Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T22:13:16.390Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Enhanced linguistic prosodic skills in musically trained individuals with Williams syndrome

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 August 2019

PASTORA MARTÍNEZ-CASTILLA*
Affiliation:
Department of Developmental and Educational Psychology, Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia (UNED), Madrid, Spain
RUTH CAMPOS
Affiliation:
Department of Basic Psychology, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid (UAM), Madrid, Spain
MARÍA SOTILLO
Affiliation:
Department of Basic Psychology, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid (UAM), Madrid, Spain
*
Address for correspondence: Pastora Martínez-Castilla, Department of Developmental and Educational Psychology, Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia (UNED), C/ Juan del Rosal, n° 10, 28040, Madrid, Spain. tel: (+34) 913989695; e-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Individuals with Williams syndrome (WS) present prosodic impairments. They are also interested in musical activities. In typical development, a body of research has shown that the linguistic prosodic skills of musically trained individuals are enhanced. However, it is not known whether, in WS, musical training is also associated with enhanced prosodic performance, a question this study sought to answer. We compared the performance on linguistic prosodic tasks among seven musically trained and fourteen musically untrained individuals with WS, and typically developing peers. Among those with WS, musically trained participants outperformed their musically untrained counterparts on the perception of acoustic parameters involved in prosody, the understanding of questioning and declarative intonation, and the comprehension of prefinal contrastive stress. The results suggest that musical training facilitates prosodic performance in WS. Our findings also suggest common processing mechanisms for acoustic parameters involved in both prosody and music, and that positive music-to-language transfer effects could take place in WS. We discuss the implications of these results for intervention purposes.

Type
Article
Copyright
Copyright © UK Cognitive Linguistics Association 2019 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

This research was funded by grant AP2003-5098 from the Ministry of Education and Science of the Spanish Government. The manuscript was proofread thanks to funds from the Department of Developmental and Educational Psychology (UNED). We thank all the participants who collaborated in this research.

References

references

Abraham, W. (2002). Human language and music: critical components. Interdisciplinary Journal for Germanic Linguistics and Semiotic Analysis 7(1), 6783.Google Scholar
Alexander, J. A., Wong, P. C. M. & Bradlow, A. R. (2005). Lexical tone perception in musicians and non-musicians. Proceedings of the 9th European Conference on Speech Communication and Technology, Lisbon, Portugal, 48 September. Online <http://faculty.wcas.northwestern.edu/ann-bradlow/publications/2005/Alexander-Wong-Bradlow-2005.pdf>.Google Scholar
Bellugi, U., Lichtenberger, L., Jones, W., Lai, Z. & George, M. (2000). The neurocognitive profile of Williams syndrome: a complex pattern of strengths and weaknesses. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 12, 729.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bellugi, U., Marks, S., Bihrle, A. & Sabo, H. (1988). Dissociation between language and cognitive function in Williams syndrome. In Bishop, D. & Mogford, K. (eds.), Language development in exceptional circumstances (pp. 177189). Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone.Google Scholar
Besson, M., Chobert, J. & Marie, C. (2011). Transfer of training between music and speech: common processing, attention, and memory. Frontiers in Psychology 2. doi=10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00094CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bidelman, G. M., Gandour, J. T. & Krishnan, A. (2010). Cross-domain effects of music and language experience on the representation of pitch in the human auditory brainstem. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 23, 425434.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Catterall, C., Howard, S., Stojanovik, V., Szczerbinski, M. & Wells, B. (2006). Investigating prosodic ability in Williams Syndrome. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics 20(7/8), 531538.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chobert, J., François, C., Velay, J.-L. & Besson, M. (2014). Twelve months of active musical training in 8- to 10-year-old children enhances the preattentive processing of syllabic duration and voice onset time. Cerebral Cortex 24, 956967.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chobert, J., Marie, C., François, C., Schön, D. & Besson, M. (2011). Enhanced passive and active processing of syllables in musician children. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 23, 38743887.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deruelle, C., Schön, D., Rondan, C. & Mancini, J. (2005). Global and local music perception in children with Williams syndrome. Neuroreport 16, 631634.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Don, A. J., Schellenberg, E. G. & Rourke, B. P. (1999). Music and language skills of children with Williams syndrome. Child Neuropsychology 5, 154170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunning, B. A., Martens, M. A. & Jungers, M. K. (2015). Music lessons are associated with increased verbal memory in individuals with Williams syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities 36, 565578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dykens, E. M., Rosner, B. A., Ly, T. & Sagun, J. (2005). Music and anxiety in Williams syndrome: a harmonious or discordant relationship? American Journal on Mental Retardation 110, 346358.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elsabbagh, M., Cohen, H. & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2010). Discovering structure in auditory input: evidence from Williams syndrome. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities 115, 128139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ewart, A. K., Morris, C. A., Atkinson, D. J., Jin, W. S., Sternes, K., Spallone, P., … Keating, M. T. (1993). Hemizygosity at the elastin locus in a developmental disorder, Williams syndrome. Nature Genetics 5, 1116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fidler, D. J., Lawson, J. E. & Hodapp, R. M. (2003). What do parents want? An analysis of education-related comments made by parents of children with different genetic syndromes. Journal of Intellectual and Developmental Disability 28, 196204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, A. (2000). Prosodic features and prosodic structure. the phonology of suprasegmentals. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Glaser, S. (2000). The missing link: connections between musical and linguistic prosody. Contemporary Music Review 19(3), 131154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ito, K. & Martens, M. A. (2017). Contrast-marking prosodic emphasis in Williams syndrome: results of detailed phonetic analysis. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 52(1), 4658.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kolinsky, R., Cuvelier, H., Goetry, V., Peretz, I. & Morais, J. (2009). Music training facilitates lexical stress processing. Music Perception 26, 235246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kraus, N. & Chandrasekaran, B. (2010). Music training for the development of auditory skills. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11, 599605.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lenhoff, H. M., Perales, O. & Hickok, G. (2001). Absolute pitch in Williams syndrome. Music Perception 18, 491503.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lense, M. & Dykens, E. (2013). Musical learning in children and adults with Williams syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research 57, 850860.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lense, M. D., Shivers, C. M. & Dykens, E. M. (2013). (A)musicality in Williams syndrome: examining relationships among auditory perception, musical skill, and emotional responsiveness to music. Frontiers in Psychology 4, 113. doi=10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00525CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levitin, D. J. & Bellugi, U. (1998). Musical abilities in individuals with Williams syndrome. Music Perception 15, 357389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levitin, D. J., Cole, K., Chiles, M., Lai, Z., Lincoln, A. & Bellugi, U. (2004). Characterizing the musical phenotype in individuals with Williams syndrome. Child Neuropsychology 10, 223247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lima, C. F. & Castro, S. L. (2011). Speaking to the trained ear: musical expertise enhances the recognition of emotions in speech prosody. Emotion 11, 10211031.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Magne, C., Schön, D. & Besson, M. (2006). Musician children detect pitch violations in both music and language better than non musician children. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18, 199211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marie, C., Delogu, F., Lampis, G., Belardinelli, M. O. & Besson, M. (2011). Influence of musical expertise on segmental and tonal processing in Mandarin Chinese. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 23, 27012715.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marie, C., Magne, C. & Besson, M. (2011). Musicians and the metric structure of words. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 23, 294305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marques, C., Moreno, S., Castro, S. L. & Besson, M. (2007). Musicians detect pitch violation in a foreign language better than non-musicians: behavioural and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 19, 14531463.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martens, M. A., Jungers, M. K. & Steele, A. L. (2011). Effect of musical experience on verbal memory in Williams syndrome: evidence from a novel word learning task. Neuropsychologia 49, 30933102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martínez-Castilla, P. & Peppé, S. (2008). Developing a test of prosodic ability for speakers of Iberian Spanish. Speech Communication 50, 900915.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martínez-Castilla, P., Rodríguez, M. & Campos, R. (2016). Developmental trajectories of pitch-related music skills in children with Williams syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities 51/52, 2339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martínez-Castilla, P. & Sotillo, M. (2008). Singing abilities in Williams syndrome. Music Perception 25, 449469.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martínez-Castilla, P. & Sotillo, M. (2014). Pitch processing in children with Williams syndrome: relationships between music and prosody skills. Brain Sciences 4, 376395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martínez-Castilla, P., Sotillo, M. & Campos, R. (2011). Prosodic abilities of Spanish-speaking adolescents and adults with Williams syndrome. Language and Cognitive Processes 26, 10551082.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martínez-Castilla, P., Sotillo, M. & Campos, R. (2013). Do individuals with Williams syndrome possess absolute pitch? Child Neuropsychology 19, 7896.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martínez-Castilla, P., Stojanovik, V., Setter, J. & Sotillo, M. (2012). Prosodic abilities in Spanish and English children with Williams syndrome: a cross-linguistic study. Applied Psycholinguistics 33, 122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mervis, C. B. & Becerra, A. M. (2007). Language and communicative development in Williams syndrome. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews 13, 315.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mervis, C. B. & John, A. E. (2010). Cognitive and behavioral characteristics of children with Williams syndrome: implications for intervention approaches. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part C: Seminar in Medical Genetics 154C(2), 229248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moreno, S., Marques, C., Santos, A., Santos, M., Castro, S. L. & Besson, M. (2009). Musical training influences linguistic abilities in 8-year-old children: more evidence for brain plasticity. Cerebral Cortex 19, 712723.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nazzi, T., Paterson, S. J. & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2003). Word segmentation by infants with Williams syndrome. Infancy 4, 251271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norton, A., Winner, E., Cronin, K., Over, K., Lee, D. J. & Schlaug, G. (2005). Are there pre-existing neural, cognitive, or motoric markers for musical ability? Brain and Cognition 59, 124134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palmer, C., Jungers, M. K. & Jusczyk, P. W. (2001). Episodic memory for musical prosody. Journal of Memory and Language 45, 526545.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pardo, A. & Ruiz, M. (2002). SPSS 11. Guía para el análisis de datos. [SPSS11. Guide for data analyses]. Mexico D.F., Mexico: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Patel, A. D. (2008). Music, language, and the brain . New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Patel, A. D. & Iversen, J. R. (2007). The linguistic benefits of musical abilities. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11, 369372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peppé, S. (2009). Why is prosody in speech–language pathology so difficult? International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 11, 258271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peretz, I. & Coltheart, M. (2003). Modularity of music processing. Nature Neuroscience 6, 688691.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phillips, C. E., Jarrold, C., Baddeley, A. D., Grant, J. & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2004). Comprehension of spatial language terms in Williams syndrome: evidence for an interaction between domains of strength and weakness. Cortex 40, 85101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinker, S. (1991). Rules of language. Science 286, 23552358.Google Scholar
Plesa-Skwerer, D., Schofield, C., Verbalis, A., Faja, S. & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2007). Receptive prosody in adolescents and adults with Williams syndrome. Language and Cognitive Processes 22, 247271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Porter, M. A. & Coltheart, M. (2005). Cognitive heterogeneity in Williams syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychology 27, 275306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reilly, J., Klima, E. S. & Bellugi, U. (1990). Once more with feeling: affect and language in atypical populations. Development and Psychopathology 2, 367391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sadakata, M. & Sekiayama, K. (2011). Enhanced perception of various linguistic features by musicians: a cross-linguistic study. Acta Psychologica 138, 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schellenberg, G. (2004). Music lessons enhance IQ. Psychological Science 15, 511514.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schellenberg, G. (2008). Commentary on ‘Effects of Early Musical Experience on Auditory Sequence Memory’ by Adam Tierney, Tonya Bergeson & David Pisoni. Empirical Musicology Review 3, 205207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schön, D., Magne, C. & Besson, M. (2004). The music of speech: music training facilitates pitch processing in both music and language. Psychophysiology 41, 341349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stambaugh, L. (1996). Special learners with special abilities. Music Educators Journal 83(3), 1923.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stojanovik, V. (2010).Understanding and production of prosody in children with Williams syndrome: a developmental trajectory approach. Journal of Neurolinguistics 23,112126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stojanovik, V. (2012). Later language. In Farran, E. K. & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (eds.), Neurodevelopmental disorders across the lifespan (pp. 205221). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stojanovik, V., Setter, J. & van Ewijk, L. (2007). Intonation abilities of children with Williams syndrome: a preliminary investigation. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 50, 16061617.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stojanovik, V., Zimmerer, V., Setter, J., Hudson, K., Poyraz-Bilgin, I. & Saddy, D. (2018). Artificial grammar learning in Williams syndrome and in typical development: the role of rules, familiarity, and prosodic cues. Applied Psycholinguistics 39, 327353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strait, D. L. & Kraus, N. (2011). Playing music for a smarter ear: cognitive, perceptual and neurobiological evidence. Music Perception 29, 133146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strømme, P., Bjørnstad, P. G. & Ramstad, K. (2002). Prevalence estimation of Williams syndrome. Journal of Child Neurology 17, 269271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tabachnick, B. G. & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson.Google Scholar
Thompson, W. F., Schellenberg, E. G. & Husain, G. (2004). Decoding speech prosody: Do music lessons help? Emotion 4, 4664.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trimmer, C. G. & Cuddy, L. L. (2008). Emotional intelligence, not music training, predicts recognition of emotional speech prosody. Emotion 8, 838849.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Herwegen, J. & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2015). Genetic developmental disorders and numerical competence across the lifespan. In Cohen Kadosh, R. & Dowker, A. (eds.), The Oxford handbook of numerical cognition (pp. 721731). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (2001). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Adults, III Edition-Spanish. Madrid: TEA.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (2005). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, IV Edition-Spanish. Madrid: TEA.Google Scholar
Wong, P. C. M., Skoe, E., Russo, N. M., Dees, T. & Kraus, N. (2007). Musical experience shapes human brainstem encoding of linguistic pitch patterns. Nature Neuroscience 10, 420422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zubizarreta, M. L. (1998). Prosody, focus, and word order. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Martínez-Castilla et al. supplementary material

Martínez-Castilla et al. supplementary material 1

Download Martínez-Castilla et al. supplementary material(File)
File 174 Bytes
Supplementary material: File

Martínez-Castilla et al. supplementary material

Martínez-Castilla et al. supplementary material 2

Download Martínez-Castilla et al. supplementary material(File)
File 20.1 KB
Supplementary material: File

Martínez-Castilla et al. supplementary material

Martínez-Castilla et al. supplementary material 3

Download Martínez-Castilla et al. supplementary material(File)
File 22.4 KB
Supplementary material: File

Martínez-Castilla et al. supplementary material

Martínez-Castilla et al. supplementary material 4

Download Martínez-Castilla et al. supplementary material(File)
File 174 Bytes