Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-g8jcs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T20:55:47.908Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

When gesture does and does not promote learning

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 March 2014

Susan Goldin-Meadow*
Affiliation:
University of Chicago
*
Correspondence addresses: Susan Goldin-Meadow, Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, 5730 South Woodlawn Avenue, Chicago, IL 60637, USA. E-mail: [email protected].

Abstract

Speakers move their hands when they talk—they gesture. These gestures can signal whether the speaker is ready to learn a particular task and, in this sense, provide a window onto the speaker's knowledge. But gesture can do more than reflect knowledge. It can play a role in changing knowledge in at least two ways: indirectly through its effects on communication with the learner, and directly through its effects on the learner's cognition. Gesturing is, however, not limited to learners. Speakers who are proficient in a task also gesture. Their gestures have a different relation to speech than the gestures that novices produce, and seem to support cognition rather than change it. Gesturing can thus serve as a tool for thinking and for learning.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © UK Cognitive Linguistics Association 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alibali, M. W. & Goldin-Meadow, S.. 1993. Transitions in learning: What the hands reveal about a child's state of mind. Cognitive Psychology 25. 468523.Google Scholar
Alibali, M. W., Flevares, L. & Goldin-Meadow, S.. 1997. Assessing knowledge conveyed in gesture: Do teachers have the upper hand? Journal of Educational Psychology 89. 183193.Google Scholar
Beattie, G. & Shovelton, H.. 1999. Mapping the range of information contained in the iconic hand gestures that accompany spontaneous speech. Journal of Language and Social Psychology 18(4). 438462.Google Scholar
Beilock, S. L. & Goldin-Meadow, S.. Under Review. Gesture grounds thought in action.Google Scholar
Bertenthal, B. 1999. Variation and selection in the development of perception and action. In Savelsbergh, G. (ed.), Non-linear analyses of developmental processes, 105121. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers.Google Scholar
Broaders, S. C., Cook, S. W., Mitchell, Z. & Goldin-Meadow, S.. 2007. Making children gesture brings out implicit knowledge and leads to learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 136. 539550.Google Scholar
Capirci, O., Iverson, J. M., Pizzuto, E. & Volterra, V.. 1996. Gestures and words during the transition to two-word speech. Journal of Child Language 23(3). 645673.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cassell, J., McNeill, D. & McCullough, K.-E.. 1999. Speech-gesture mismatches: Evidence for one underlying representation of linguistic and nonlinguistic information. Pragmatics and Cognition 7(1). 134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Church, R. B., Ayman-Nolley, S. & Mahootian, S.. 2004. The effects of gestural instruction on bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism 7(4). 303319.Google Scholar
Church, R. B. & Goldin-Meadow, S.. 1986. The mismatch between gesture and speech as an index of transitional knowledge. Cognition 23(1). 4371.Google Scholar
Cook, S. W. & Goldin-Meadow, S.. 2006. The role of gesture in learning: Do children use their hands to change their minds? Journal of Cognition and Development 7(2). 211232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cook, S. W. & Goldin-Meadow, S.. Under Review. Gesture leads to knowledge change by creating implicit knowledge.Google Scholar
Cook, S. W., Mitchell, Z. & Goldin-Meadow, S.. 2008. Gesturing makes learning last. Cognition 106. 10471058.Google Scholar
Cook, S. W. & Tanenhaus, M. K.. 2009. Embodied communication: Speakers' gestures afect listeners' actions. Cognition 113. 98104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ehrlich, S. B., Levine, S. C. & Goldin-Meadow, S.. 2006. The importance of gesture in children's spatial reasoning. Developmental Psychology 42. 12591268.Google Scholar
Feyereisen, P. & de Lannoy, J.-D.. 1991. Gestures and speech: Psychological investigations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Furuyama, N. 2000. Gestural interaction between the instructor and the learner in origami instruction. In McNeill, D. (ed.), Language and gesture, 99117. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Garber, P. & Goldin-Meadow, S.. 2002. Gesture ofers insight into problem-solving in adults and children. Cognitive Science 26. 817831.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. 2003. Hearing gesture: How our hands help us think. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. & Beilock, S. L.. 2010. Action's influence on thought: The case of gesture. Perspectives on Psychological Science, in press.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., Alibali, M. W. & Church, R. B.. 1993. Transitions in concept acquisition: Using the hand to read the mind. Psychological Review 100(2). 279297.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. & Butcher, C.. 2003. Pointing toward two-word speech in young children. In Kita, S. (ed.), Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet, 85107. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., Cook, S. W. & Mitchell, Z.. 2009. Gesturing gives children new ideas about math. Psychological Science 20(3). 267272.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., Kim, S. & Singer, M.. 1999. What the teachers' hands tell the students' minds about math. Journal of Educational Psychology 91. 720730.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., Nusbaum, H., Kelly, S. D. & Wagner, S.. 2001. Explaining math: Gesturing lightens the load. Psychological Science 12. 516522.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. & Sandhofer, C.. 1999. Gestures convey substantive information about a child's thoughts to ordinary listeners. Developmental Science 2(1). 6774.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. & Singer, M. A.. 2003. From children's hands to adults' ears: Gesture's role in the learning process. Developmental Psychology 39. 509520.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., Wein, D. & Chang, C.. 1992. Assessing knowledge through gesture: Using children's hands to read their minds. Cognition and Instruction 9. 201219.Google Scholar
Hostetter, A. B. & Alibali, M. W.. 2008. Visible embodiment: Gestures as simulated action. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 15. 495514.Google Scholar
Iverson, J. M. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (eds.). 1998a. The nature and functions of gesture in children's communications (New Directions for Child Development 79). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Iverson, J. M. & Goldin-Meadow, S.. 1998b. Why people gesture as they speak. Nature 396. 228.Google Scholar
Iverson, J. M. & Goldin-Meadow, S.. 2005. Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological Science 16. 368371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iverson, J. M., Capirci, O., Volterra, V. & Goldin-Meadow, S.. 2008. Learning to talk in a gesture-rich world: Early communication of Italian vs. American children. First Language 28. 164181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelly, S. D. & Church, R. B.. 1997. Can children detect conceptual information conveyed through other children's nonverbal behaviors? Cognition and Instruction 15. 107134.Google Scholar
Kelly, S. D. & Church, R. B.. 1998. A comparison between children's and adults' ability to detect conceptual information conveyed through representational gestures. Child Development 69. 8593.Google Scholar
Kendon, A. 1980. Gesticulation and speech: Two aspects of the process of utterance. In Key, M. R. (ed.), Relationship of verbal and nonverbal communication, 207228. The Hague: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Kimbara, I. 2006. On gestural mimicry. Gesture 6. 3961.Google Scholar
Kita, S. 2000. How representational gestures help speaking. In McNeill, D. (ed.), Language and gesture: Window into thought and action, 162185. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Krauss, R. M., Chen, Y. & Gottesman, R. F.. 2000. Lexical gestures and lexical access: A process model. In McNeill, D. (ed.), Language and gesture, 261283. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D. 1992. Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Morsella, E. & Krauss, R. M.. 2004. The role of gestures in spatial working memory and speech. American Journal of Psychology 117. 411424.Google Scholar
Newell, A. & Simon, H. A.. 1972. Human problem solving. Englewood Clifs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Özçalıąkan, Ş. & Goldin-Meadow, S.. 2005. Gesture is at the cutting edge of early language development. Cognition 96(3). B101B113.Google Scholar
Özçalıąkan, Ş. & Goldin-Meadow, S.. 2009. When gesture-speech combinations do and do not index linguistic change. Language and Cognitive Processes 28. 190217.Google Scholar
Perry, M., Berch, D. B. & Singleton, J. L.. 1995. Constructing shared understanding: The role of nonverbal input in learning contexts. Journal of Contemporary Legal Issues Spring(6). 213236.Google Scholar
Perry, M., Church, R. B. & Goldin-Meadow, S.. 1988. Transitional knowledge in the acquisition of concepts. Cognitive Development 3(4). 359400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perry, M. & Elder, A. D.. 1997. Knowledge in transition: Adults' developing understanding of a principle of physical causality. Cognitive Development 12. 131157.Google Scholar
Pine, K. J., Lufkin, N. & Messer, D.. 2004. More gestures than answers: Children learning about balance. Developmental Psychology 40. 10591067.Google Scholar
Ping, R. & Goldin-Meadow, S.. 2008. Hands in the air: Using ungrounded iconic gestures to teach children conservation of quantity. Developmental Psychology 44. 12771287.Google Scholar
Ping, R. & Goldin-Meadow, S.. Under Review. Gesturing saves cognitive resources when talking about non-present objects. Cognitive Science, in press.Google Scholar
Riseborough, M. G. 1981. Physiographic gestures as decoding facilitators – 3 experiments exploring a neglected facet of communication. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 5(3). 172183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruiter, J. P. de. 1998. Gesture and speech production. Nijmegen, The Netherlands: Katholieke Universiteit Nijmegen doctoral dissertation.Google Scholar
Siegler, R. S. 1994. Cognitive variability: A key to understanding cognitive development. Current Directions in Psychological Science 3. 15.Google Scholar
Singer, M. A. & Goldin-Meadow, S.. 2005. Children learn when their teacher's gestures and speech difer. Psychological Science 16(2). 8589.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thelen, E. 1989. Self-organization in developmental processes: Can systems approaches work? In Gunnar, M. & Thelen, E. (eds.), Systems and development: The Minnesota Symposium on Child Psychology, 77117. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Valenzeno, L., Alibali, M. W. & Klatzky, R.. 2003. Teachers' gestures facilitate students' learning: A lesson in symmetry. Contemporary Educational Psychology 28. 187204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, S. M., Nusbaum, H. & Goldin-Meadow, S.. 2004. Probing the mental representation of gesture: Is handwaving spatial? Journal of Memory and Language 50. 395407.Google Scholar
Wesp, R., Hess, J., Keutmann, D. & Wheaton, K.. 2001. Gestures maintain spatial imagery. The American Journal of Psychology 114. 591600.Google Scholar