Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-hc48f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T16:49:40.398Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The enactment of language: Decades of interactions between linguistic and motor processes

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 March 2014

Sarah E. Anderson*
Affiliation:
Cornell University University of California, Merced
Michael J. Spivey
Affiliation:
Cornell University University of California, Merced
*
Correspondence addresses: Sarah Anderson, Psychology Department, Uris Hall, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY 14853, USA. E-mail: [email protected].

Abstract

Many long-standing predominant theories of cognition have viewed higher levels of processing, such as language and cognition, as free from the influence of lower levels of processing, such as action and perception. However, many recent experiments have found evidence that the delineation between traditional modules is not so clean or precise, with motor output and language comprehension interacting much more fluidly than traditional theories predict. Evidence for this account includes findings of systematic activation of motor cortex while processing action words, as well as functional consequences of language on action and of action on language. It is worth noting that this recent spate of interest in the embodiment of cognition is not without historical precedence. Here, we will review such evidence coming from previous decades of research on the interaction between language and action, in addition to exploring the empirical results of these more recent experiments and methodologies. It seems to be the case, as evidenced from research in cognitive neuroscience and modeling, that motor representations and actions can influence language processing in predictable ways. Not only is there continuous competition between simultaneously active representations in language processing, with multiple sources of information interacting immediately, this competition is apparent in the motor output produced as a response to language.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © UK Cognitive Linguistics Association 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abrams, R. A. & Balota, D. A.. 1991. Mental chronometry: Beyond reaction time. Psychological Science 2. 153157.Google Scholar
Ahmed, A. & Ruffman, T.. 1998. Why do infants make A not B errors in a search task, yet show memory for location of hidden objects in nonsearch task? Developmental Psychology 34. 441453.Google Scholar
Allopenna, P.Magnuson, D. J. S. & Tanenhaus, M. K.. 1998. Tracking the time course of spoken word recognition: Evidence for continuous mapping models. Journal of Memory and Language 38. 419439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balota, A. D. & Abrams, R. A.. 1995. Mental chronometry: Beyond onset latencies in the lexical decision task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 21. 12891302.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L.W. 1999a. Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22. 577660.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L.W. 1999b. Language comprehension: Archival memory or preparation for situated action? Discourse Processes 28. 6180.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. W., Barbey, A. K., Simmons, W. K. & Wilson, C. D.. 2003. Grounding conceptual knowledge in modality-specific systems. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7. 8491.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L.W., Solomon, K. O. & Wu, L. L.. 1999. Perceptual simulation in conceptual tasks. In Hiraga, M. K., Sinha, C. & Wilcox, S. (eds.), Cultural, typological, and psychological perspectives in Cognitive Linguistics: The proceedings of the 4th conference of the International Cognitive Linguistics Association, vol. 3, 209228. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Bertenthal, B. I. 1996. Origins and early development of perception, action, and representation. Annual Review of Psychology 47. 431459.Google Scholar
Bongard, J., Lipson, H. & Zykov, V.. 2006. Resilient machines through continuous self-modeling. Science 314. 11181121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boulenger, V., Deprez, V., Jeannerod, M., Nazir, T. A., Paulignan, Y. & Roy, A. C.. 2006. Cross-talk between language processes and overt motor behavior in the first 200 msec of processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18. 16071615.Google Scholar
Brooks, R. 1999. Cambrian intelligence: The early history of the new AI. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., Fadiga, L., Fink, G. R., Fogassi, L., Freund, H. J., Gallese, V., Rizzolatti, G., Seitz, R. J. & Zilles, K.. 2001. Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: An fMRI study. European Journal of Neuroscience 13. 400404.Google Scholar
Caciopoo, J. T., Priester, J. P. & Bernston, G. G.. 1993. Rudimentary determination of attitudes: II. Arm flexion and extension have differential effects on attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 65. 517.Google Scholar
Calvo-Merino, B., Glaser, J., Grezes, D. E., Haggard, P. & Passingham, R. E.. 2006. Seeing or doing? Influence of visual and motor familiarity in action observation. Current Biology 16. 19051910.Google Scholar
Casasanto, D. & Lozano, . 2007. Embodied language production: Evidence from gesture, speech, disfluency, and motor action. Paper presented at Embodied Sentence Processing Conference: Behavioral, Neuropsychological, and Computational Perspectives, Saarbrücken, Germany.Google Scholar
Chambers, C., Magnuson, J. & Tanenhaus, M.. 2004. Actions and affordances in syntactic ambiguity resolution. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 30. 687696.Google Scholar
Chen, S. & Bargh, J. A.. 1999. Consequences of automatic evaluation: Immediate behavior predispositions to approach or avoid the stimulus. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 25. 215224.Google Scholar
Cisek, C. 2006. Integrated neural processes for defining potential actions and deciding between them: A computational model. The Journal of Neuroscience 26. 97619770.Google Scholar
Cisek, P. & Kalaska, J. F.. 2005. Neural correlates of reaching decisions in dorsal premotor cortex: Specification of multiple direction choices and final selection of action. Neuron 45. 801814.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, A. 1997. Being there: Putting brain, body, and world together again. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dietrich, E. & Markman, A.. 2003. Discrete thoughts: Why cognition must use discrete representations. Mind and Language 18. 95119.Google Scholar
Dijkstra, K., Kaschak, M. P. & Zwaan, R. A.. 2006. Body posture facilitates retrieval of autobiographical memories. Cognition 102. 139149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duncker, K. 1945. On problem solving. Psychological Monographs 58–5, whole no. 270).Google Scholar
Eberhard, K., Sedivy, J., Spivey-Knowlton, M. & Tanenhaus, M.. 1995. Eye movements as a window into real-time spoken language comprehension in natural contexts. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 24. 409436.Google Scholar
Ellis, R. & Tucker, M.. 2000. Micro-affordances: The potentiation of components of action by seen objects. British Journal of Psychology 91. 451471.Google Scholar
Elman, J. & McClelland, J.. 1988. Cognitive penetration of the mechanisms of perception: Compensation for coarticulation of lexically restored phonemes. Journal of Memory and Cognition 27. 143165.Google Scholar
Fischer, M. H. & Zwaan, R. A.. 2008. Embodied language: A review of the role of the motor system in language comprehension. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 61. 825850.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. 1975. The language of thought. New York: Crowell.Google Scholar
Frazier, L. & Clifton, C.. 1996. Construal. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Frazier, L. & Fodor, J.. 1978. The sausage machine: A new two-stage parsing model. Cognition 6. 291325.Google Scholar
Gibson, J. 1979. The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin.Google Scholar
Glenberg, A. 1997. What memory is for. Behavioral and Brain Science 20. 155.Google Scholar
Glenberg, A. M. & Kashak, M. P.. 2002. Grounding language in action. Psychonomics Bulletin and Review 9. 558565.Google Scholar
Glucksberg, S. 1964. Functional fixedness: Problem solution as a function of observing responses. Psychonomic Science 1. 117118.Google Scholar
Gold, J. & Shadlen, M.. 2000. Representation of a perceptual decision in developing oculomotor commands. Nature 404. 390394.Google Scholar
Grant, E. & Spivey, M.. 2003. Eye movements and problems solving: Guiding attention guides thought. Psychological Science 14. 462466.Google Scholar
Greenwald, A. 1970. A choice reaction time test of ideomotor theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology 86. 2025.Google Scholar
Hauk, O., Johnsrude, I. & Pulvermuller, F.. 2004. Somatotopic representation of action words in the motor and premotor cortex. Neuron 41. 301307.Google Scholar
Hauk, O. & Pulvermuller, F.. 2004. Neurophysiological distinction of action words in frontocentral cortex. Human Brain Mappings 21. 191201.Google Scholar
Hebb, D. 1968. Concerning imagery. Psychological Review 75. 466477.Google Scholar
Houghton, F. & Tipper, S. P.. 1996. Inhibitory mechanisms of neural and cognitive control: Application to selective attention and sequential action. Brain and Cognition 30. 2043.Google Scholar
Kaden, S. E., Wapner, S. & Werner, H.. 1955. Studies in physiognomic perception: II. Effect of directional dynamics of pictured object and words on the position of the apparent horizon. Journal of Psychology 39. 6170.Google Scholar
Klatzky, R. L., Lederman, S. J., McCloskey, B. P. & Pellegrino, J.. 1993. Cognitive representations of functional interactions with objects. Memory and Cognition 21. 294303.Google Scholar
Marlsen-Wilson, M. 1987. Functional parallelism in spoken word recognition. Cognition 25. 71102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markman, A. & Brendl, C.. 2005. Constraining theories of embodied cognition. Psychological Science 16. 610.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matlock, T. 2004. Fictive motion as cognitive simulation. Memory and Cognition 32, 13891400.Google Scholar
Meegan, D. & Tipper, S. P.. 1998. Reaching into cluttered visual environments: Spatial and temporal influences of distracting objects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 51A. 225249.Google Scholar
Nazir, T. A., Boulenger, V., Jeannerod, M., Paulignan, Y., Roy, A., Silber, B.. 2007. Language-induced motor perturbations during the execution of a reaching movement. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18, 16071615.Google Scholar
Neisser, U. 1967. Cognitive Psychology. East Norwalk, CT: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Neisser, U. 1976. Cognition and reality: Principles and implications of Cognitive Psychology. San Francisco, CA: W.H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Newell, A., Shaw, J. C. & Simon, H. A.. 1958. Elements of a theory of human problem solving. Psychological Review 65. 151166.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. 1952. The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International University Press.Google Scholar
Pulvermuller, F. 1999. Words in the brain's language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22. 253279.Google Scholar
Pulvermuller, F. 2001. Brain reflections of words and their meanings. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 5. 517524.Google Scholar
Pulvermuller, F., Hauk, O., Ilmoniemi, R. J. & Nikulin, V. V.. 2005. Functional links between motor and language systems. European Journal of Neuroscience 21. 793797.Google Scholar
Pylyshyn, Z. 1984. Computation and cognition: Toward a foundation for Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Richardson, D. C. & Matlock, T.. 2007. The integration of figurative language and static depictions: An eye movement study of fictive motion. Cognition 102. 129138.Google Scholar
Riskind, J. H. 1983. Nonverbal expressions and the accessibility of life experience memories: A congruence hypothesis. Social Cognition 2. 6286.Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L. & Gallese, V.. 1996. Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Cognitive Brain Research 3. 131141.Google Scholar
Smith, L. B., McLin, D., Thelen, E. & Titzer, R.. 1999. Knowing in the context of acting: The task dynamics of the A not-B error. Psychological Review 106. 235260.Google Scholar
Snedeker, J. & Trueswell, J.. 2004. The developing constraints on parsing decisions: The role of lexical-biases and referential scenes in child and adult sentence processing. Cognitive Psychology 49. 238299.Google Scholar
Spivey, M. & Geng, J.. 2001. Oculomotor mechanisms activated by imagery and memory: Eye movements to absent objects. Psychological Research 65. 235241.Google Scholar
Spivey, M. J., Grosjean, M. & Knoblich, G.. 2005. Continuous attraction toward phonological competitors. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 102. 1039310398.Google Scholar
Spivey, M., Eberhard, K., Sedivy, J., Tanenhaus, M.. 2002. Eye movements and spoken language comprehension: Effects of visual context on syntactic ambiguity resolution. Cognitive Psychology 45. 447481.Google Scholar
Stevens, J., Decety, J., Fonlupt, P. & Shiffrar, M.. 2000. New aspects of motion perception: Selective neural encoding of apparent human movements. Neuroreport: An International Journal of Rapid Communication of Neuroscience Research 11. 109115.Google Scholar
Tanenhaus, M., Eberhard, K., Sedivy, J. & Spivey-Knowlton, M.. 1995. Integration of visual and linguistic information during spoken language comprehension. Science 268. 16321634.Google Scholar
Tanenhaus, M. & Trueswell, J.. 1995. Sentence comprehension. In Miller, J. and Eimas, P. (eds.), Handbook of cognition and perception. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Tettamanti, M., Buccino, G., Cappa, S., Danna, M., Fazio, F., Gallese, V., Perani, D., Rizzolatti, G., Saccuman, M. & Scifo, P.. 2005. Listening to action-related sentences activates fronto-parietal motor circuits. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 17. 273281.Google Scholar
Thelen, E., Scheier, C., G., , Schoener, & Smith, L.. 2001. The dynamics of embodiment: A field theory of infant perseverative reaching. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24. 186.Google Scholar
Thelen, E. & Smith, L. B. (eds.). 1994. A dynamical systems approach to development and cognition. Bradford Books & MIT Press.Google Scholar
Thomas, L. & Lleras, A.. 2007. Moving eyes and moving thought: On the spatial compatibility between eye movements and cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 14. 663668.Google Scholar
Tipper, S. P., Houghton, G., Howard, L. A.. 2000. Behavioural consequences of selection from neural population codes. In Monsell, S. and Driver, J. (eds.), Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance XVIII, 233245. Cambridge, MA: MIT.Google Scholar
Trueswell, J., Kello, C. & Tanenhaus, M.. 1993. Verb-specific constraints in sentence processing: Separating effects of lexical preference from garden-paths. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 19. 528553.Google Scholar
Trueswell, J., Sekerina, K., Hill, N. & Logrip, L.. 1999. The kindergarten-path effect: Studying on-line sentence processing in young children. Cognition 73. 89134.Google Scholar
Tucker, M. & Ellis, R.. 1998. On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 24. 830846.Google Scholar
Zwaan, R. A. 2004. The immersed experiencer: Toward an embodied theory of language comprehension. In Ross, B. H. (ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation 44, 3562. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Zwaan, R. A. & Taylor, L. J.. 2006. Seeing, acting, understanding: motor resonance in language comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 135. 111.Google Scholar
Zwitserlood, P. 1989. The locus of the effects of sentential-semantic context in spoken-word processing. Cognition 32. 25.Google Scholar