We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
Margaret Winters & Geoffrey Nathan, Cognitive Linguistics for Linguists, Springer International Publishing, 2020. Pp. xi + 81. ISBN 978-3-030-33603-5 (paperback), 978-3-030-33604-2 (E-book).
Published online by Cambridge University Press:
03 April 2020
An abstract is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)
References
REFERENCES
Achard, M. (2007). Complementation. In Geeraerts, D. & Cuyckens, H. (eds), The Oxford handbook of cognitive linguistics (pp. 782–802). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Brugman, C. (1981). Story of over. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Linguistics Club.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1967 [1959]). A review of B. F. Skinner’s Verbal behavior. In Jakobovits, L. & Miron, M. (eds), Readings in the psychology of language (pp. 142–143). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Deane, P. (1992). Grammar in mind and brain: explorations in cognitive syntax (Cognitive Linguistics Research). Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fauconnier, G. & Turner, M. (2002). The way we think: conceptual blending and the mind’s hidden complexity. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Fillmore, C. (1988). The mechanisms of ‘Construction Grammar’. In Axmaker, S., Jaisser, A. & Singmaster, H. (eds), Proceedings of the Fourteenth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society (pp. 35–55). Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society.Google Scholar
Fillmore, C., Kay, P. & O’Connor, M. C. (1988). Regularity and idiomaticity in grammatical constructions: the case of let alone. Language64, 501–538.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geeraerts, D. (1988). Cognitive grammar and the history of lexical semantics. In Rudzka-Ostyn, B. (ed.), Topics in cognitive linguistics (pp. 647–677). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geeraerts, D. & Cuyckens, H. (2010). The Oxford handbook of Cognitive Linguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, A. (1995). Constructions: a construction grammar approach to argument structure. Chicago, IL & London: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. (1977). Linguistic gestalts. Proceedings of the Chicago Linguistic Society13, 236–287.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. & Thompson, H. (1975). Introducing cognitive grammar. Proceedings of the First Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society295–313.Google Scholar
Langacker, R. (1978). The form and meaning of the English auxiliary. Language54, 853–882.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langacker, R. (1982). Space grammar, analyzability and the English passive. Language58, 22–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langacker, R. (1984). Active zones. Proceedings of the Berkeley Linguistics Society172–188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langacker, R. (1987). Foundations of Cognitive Grammar. Volume 1: theoretical prerequisites. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Lindner, C. (1982). What goes up doesn’t necessarily come down: the ins and outs of opposites. Proceedings of the Chicago Linguistic Society18, 305–323.Google Scholar
Rosch, E. (1975). Cognitive representations of semantic categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General104, 192–233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
vanHoek, K. (1997). Anaphora and conceptual structure. Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Winters, M. (1989). Diachronic prototype theory: on the evolution of the French subjunctive. Linguistics27, 703–730.CrossRefGoogle Scholar