. Model 1 (the full model with an interaction) was compared with Model 0 (the null model with no interaction). The contrast between these two models produced a BF 10 of 0.22 ± 6.05%, favoring the hypothesis that there was no interaction between Relatedness and Frequency Type in the latency analysis.

We also conducted a Bayes Factor analysis for word latencies to quantify the statistical evidence in favor of or against the main effect of Relatedness. Model 1 (the full model with a main effect) was compared with Model 0 (the null model with no main effect). The contrast between these two models produced a BF 10 of 3.02 ± 0.75%, favoring the hypothesis that there was a Relatedness effect in the latency analysis.

Finally, we conducted a Bayes Factor analysis for error rates to quantify the statistical evidence in favor of or against the main effect of Relatedness. Model 1 (the full model with a main effect) was compared with Model 0 (the null model with no main effect). The contrast between these two models produced a BF 10 of 0.29 ± 3.06%, favoring the hypothesis that there was no Relatedness effect in the error analysis.

8. Discussion

Experiment 2’s results using nonword neighbor primes were different from Experiment 1’s results using word neighbor primes with respect to the low-frequency targets, in that the nonword primes produced a significant facilitation effect for those targets, at least in the latency data. That is, even the targets that showed an inhibition effect from high-frequency neighbor word primes in Experiment 1 produced a small but significant facilitation effect in Experiment 2. These results are consistent with IA-type models. That is, because nonwords do not have lexical representations, one would not expect them to produce much lexical competition/inhibition. Therefore, any facilitation obtained due to either orthographic similarity (or morphological relatedness due to the shared constituent character) would be expected to have a facilitatory impact. These results support the idea that lexical competition effects do exist in word recognition for Chinese two-character words, with those effects being noticeable when the primes are high-frequency words, just as is typically observed in other script languages.

One potential puzzle is that our nonword neighbor priming effects were smaller than parallel effects obtained in Kanji (30 ms from nonword neighbor primes for low-frequency word targets, Nakayama et al., Reference Nakayama, Sears, Hino and Lupker2014). One possibility is that the reduced nonword neighbor priming effects could be a “shared neighborhood” effect, as the Chinese target words have a large number of orthographic neighbors (the mean number of orthographic neighbors is above 56). That is, with a large number of orthographic neighbors, there is more potential for any activation produced by the prime to be divided among the neighbors, leading to a small priming effect (Forster et al., Reference Forster, Davis, Schoknecht and Carter1987; Nakayama et al., Reference Nakayama, Sears and Lupker2008; Van Heuven et al., Reference Van Heuven, Dijkstra, Grainger and Schriefers2001). However, even if this argument is viable, it may seem a bit surprising that the impact of orthographic similarity was so small. A second (and not mutually exclusive) possibility is that, in Experiment 2, there might have been some inhibition from the unrelated character in the related primes (i.e., the character that, combined with the character shared between the prime and the target, made up the related nonword prime). Experiment 3 was an attempt to examine this issue more closely.

9. Experiment 3

In Experiment 3 we used the same word targets as used in Experiments 1 and 2, and each word target was primed by the single overlapping character of the orthographic neighbor used in those experiments. For example, the word target “容貌” was primed either by a single constituent character “容” or by an unrelated character “怀”. Thus, there is no information in any related prime that is inconsistent with the target and that might inhibit processing of that target. Only low-frequency targets were used as they were the targets that showed different priming effects across the two previous experiments.

Note also that the position of the prime character in the target was manipulated. In left-to-right languages, sequential models of letter processing in word reading propose an initial left-to-right processing sequence, with each letter requiring 10 to 25 milliseconds to process (Whitney, Reference Whitney2001). Furthermore, in paradigms concentrating on full word recognition (e.g., Scaltritti & Balota, Reference Scaltritti and Balota2013) and even sentence reading (e.g., Johnson & Eisler, Reference Johnson and Eisler2012; Jordan et al., Reference Jordan, Thomas, Patching and Scott-Brown2003), a distinctive status for letters in the first position has been demonstrated. Chinese is also a left-to-right language, so in this experiment, we introduced position as a factor in order to test whether there is any first character priming advantage. That is, the question was would there be a larger priming effect when Chinese compound targets were primed by their first character than by their second character?

9.1. Method

9.1.1. Participants

We conducted power calculations based on the significant main effect obtained in a similar priming experiment (partial η 2 = 0.459; Nakayama et al., Reference Nakayama, Sears, Hino and Lupker2014, Experiment 4). Using G*Power 3.1 software, it was determined that a power of 0.80 would be achieved with 13 participants. We also noted the sample sizes used in Nakayama et al.’s (Reference Nakayama, Sears, Hino and Lupker2014) Experiment 4 and Zhou et al.’s (Reference Zhou, Marslen-Wilson, Taft and Shu1999) Experiment 1, which were 36 and 40, respectively. Ultimately, 40 undergraduate students from Zhejiang Gongshang University participated in this experiment. Each received a small gift for their participation. All were native speakers of Chinese and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision with no reading disorder.

9.1.2. Materials

The word targets were the 120 low-frequency words used in Experiment 1. Each word target was primed by a constituent character or an unrelated character. The constituent character was the same character that was shared in the orthographic neighbor pair in Experiment 1. The unrelated prime-target pairs were again created by re-pairing the primes and targets. Relatedness was again counterbalanced across participants, resulting in two lists with 120 word targets (with 60 related primes and 60 unrelated primes in each list). In both lists, half of the related primes shared the constituent character in the first position and the other half of the related primes shared the constituent character in the second position, and each target was preceded by its constituent character prime in one list and an unrelated character prime in the other list.

Another 120 two-character Chinese nonword targets, selected from those targets used in Experiments 1 and 2, were also included, but only half of the nonwords from those experiments were used in Experiment 3. The manipulation of prime type for the nonword targets was done in a similar fashion as for word targets. However, there was no counterbalancing of lists for nonword targets (there was only one list of 60 constituent character primes and targets and one list of 60 unrelated character primes and targets). The other details were the same as in Experiment 1.

9.1.3. Procedure

The procedure was the same as that in Experiment 1. Note that the one-character prime was presented centrally.

9.2. Results

For word targets, latencies less than 250 ms or greater than 1,800 ms (2.7% of the data) and incorrect responses (11.4% of the data) were excluded from the latency analyses. The mean RTs and percentage error rates, as a function of Relatedness (neighbor vs. unrelated) and Position in the target (first vs. second), from the subject-based analysis for the word targets are shown in Table 3. For the latency analysis of word targets, the model was: RT = glmer (RT ~ Relatedness * Position + (Position |subject) + (1 |item), family = Gamma(link = “identity”), control = glmerControl (optimizer = “bobyqa”)). For the error rate analysis of word targets, the model was: Accuracy = glmer (accuracy ~ Relatedness * Position + (Position |subject) + (1 |item), family = “binomial”, control = glmerControl(optimizer = “bobyqa”)). The other details were the same as those in Experiment 1.

Table 3. Mean lexical decision latencies (RTs, in milliseconds) and percentage error rate for words in experiment 3

Abbreviation. RT = reaction time; %E = percentage error rate.

9.2.1. Latency and error rate analyses

The main effect of Relatedness was significant, ß = −5.48, SE = 2.05, z = −2.67, p = 0.008, indicating faster latencies for related than unrelated primes, whereas the main effect of Position was not significant, ß = −10.57, SE = 6.41, z = −1.65, p = 0.099. The interaction between Relatedness and Position was also not significant, ß = −2.47, SE = 2.02, z = −1.22, p = 0.222.

In the error rate analysis, the main effect of Relatedness was not significant, ß = 0.08, SE = 0.05, z = 1.61, p = 0.107, however, the main effect of Position was significant, ß = 0.39, SE = 0.15, z = 2.62, p = 0.009. Targets primed by related primes overlapping in the second position produced more errors than targets primed by related primes overlapping in the first position, averaged across conditions. Again, there was no significant interaction between Relatedness and Position, ß = 0.08, SE = 0.05, z = 1.53, p = 0.127.

9.2.2. Bayes Factor analyses

We also conducted a Bayes Factor analysis for word latencies to quantify the statistical evidence in favor of or against the null interaction between Relatedness and Position. Model 1 (the full model with an interaction) was compared with Model 0 (the null model with no interaction). The contrast between these two models produced a BF 10 of 0.30 ± 5.97%, favoring the hypothesis that there was no interaction between Relatedness and Position in the latency analysis.

We also conducted a Bayes Factor analysis for word latencies to quantify the statistical evidence in favor of or against the main effect of Relatedness. Model 1 (the full model with a main effect) was compared with Model 0 (the null model with no main effect). The contrast between these two models produced a BF 10 of 1.44 ± 0.71%, only very slightly favoring the hypothesis that there was a main effect of Relatedness in the latency analysis.

9.3. Discussion

A facilitatory priming effect was observed in this experiment, with targets primed by one of their constituent characters being responded to faster and more accurately than targets primed by an unrelated character, even though this effect was weak according to the Bayes factor analysis. These results are consistent with previous priming results using a longer prime duration (Gao et al., Reference Gao, Wang, Zhao and Yuan2022; Peng et al., Reference Peng, Li and Liu1994) as well as those observed in alphabetic script languages (Adelman et al., Reference Adelman, Johnson, McCormick, McKague, Kinoshita, Bowers, Perry, Lupker, Forster, Cortese, Scaltritti, Aschenbrenner, Coane, White, Yap, Davis, Kim and Davis2014; Peressotti & Grainger, Reference Peressotti and Grainger1999; Stinchcombe et al., Reference Stinchcombe, Lupker and Davis2012) and in Kanji (Nakayama et al., Reference Nakayama, Sears, Hino and Lupker2014). Moreover, the size of the facilitatory priming effect was not significantly affected by the character position being primed, a result that is also consistent with the findings in other similar masked priming studies (e.g., Shoolman & Andrews, Reference Shoolman, Andrews, Kinoshita and Lupker2003; Nakayama et al., Reference Nakayama, Sears, Hino and Lupker2014).

Importantly, these results support two further conclusions. First, the small priming effect in Experiment 2 was not due to the existence of a second character in the primes that was incompatible with the target. As noted, it may have been the case that the small size of the priming effects in Experiment 2 was due to the large size of the neighborhood activated by the primes. The single-character primes used in Experiment 3, of course, do not have neighbors in the classical sense. However, the basic argument here would be that they do activate representations of words containing those characters (e.g., see Davis & Taft, Reference Davis and Taft2005). Therefore, the primes in Experiment 3 would have activated a very large set of word representations, potentially preventing any given word’s representation that contained that character from becoming strongly activated.

The second, and more central, conclusion, based on the fact that the identical targets produced different effects in Experiments 3 (where the related primes were constituents of their targets) and 1 (where the related primes were orthographic neighbors of those targets) is that the inhibition effect in Experiment 1 was due to lexical competition among the representations of two-character Chinese words.

10. General discussion

The empirical goal of the present experiments was to examine masked orthographic neighbor priming effects for two-character Chinese words in an effort to contrast those effects with the effects found in other script languages. The specific focus of Experiment 1 was whether there would be an inhibitory neighbor priming effect when prime-target frequency was manipulated in the same way that it is typically manipulated in experiments examining other scripts and when there was essentially no semantic/morphological relationship strength between the primes and targets. Indeed, Experiment 1 showed an inhibition effect for pairs involving a high-frequency neighbor prime and a low-frequency target in both the latency and error rate data, whereas a small, nonsignificant priming effect was obtained for pairs involving a low-frequency neighbor prime and high-frequency target. In Experiment 2, the same targets produced a small but significant facilitatory priming effect in the latency data for both the high-frequency targets and low-frequency targets when primed by nonword primes, again, as typically obtained in other language/script experiments. In Experiment 3, a facilitatory priming effect was obtained in the latency data when the related prime was a component (i.e., one character) of the (low-frequency) target with the size of the priming effect not being significantly affected by character position. In general, the conclusion seems to be that whereas orthographic neighbor priming does obtain for Chinese two-character words, lexical competition plays a role in the recognition of those words.

These results are consistent with localist IA-type models of visual word identification (Davis, Reference Davis, Kinoshita and Lupker2003, Reference Davis2010; Grainger & Jacobs, Reference Grainger and Jacobs1996; McClelland & Rumelhart, Reference McClelland and Rumelhart1981). One of the fundamental assumptions of the majority of those models is that there is competition among the lexical units of orthographically similar words (i.e., orthographic neighbors). This prediction has been supported by prior research on masked priming with alphabetic script languages (e.g., Davis & Lupker, Reference Davis and Lupker2006; Nakayama et al., Reference Nakayama, Sears and Lupker2008; Segui & Grainger, Reference Segui and Grainger1990). The fact that Nakayama et al. (Reference Nakayama, Sears and Lupker2011) found a similar inhibitory neighbor priming effect with Japanese Katakana words suggests that lexical processing in nonalphabetic script languages also involves lexical competition. To this point, however, there had been no demonstration that such was also the case in Chinese.

As noted, there was, however, one prior direct examination of inhibitory priming in a logographic script language. Nakayama et al. (Reference Nakayama, Sears, Hino and Lupker2014) using Japanese Kanji stimuli, demonstrated inhibitory neighbor priming effects, although those effects were typically limited to error rates (see their Experiments 1A, 1B, and 3A). Only in their Experiment 3B, when participants were instructed to prioritize accuracy over speed when responding, was an inhibitory neighbor priming effect shown in the latency data. Therefore, evidence for inhibitory effects in that set of experiments was a bit weak. In the present Experiment 1, participants were given standard lexical decision instructions (i.e., to respond as quickly and accurately as possible) with the result being that inhibitory priming effects were obtained in both latencies and error rates, meaning that our data were a bit more similar to data obtained in previous alphabetic script language studies (e.g., Andrews & Hersch, Reference Andrews and Hersch2010; Brysbaert et al., Reference Brysbaert, Lange and Van Wijnendaele2000; Davis & Lupker, Reference Davis and Lupker2006; Nakayama et al., Reference Nakayama, Sears and Lupker2008; Segui & Grainger, Reference Segui and Grainger1990).

Our findings differ to a greater degree from those of the previous investigations of neighbor priming effects for two-character Chinese words, particularly the extensive set of experiments reported by Zhou et al. (Reference Zhou, Marslen-Wilson, Taft and Shu1999). Zhou et al. reported a significant facilitatory priming effect in essentially all relevant orthographic priming conditions. As noted, however, a crucial factor that was not controlled in Zhou et al.’s studies was the relative frequencies of the primes and targets, with the average prime frequencies being either less than or equal to the average target frequencies. In that situation, it would be difficult for the primes to produce strong lexical competition for the targets. As noted, the inhibitory priming effect in our Experiment 1 only emerged when the prime frequencies were considerably higher than the target frequencies. In addition, Zhou et al. did not endeavor to make sure that prime-target pairings (both neighbor pairs and unrelated pairs) were not semantically related at the whole word level, as was done in our Experiment 1. It is plausible that the differences in findings between that experiment and Zhou et al.’s experiments may have been at least partially due to there being some level of semantic relatedness between primes and targets in their experiments even in their CHAR condition.

10.1. Disentangling the impact of morphological and orthographic similarity

Zhou et al.’s (Reference Zhou, Marslen-Wilson, Taft and Shu1999) experiments are not the only experiments that seem to show morphological priming in Chinese. As noted, in their experiments, the orthographically related prime-target pairs in their MORPH condition were also morphologically (and semantically) related in the sense that the overlapping character represented the same morpheme in the prime and target. When there was no morphological overlap (i.e., their CHAR condition), there was reduced priming compared to their MORPH condition. One interpretation of this pattern is that both effects are orthographic with the additional effect in the MORPH condition being due to morphological similarity. (As the primes were all low-frequency words, one would not have expected them to have produced much lexical competition.) However, an alternative interpretation is that the effects were completely due to orthographic similarity and there is an inhibition of processing whenever a shared character in the prime and target must activate different morphemes.

This latter (morphological competition) interpretation gains some support from the results of Yang et al. (Reference Yang, Taikh and Lupker2022) and Tsang and Chen (Reference Tsang and Chen2014). Yang et al. examined masked transposed character priming of two-character Chinese words. They showed that priming effects were reduced when the two characters represented morphemes that were unrelated to the full meaning of the target word (English example: petcar priming CARPET). Tsang and Chen (Reference Tsang and Chen2014) also investigated masked priming of two-character Chinese words. In their related condition, the first character of the prime and target matched. In their crucial condition (their “transparent-opaque” condition), the shared character in the prime represented a morpheme consistent with the meaning of the whole word while the shared character in the target did not. This condition, which, in many ways, resembles the CHAR condition in Zhou et al.’s (Reference Zhou, Marslen-Wilson, Taft and Shu1999) experiments, produced no priming effect, suggesting that it was difficult to process the meaning of the shared character once the prime had activated the more common morpheme.

The idea of morpheme competition is not a new one as it has already been incorporated into Taft and Nguyen-Hoan’s (Reference Taft and Nguyen-Hoan2010) model. However, coupled with the present results, what it leaves us with is the challenge of teasing apart what is being facilitated and what is being inhibited in masked priming experiments in Chinese. One would assume that the best way to separate the impact of orthography and morphology is to contrast priming effects of one factor when there is no potential for priming from the other. Doing so is almost impossible when trying to investigate morphological priming since morphologically related primes in Chinese are, by definition, orthographically identical. Doing so is seemingly possible when investigating orthographic priming as was done in the present experiments in which the primes and targets were only minimally morphologically similar. However, even in that situation, the issue is not simple. If the prime and target are orthographically similar (i.e., sharing a character) but those two characters are associated with different morphemes, the potential for morphological competition becomes a real one. In contrast, there would likely not be competition in the unrelated prime condition. The result would be a reduction in the size of any facilitation effect. To the extent that there might have been morphological competition in the present experiments, that fact, together with the issue of the large neighborhood sizes of the primes, may provide an explanation for why our facilitatory priming effects in Experiments 2 and 3 were so small.Footnote 3

11. Conclusions

The present research offers new evidence concerning how orthographic neighbors affect reading Chinese two-character words based on the results of three masked priming experiments. Our theoretically-based goal was to test whether the nature of orthographically-driven lexical processing when reading two-character Chinese words paralleled that in other languages with a key question being whether lexical competition occurs when reading those words. We found the typical (in other scripts) inhibition effect only for high-frequency neighbor prime and low-frequency target word pairs in both the latency and error rate data when the contribution of morphology/semantics was minimized. Further, when the same targets were primed by their nonword neighbors or constituent characters, the inhibitory priming effects for the low-frequency word targets changed to become a facilitation effect. Together, these findings support the idea that orthographically-driven lexical processing involved in reading Chinese is reasonably similar to that in other script languages in that it involves a lexical competition process, consistent with the assumptions of localist IA-type models.

Financial disclosure

This research was supported by the Fundamental Research Funds for the Provincial Universities of Zhejiang (PYY202256) to Huilan Yang, Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada Grant A6333 to Stephen J. Lupker, Zhejiang Province Higher Education “14th Five-Year Plan” Teaching Reform Project (jg20220256) to Sumin Zhang, Zhejiang Gongshang University “Digital+” Disciplinary Construction Management Project (SZJ2022A004) to Liwen Chen, and The Humanity and Social Science Fund of Ministry of Education of China (20YJC190024) to Sanmei Wu. The raw data used for the analyses and word stimuli used in all different experiments are publicly available at https://osf.io/8cdnm/.

Footnotes

1 The present experiments used a row of hashmarks (####) as a forward mask, which is the standard forward mask adopted in alphabetic script language priming experiments. It is, nonetheless, possible that a row of hashmarks is not very effective as a forward mask when the prime is logographic, and that the observed effects arose from conscious processing of the prime. In order to test the effectiveness of the hashmarks as a forward mask, we recruited another 20 Chinese native speakers to do a masked stimulus lexical decision task. Each trial began with a pattern mask (####) for 500 ms, followed by a 50 ms stimulus, and then one of 12 different types of ancient Chinese characters (e.g., 㐡㐡, 㔣㔣, 㐯㐯), mimicking the impact of the target as a backward mask in the reported experiments, appeared for 3000 ms or until the participant responded. The stimuli were all displayed in the middle of the screen. Participants were asked to decide if each masked stimulus was a real Chinese word or not and to press the “J” button if it was a word, and the “F” button if it wasn’t. They were instructed to respond as rapidly and accurately as possible. Each participant’s trial order was randomized. The stimuli in this experiment were the targets from Experiment 1. Participants were told that half of the trials would be nonwords and half would be words in order to minimize response bias. Participants were told that they might not be able to identify the masked stimulus, but it was nevertheless necessary to make their best guess as to which response was correct. A d’ value was calculated for each participant. The mean d’ value (M = 0.03, SD = 0.10) was tested against 0 (0 means chance responding). This contrast was nonsignificant, t = 1.16, p = 0.262, indicating that the string of hashmarks was a good forward mask in the present experiments.

2 It was likely that the high- and low-frequency words also differed in terms of morpheme/character frequency of the nonshared character which may have played some role in producing the inhibition effect of high-frequency primes on low frequency targets. In order to determine whether the interaction effect remained significant after accounting for any morpheme frequency differences in the nonshared character, we first standardized the morpheme frequency of the nonshared character (M = 337.92, SD = 1219.95 before standardization) using the scale function in R (R Core Team, 2015), then we added the standardized morpheme frequency of the nonshared character for each target word as a continuous variable to the linear mixed effects models from Experiment 1. In this analysis, the main effects of Relatedness and Frequency Type were significant (all p’s < .05), however, the main effect of Morpheme frequency was not (p = 0.361). Importantly, there was still a significant two-way interaction between Relatedness and Frequency Type, ß = 5.01, SE = 1.21, z = 4.16, p < .001. In a follow-up analysis, only the inhibitory priming effect from the high-frequency neighbor primes for low-frequency targets was significant (z = 3.93, p < .001). The facilitation effect from the low-frequency neighbor primes for high-frequency targets was marginal (z = −1.70, p = .09). These results indicate that the core interaction in Experiment 1 was not driven by the nature of the unshared character.

3 As noted, an effort was made to select stimuli for Experiment 1 in which the shared characters in the primes and targets represented different morphemes, which is why the potential for morpheme competition did exist. Note, however, that the meaning dominance of the morphemes associated with those characters was not significantly different for low- versus high-frequency words (t = −1.46, p = 0.148), with the shared character in low-frequency words actually having a slightly higher meaning dominance than the shared character in high-frequency words. Therefore, unlike in Tsang and Chen’s (Reference Tsang and Chen2014) Experiment 1, neither priming condition (low-high vs. high-low) involved pairs that were consistently transparent-opaque.

References

Adelman, J. S., Johnson, R. L., McCormick, S. F., McKague, M., Kinoshita, S., Bowers, J. S., Perry, J. R., Lupker, S. J., Forster, K. I., Cortese, M. J., Scaltritti, M., Aschenbrenner, A. J., Coane, J. H., White, L., Yap, M. J., Davis, C., Kim, J., & Davis, C. J. (2014). A behavioral database for masked form priming. Behavior Research Methods, 46(4), 10521067. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-013-0442-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andrews, S., & Hersch, J. (2010). Lexical precision in skilled readers: Individual differences in Masked neighbor priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(2), 299318. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018366CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 255278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B. M., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 148. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brysbaert, M., Lange, M., & Van Wijnendaele, I. (2000). The effects of age-of-acquisition and frequency-of-occurrence in visual word recognition: Further evidence from the Dutch language. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 12(1), 6585. https://doi.org/10.1080/095414400382208CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brysbaert, M., & Stevens, M. (2018). Power analysis and effect size in mixed effects models: A tutorial. Journal of Cognition, 1(1), 120. https://doi.org/10.5334/joc.10CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cai, Q., & Brysbaert, M. (2010). SUBTLEX-CH: Chinese word and character frequencies based on film subtitles. PloS One, 5(6), e10729. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0010729CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coltheart, M., Davelaar, E., Jonasson, J. T., & Besner, D. (1977). Access to the internal lexicon. In Dornic, S. (Ed.), Attention and performance VI (pp. 535555). Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Crepaldi, D., Rastle, K., Coltheart, M., & Nickels, L. (2010). ‘Fell’primes ‘fall’, but does ‘bell’prime ‘ball’? Masked priming with irregularly-inflected primes. Journal of Memory and Language, 63(1), 8399. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2010.03.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crepaldi, D., Rastle, K., Davis, C. J., & Lupker, S. J. (2013). Seeing stems everywhere: position-independent identification of stem morphemes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39(2), 510525. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029713Google ScholarPubMed
Davis, C. J. (2003). Factors underlying masked priming effects in competitive network models of visual word recognition. In Kinoshita, S. & Lupker, S. J. (Eds.), Masked priming: The state of the art (pp. 121170). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Davis, C. J., & Lupker, S. J. (2006). Masked inhibitory priming in English: Evidence for lexical inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(3), 668687. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.32.3.668Google ScholarPubMed
Davis, C. J., & Taft, M. (2005). More words in the neighborhood: Interference in lexical decision due to deletion neighbors. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(5), 904910. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196784CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, C. J. (2010). The spatial coding model of visual word identification. Psychological Review, 117(3), 713758. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019738.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diependaele, K., Sandra, D., & Grainger, J. (2009). Semantic transparency and masked morphological priming: The case of prefixed words. Memory & Cognition, 37(6), 895908. https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.37.6.895CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duñabeitia, J. A., Perea, M., & Carreiras, M. (2009). There is no claim with coats in the calm coast: Delimiting the transposed-letter priming effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(10), 19301947. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210802696070CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forster, K. I., & Davis, C. (1984). Repetition priming and frequency attenuation in lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10(4), 680698. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.10.4.680Google Scholar
Forster, K. I., Davis, C., Schoknecht, C., & Carter, R. (1987). Masked priming with graphemically related forms: Repetition or partial activation? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 39(2), 211251. https://doi.org/10.1080/14640748708401785CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forster, K. I., & Veres, C. (1998). The prime lexicality effect: Form priming as a function of prime awareness, lexical status, and discrimination difficulty. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24(2), 498514. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.24.2.498Google ScholarPubMed
Gao, F., Wang, J., Zhao, C. G., & Yuan, Z. (2022). Word or morpheme? Investigating the representation units of L1 and L2 Chinese compound words in mental lexicon using a repetition priming paradigm. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 25(7), 23822396. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2021.1913984CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grainger, J., & Jacobs, A. M. (1996). Orthographic processing in visual word recognition: A multiple read-out model. Psychological Review, 103(3), 518565. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.103.3.518CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gu, J., Li, X., & Liversedge, S. P. (2015). Character order processing in Chinese reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41(1), 127137. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038639Google ScholarPubMed
Heathcote, L., Nation, K., Castles, A., & Beyersmann, E. (2018). Do ‘blacheap’ and ‘subcheap’ both prime ‘cheap’? An investigation of morphemic status and position in early visual word processing. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71(8), 16451654. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2017.1362704CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hino, Y., & Lupker, S. J. (1998). The effects of word frequency for Japanese kana and kanji words in naming and lexical decision: Can the dual-route model save the lexical-selection account? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(5), 14311453. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.24.5.1431Google Scholar
Hino, Y., Miyamura, S., & Lupker, S. J. (2011). The nature of orthographic-phonological and orthographic-semantic relationships for Japanese Kana and Kanji words. Behavior Research Methods, 43(4), 11101151. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-011-0101-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huang, H. W., Lee, C. Y., Tsai, J. L., Lee, C. L., Hung, D. L., & Tzeng, O. J. (2006). Orthographic neighborhood effects in reading Chinese two-character words. Neuroreport, 17(10), 10611065. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.wnr.0000224761.77206.1dCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, R. L., & Eisler, M. E. (2012). The importance of the first and last letter in words during sentence reading. Acta Psychologica, 141(3), 336351. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.09.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jordan, T. R., Thomas, S. M., Patching, G. R., & Scott-Brown, K. C. (2003). Assessing the importance of letter pairs in initial, exterior, and interior positions in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(5), 883893. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.29.5.883Google ScholarPubMed
Kawakami, M. (2002). Kanji niji-jukugo no ruijigo suu to kousei moji no syutsugen hindo ga goihandan kadai ni oyobosu kouka [Effects of neighborhood size and character frequency on lexical decisions to two-character Kanji compound words]. Japanese Journal of Psychology, 73, 346351. https://doi.org/10.4992/jjpsy.73.346Google Scholar
Levy, R. (2014) Using R formulae to test for main effects in the presence of higher-order interactions. Preprint, arXiv:1405.2094.Google Scholar
Lo, S., & Andrews, S. (2015). To transform or not to transform: using generalized linear mixed models to analyse reaction time data. Frontiers in Psychology, 30(6), 1171. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01171Google Scholar
McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Pt. 1. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88(5), 375407. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.88.5.375CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morey, R. D., Rouder, J. N., & Jamil, T. (2015). BayesFactor: Computation of Bayes factors for common designs. R Package Version 0.9, 9, 2014. Retrieved from http://cran.r-project.org/package=%0ABayesFactor.Google Scholar
Nakayama, M., Sears, C. R., Hino, Y., & Lupker, S. J. (2014). Do masked orthographic neighbor primes facilitate or inhibit the processing of Kanji compound words? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(2), 813840. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035112Google ScholarPubMed
Nakayama, M., Sears, C. R., & Lupker, S. J. (2008). Masked priming with orthographic neighbors: A test of the lexical competition assumption. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34(5), 12361260. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.34.5.1236Google Scholar
Nakayama, M., Sears, C. R., & Lupker, S. J. (2011). Lexical competition in a non-Roman, syllabic script: An inhibitory neighbour priming effect in Japanese Katakana. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26(8), 11361160. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2010.491251CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peng, D., Li, Y., & Liu, Z. (1994). Identification of the Chinese two-character word under repetition priming condition. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 26(04), 393400.Google Scholar
Peressotti, F., & Grainger, J. (1999). The role of letter identity and letter position in orthographic priming. Perception & Psychophysics, 61(4), 691706. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03205539CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team. (2015). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna. Retrieved from https://cran.r-project.org/doc/manuals/r-release/R-ints.html.Google Scholar
Rastle, K., Davis, M. H., & New, B. (2004). The broth in my brother’s brothel: Morpho-orthographic segmentation in visual word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(6), 10901098. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196742CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, L. (2020). emmeans: Estimated Marginal Means, aka Least-Squares Means. R package version 1.5.0. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=emmeans.Google Scholar
Scaltritti, M., & Balota, D. A. (2013). Are all letters really processed equally and in parallel? Further evidence of a robust first letter advantage. Acta Psychologica, 144(2), 397410. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.07.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime: User’s guide. Psychology Software Tools.Google Scholar
Segui, J., & Grainger, J. (1990). Priming word recognition with orthographic neighbors: Effects of relative prime-target frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(1), 6576. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.16.1.65Google ScholarPubMed
Shoolman, N., & Andrews, A. (2003). Racehorses, reindeer, and Sparrows: Using masked priming to investigate morphological influences on com-pound word identification. In Kinoshita, S. & Lupker, S. J. (Eds.), Masked priming: The state of the art (pp. 241278). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Singmann, H., & Kellen, D. (2019). An Introduction to mixed models for experimental psychology. In Spieler, D.H. & Schumacher, E. (Eds.), New methods in neuroscience and cognitive psychology (pp. 139). Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780429318405-2Google Scholar
Stinchcombe, E. J., Lupker, S. J., & Davis, C. J. (2012). Transposed-letter priming effects with masked subset primes: A re-examination of the “relative position priming constraint”. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27(4), 475499. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2010.550928CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taft, M., & Nguyen-Hoan, M. (2010). A sticky stick? The locus of morphological representation in the lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25(2), 277296. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960903043261CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsai, J. L., Lee, C. Y., Lin, Y. C., Tzeng, O. J. L., & Hung, D. L. (2006). Neighborhood size effects of Chinese words in lexical decision and reading. Language and Linguistics, 7(3), 659675.Google Scholar
Tsang, Y. K., & Chen, H. C. (2014). Activation of morphemic meanings in processing opaque words. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(5), 12811286. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0589-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tse, C. S., Yap, M. J., Chan, Y.-L., Sze, W. P., Shaoul, C., & Lin, D. (2017). The Chinese lexicon project: A megastudy of lexical decision performance for 25,000+ traditional Chinese two-character compound words. Behavior Research Methods, 49(4), 15031519. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-016-0810-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Heuven, W. J., Dijkstra, T., Grainger, J., & Schriefers, H. (2001). Shared neighborhood effects in masked orthographic priming. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8(1), 96101. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196144CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitney, C. (2001). How the brain encodes the order of letters in a printed word: The SERIOL model and selective literature review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8(2), 221243. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196158CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wu, Y., Duan, R., Zhao, S., & Tsang, Y. K. (2020). Processing ambiguous morphemes in Chinese compound word recognition: behavioral and ERP evidence. Neuroscience, 446, 249260. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2020.08.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, H., Hino, Y., Chen, J., Yoshihara, M., Nakayama, M., Xue, J., & Lupker, S. J. (2020). The origins of backward priming effects in logographic scripts for four-character words. Journal of Memory and Language, 113, 104107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2020.104107CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, H., Taikh, A., & Lupker, S. J. (2022). A reexamination of the impact of morphology on transposed character priming effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 48(6), 785797. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001119Google ScholarPubMed
Zhou, X., & Marslen-Wilson, W. (2000). The relative time course of semantic and phonological activation in reading Chinese. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(5), 12451265. https://doi.org/10.1O37//O278-7393.26.5.1245Google ScholarPubMed
Zhou, X., Marslen-Wilson, W., Taft, M., & Shu, H. (1999). Morphology, orthography, and phonology reading Chinese compound words. Language and Cognitive Processes, 14(5–6), 525565. https://doi.org/10.1080/016909699386185CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Mean lexical decision latencies (RTs, in milliseconds) and percentage error rate for words in experiment 1

Figure 1

Table 2. Mean lexical decision latencies (RTs, in milliseconds) and percentage error rate for words in experiment 2

Figure 2

Table 3. Mean lexical decision latencies (RTs, in milliseconds) and percentage error rate for words in experiment 3