Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-v9fdk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-20T01:21:47.342Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Memory reflected in our decisions: Higher working memory capacity predicts greater bias in risky choice

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2023

Jonathan Corbin*
Affiliation:
Appalachian State University
Todd McElroy
Affiliation:
Appalachian State University
Cassie Black
Affiliation:
Appalachian State University
*
*Address for Corbin or McElroy: Department of Psychology, Appalachian State University, Boone, NC 28608. Email: [email protected].
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The current study looks at the role working memory plays in risky-choice framing. Eighty-six participants took the Automatic OSPAN, a measurement of working memory; this was followed by a risky-choice framing task. Participants with high working memory capacities demonstrated well pronounced framing effects, while those with low working memory capacities did not. This pattern suggests that, in a typical risky-choice decision task, elaborative encoding of task information by those with high working memory capacity may lead them to a more biased decision compared to those with low working memory.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
The authors license this article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License.
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors [2010] This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

1 Introduction

Throughout our lives, we are faced with many small and large decisions that exist within a variety of contexts. Many of these choices involve some aspect of risk, like choosing whether to invest in certain stocks, deciding on medical treatment options, or even deciding whether to risk human lives. The most studied examples of risk and decision making involve risky choices presented within a positive or negative framework, or risky-choice framing effects.

Risky-choice framing effects are derived from Prospect Theory predictions (Kahneman & Tversky, Reference Kahneman and Tversky1979) and have become one of the most studied examples of rational decision making. According to Prospect Theory the presentation of an outcome as either a loss or gain affects the amount of risk a person is willing to accept. This effect is attributed to differences in perceived subjective value and is captured by the value function, which is concave for gains, yielding risk-aversive preferences, and convex for losses, yielding risk-seeking preferences.

In the most widely tested example, Tversky and Kahneman (Reference Tversky and Kahneman1981) presented participants with an Asian disease problem wherein 600 lives were at stake. Participants were then presented with a set of alternatives, one risk-free and the other risky. The alternatives were framed either positively or negatively, in terms of people that would be “saved” or “die”. Their findings revealed that, despite identical expected values, most participants preferred the risk-free alternative when the problem was framed positively and the risky alternative when framed negatively. This type of task design is commonly referred to as “risky-choice” (Levin, Schneider, & Gaeth, Reference Levin, Schneider and Gaeth1998).

1.1 Working memory and framing

There has been considerable research and theoretical interest in how working memory capacity (WMC) may influence decision making. Multiple studies have shown that WMC can predict differences in strategy use when encoding information (Cokely, Kelley, & Gilchrist, Reference Cokely, Kelley and Gilchrist2006; McNamara & Scott, Reference McNamara and Scott2001; Turley-Ames & Whitfield, Reference Turley-Ames and Whitfield2003) as well as number and strength of alternate hypotheses one can generate when making probability judgments (Dougherty & Hunter, Reference Dougherty and Hunter2003). Those with high WMC appear to employ elaborative encoding techniques while those with low WMC tend to rely on rote-rehearsal (Turley-Ames & Whitfield, Reference Turley-Ames and Whitfield2003). The difference in encoding technique generally leads to superior decision making for those with high WMC (Cokely & Kelley, Reference Cokely and Kelley2009). The difference may distinguish high and low WMC with regard to traditional risky-choice framing tasks.

One dual-process model that focuses on memory is fuzzy-trace theory (Reyna & Brainerd, Reference Reyna and Brainerd1989; Reyna & Brainerd, Reference Reyna and Brainerd1991; Reyna & Brainerd, Reference Reyna and Brainerd1995). Fuzzy-trace theory states that framing effects occur because people are focusing on the gist of information rather than verbatim information. Gist information consists of people preferring something over nothing in the gains condition and nothing over something in the loss condition as compared to verbatim information which consists of the exact numerical quantities in the framing problem (Reyna & Brainerd, Reference Reyna and Brainerd1991). Recent research by Kuhberger and Tanner (Reference Kuhberger and Tanner2009) has shown multiple replications of framing predictions made by fuzzy-trace theory. Work with framing effects in children has discovered that younger children do not exhibit framing effects, due to the fact that they focus on verbatim information. As children age, framing effects begin to appear (Reyna & Ellis, Reference Reyna and Ellis1994; Reyna & Farley, Reference Reyna and Farley2006). This corresponds to the finding that, as children age, their WMC increases (Dempster, Reference Dempster1981). Accordingly, people with high WMC may encoding more elaborative representations that enable reasoning based on gist (e.g., encoding a richer context), while people with low WMC may primarily focus on salient and easily available numerical quantities (verbatim). The prediction that can be made from this theoretical approach is that high WMC participants should show framing effects while low WMC participants should show little or no framing effects (e.g., Reyna & Brainerd, Reference Reyna and Brainerd1991).

Stanovich and West (Reference Stanovich and West2000) have suggested that non-normative reasoning would increase in tasks where working memory is vital or when it becomes overloaded. In later research, Cokely and Kelley (Reference Cokely and Kelley2009) conducted a well-designed study in which participants were presented with a series of 40 gain/loss choice problems. This study was designed to test predictions for expected-value choices and elaborative heuristic search. Both the gain problems (e.g., “gain $150” or “5% chance of gaining $2000”) and loss problems (e.g., “lose $50” or “5% chance to lose $4000”) were presented in a numerical format. People with a higher WMC made more choices that coincided with expected value compared to those with low WMC. However, higher WMC was not associated with expected value calculations. Instead, participants with higher WMC engaged in more thorough and elaborative heuristic based decision making.

While this methodology was appropriate for testing their hypotheses, it differs substantially from the typical risky-choice design (Levin, Schneider, & Gaeth, Reference Levin, Schneider and Gaeth1998) represented by the Asian disease problem (Table 1). One crucial difference between designs is the equality of expected value between alternatives. In the Asian disease problem, the alternatives have equal expected values whereas in the Cokely and Kelley (Reference Cokely and Kelley2009) problems, the alternatives had unequal expected values. Another difference lies in their presentational format. Cokely and Kelley noted that a large amount of deliberation and contextualization of problems led to expected-value choices. However, the numerical format used differs substantially from the context of the Asian disease problem. In fact, prior research has shown that presentation of a decision task in the context of money shows a weaker framing effect compared to the context of human lives (Fagley & Miller, Reference Fagley and Miller1997). Therefore, given the differences between the Cokely and Kelley, method and a typical risky-choice design, alternative predictions can be made for how working memory may interact with a decision task such as the Asian disease problem.

Table 1: The Asian disease problem framed as gains and losses.

1.2 Predictions

The current experiment investigates how WMC interacts with the framing of a decision problem using Tversky and Kahneman’s (Reference Tversky and Kahneman1981) Asian disease problem. Two competing hypotheses make distinctly different predictions for how working memory may influence decisions in a risky-choice scenario. First, one could expect the results to mirror those of Cokely and Kelley (Reference Cokely and Kelley2009), in that people with high WMC are better able to process the probabilities within the task, leading to decreased framing effects as compared to those with low WMC. Alternatively, research by Delaney & Sahakyan (Reference Delaney and Sahakyan2007) indicated that people with high WMC are more context dependent than those with low WMC. Since risky-choice decision tasks such as the Asian disease problem are more contextually developed, a second hypothesis is that those with high WMC may be more affected by the contextual information within the scenario (the frame) than those with low WMC, leading to larger framing effects in those with high WMC. The latter hypothesis is also consistent with theoretical predictions derived from fuzzy-trace theory (Reyna & Brainerd, Reference Reyna and Brainerd1991), and with the inference from developmental increases in both WMC and framing effects.

2 Methods

2.1 Participants

Eighty-six undergraduates in Appalachian State University’s research pool participated in exchange for course credit. One participant was excluded from the data set due to procedural problems, leaving the number of participants at eighty-five.

2.2 Materials

The Turner and Engle (Reference Turner and Engle1989) OSPAN task requires participants to verify the truth of math operations while trying to remember a set of unrelated letters. This study uses an automated version of the Turner and Engle OSPAN task, developed by Unsworth, Heitz, Schrock and Engle (Reference Unsworth, Heitz, Schrock and Engle2005). For each trial, the participants were presented with a math problem and asked to determine the truth of the math problem (i.e. 2/1 + 6 = 7). Immediately after the participants judged the truth of the math problem, they were presented with a letter to remember. The operation-letter pairings were presented in sets of two to seven items. Following each complete set, the participants were instructed to recall the letters in the order presented. In order to ensure that participants were not trading off between solving the operations and remembering the letters, an 85% accuracy criterion on the math operations was required. The participants received several sets of practice trials before beginning the task. For all of the span measures, items were scored for accuracy in specific item recall (i.e., correct letter) as well as the correct position within the serial order of presentation. Therefore the total score for the OSPAN represents total number of correct items as well as the correct ordinal position.

2.3 Procedure

Participants entered the research lab, signed informed consent, and were asked to complete the OSPAN on the computer. When they finished the task, they were given the Asian disease problem (see Table 1) and asked to rate their decision on a 7-point scale ranging from 1 (Definitely would recommend A) to 7 (Definitely would recommend B).

3 Results

First, an independent samples t-test was run in order to determine whether or not there was a significant impact of the frame on decisions. This analysis shows a significant framing effect t(82) = –2.67, p = .009, the level of risk-seeking was higher in the loss condition (M = 4.28) than the gain condition (M = 3.33). In order to look at the effect of working memory on framing, participants’ OSPAN scores were entered into a general linear model as a covariate, with frame as the independent variable and their decision as the dependent variable. The interaction between frame and OSPAN score was significant F(1, 84)= 5.35, p = .023, while the addition of the OSPAN score to the model rendered the framing effect non-significant F(1, 84) = 1.336, n.s.Footnote 1

Another way of looking at the data is to explore participants in the high and low ends of the sample. To do so, we constructed a model, calculating high and low working memory scores by adding and subtracting a standard deviation from the OSPAN mean (M = 40.18, high = 56.72, low = 23.66). Each value was entered into a regression equation to predict one’s decision on the Asian disease problem based on WMC. Surprisingly, for those with high WMC, the model predicted large framing effects (Gains = 3.06, Losses = 4.8) whereas a reversal was predicted for those with low WMC (Gains = 4.81, Losses = 3.67) (see Figure 1).

Figure 1: Modeled decision for high and low WMC participants (calculated as 1 SD from the mean) in the Asian Disease problem.

4 Discussion

This experiment explored the impact of working memory capacity on the choices individuals make in a risky-choice scenario. The results support the hypothesis that those who have high WMC are more sensitive to the frame within the risky-choice scenario, and therefore show framing effects, while the framing effect is absent among those with low WMC.

Our findings may provide insight for dual-processing approaches that have been widely embraced in the decision making literature (e.g., Epstein, Pacini, Denes-Raj, & Heier, Reference Epstein, Pacini, Denes-Raj and Heier1996; Evans & Over, Reference Evans, St and Over1996; Kahneman & Frederick, Reference Kahneman, Frederick, Gilovich, Griffin and Kahneman2002; Sloman, Reference Sloman1996; Stanovich, Reference Stanovich1999). The dual-process accounts generally posit that individuals construe information in two different ways. Consideration of working memory as a variable resource, which can determine cognitive ability for making rational choices, may help clarify future research in this area (Cokely & Kelley, Reference Cokely and Kelley2009).

Similarly, some dual-process approaches have relied on effort and importance as determining factors for the two processing styles. These findings tend to show that effort, as motivated through importance, will influence the likelihood of biases and decision making fallacies such as the framing effect (e.g., Biswas, Reference Biswas2009; Igou & Bless, Reference Igou and Bless2007; Meyers-Leny & Maheswaran, Reference Meyers-Leny and Maheswaran2004; McElroy & Seta, Reference McElroy and Seta2003). Future research in this area should consider how effort may influence an individual’s ability to perform decision making tasks in light of working memory. It may be the case that importance and effort can both facilitate improvement and overload working memory, depending upon personal and situational factors.

In related work, Stanovich and West (Reference Stanovich and West1998, 1999, 2000) have explored whether rational choice may be tied to intelligence. Recently, Stanovich and West (Reference Stanovich and West2008) were surprised to find that there was a small interaction between SAT and framing, where those with higher scores displayed slightly larger framing effects (opposite to their predictions). Since working memory has been shown to have a moderate correlation with scores on IQ tests (Luciano, et al., Reference Luciano, Wright, Smith, Geffen, Geffen and Martin2001), the association between framing effects and higher cognitive ability is consistent with our findings. Further, Stanovich (Reference Stanovich2008) suggests an individual difference variable, the Master Rationality Motive (MRM), as a means for understanding rational choice. This approach suggests that individuals vary in how much they seek rational integration of information. This motive is seen as the impetus for searching across preferences, ending in rational integration. As Stanovich points out, the MRM is to be differentiated from cognitive ability or intelligence. As Stanovich points out, the MRM can be differentiated from cognitive ability and intelligence. However, the current data suggest that cognitive abilities, such as working memory capacity, can in some cases predict MRM activities.

A number of individual difference factors have been shown to attenuate the framing effect and are likely to rely on working memory. For example, need-for-cognition, which reflects the extent that people engage in effortful thought and how much they enjoy it, has been shown to influence the strength of the framing effect (e.g., Chatterjee, Heath, Milberg & France, Reference Chatterjee, Heath, Milberg and France2000; Simon, Fagley & Halleran, Reference Simon, Fagley and Halleran2004; Smith & Levin, Reference Smith and Levin1996; Zhang & Buda, Reference Zhang and Buda1999). Numeracy, a skill variable that tests individuals’ abilities to do statistical and probabilistic reasoning tasks seems a likely candidate for working memory. Similar to need-for-cognition, numeracy has also been shown to be a predictor of how prone individuals are to biases and decision making fallacies (Peters & Levin, Reference Peters and Levin2008; Peters et al., Reference Peters, Västfjäll, Slovic, Mertz, Mazzocco and Dickert2006).

One question that deserves further investigation is how our findings may be reconciled with those of Cokely and Kelley (Reference Cokely and Kelley2009). It is clear that the two studies rely on different methodologies, both involving a type of framing, yet producing different findings. Differentiating specific factors that may be leading to the different findings is beyond the scope of this paper. However, a couple of factors seem to be reasonable starting points.

First, it would seem prudent to examine whether the equality of expected values among the alternatives interacts with WMC and framing. Results from Fulginiti and Reyna (Reference Fulginiti and Reyna1993) show that, when the expected values for alternatives in the Asian disease problem are slightly unequal (in a direction opposite to the typical framing preference), framing effects diminish. It may be the case that when equal expected values are presented, as in the Asian disease problem, this allows high WMC participants to “cancel out” that part of the alternative comparison and consequently, they may be more influenced by the frame. Conversely, when unequal expected values are involved, this may lead high WMC participants to focus more on that part of the alternative comparison and less on the frame. As a result, framing effects may be less robust for high WMC participants.

Another distinction is the format of the decision task; numeric or verbal. Specifically, it may be that when the decision task is presented in a numerical format, high WMC participants are focused on the numeric information whereas low WMC participants are unable to process this part of the task as efficiently and focus more on the frame. Such processing differences should lead low WMC participants to be more influenced by the frame when the task involves numerically based information. Future research investigating working memory and framing should consider these factors as potential mechanisms for determining the strength and likelihood of the framing effect.

In sum, we have examined the differences that exist in risky-choice decision making between those with higher verses lower cognitive ability, as measured through WMC. Our results have shown that higher cognitive abilities do not always reflect rational decision making. Rather than acting in accordance with normative reasoning, the results indicate that higher ability individuals may rely on more thorough encoding and decision making processes (Baron, Reference Baron1985; Cokely & Kelley, Reference Cokely and Kelley2009). Specifically, individuals higher in WMC may rely on gist based memory representations due to differential encoding, leading them toward a more biased decision in the case of traditional risky choice framing problems. This possibility provides an alternative approach to traditional dual process theories of rationality that focuses on quantitative differences in memory rather than qualitative differences in reasoning in order to predict performance.

Footnotes

*

Thanks to James Mills for help in collecting the data, and to the reviewers for helpful comments.

1 The data are included with the article in this issue of the journal.

References

Baron, J. (1985). Rationality and intelligence. New York, NY US: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Biswas, D. (2009). The effects of option framing on consumer choices: Making decisions in rational versus experiential processing modes. Journal of Consumer Behavior, 8, 284299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chatterjee, S., Heath, T.B., Milberg, S.J., & France, K. R. (2000). The differential processing of price in gains and losses: The effects of frame and need for cognition. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 13, 6175.3.0.CO;2-J>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cokely, E. T., & Kelley, C. M. (2009). Cognitive abilities and superior decision making under risk: A protocol analysis and process model evaluation. Judgment and Decision Making, 4, 2033.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cokely, E., Kelley, C., & Gilchrist, A. (2006). Sources of individual differences in working memory: Contributions of strategy to capacity. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 991997.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delaney, P., & Sahakyan, L. (2007). Unexpected costs of high working memory capacity following directed forgetting and contextual change manipulations. Memory and Cognition, 35, 10741082.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dempster, F. N. (1981). Memory span: sources of individual and developmental differences. Psychological Bulletin, 89, 63100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dougherty, M., & Hunter, J. (2003). Hypothesis generation, probability judgment, and individual differences in working memory capacity. Acta Psychologica, 113, 263282.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epstein, S., Pacini, R., Denes-Raj, V., & Heier, H. (1996). Individual differences in intuitive experiential and analytical-rational thinking styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 390 — 405.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, J. St, B. T., & Over, D. E. (1996). Rationality and reasoning. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Fagley, N., & Miller, P. (1997). Framing effects and arenas of choice: Your money or your life?. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 71, 355373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fulginiti, J. V. & Reyna, V. F.. (1993, November). Qualitative versus quantitative processing in decision making: A fuzzy-trace theory analysis. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Society for Judgment and Decision Making, Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Igou, E. R., & Bless, H. (2007). On undesirable consequences of thinking: Framing effects as a function of substantive processing. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 20, 125142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D. & Frederick, S. (2002). Representativeness revisited: Attribute substitution in intuitive judgment. pp. 4981 in Gilovich, T., Griffin, D., and Kahneman, D. [eds]. Heuristics & Biases: The Psychology of Intuitive Judgment. New York. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47, 263291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhberger, A., & Tanner, C. (2009). Risky choice framing: Task versions and a comparison of prospect theory and fuzzy trace theory. Journal of behavioral decision making. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/bdm.656.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levin, I. P., Schneider, S.L., & Gaeth, G.J. (1998). All frames are not created equal: A typology and critical analysis of framing effects. Organizational Behavior & Human Decision Processes, 76, 149188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luciano, M., Wright, M. J., Smith, G. A., Geffen, G. M., Geffen, L. B. & Martin, N. G. (2001). Genetic covariance among measures of information processing speed, working memory and IQ. Behavior Genetics, 31, 581592.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McElroy, T., & Seta, J. J. (2003). Framing effects: An analytic-holistic perspective. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 610617.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNamara, D., & Scott, J. (2001). Working memory capacity and strategy use. Memory & Cognition, 29, 1017.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyers-Leny, J., & Maheswaran, D. (2004). Exploring message framing outcomes when systematic, heuristic or both types of processing occur. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14, 159167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peters, E., & Levin, I. P. (2008). Dissecting the risky-choice framing effect: Numeracy as an individual-difference factor in weighting risky and riskless options. Judgment and Decision Making, 3, 435448.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peters, E., Västfjäll, D., Slovic, P., Mertz, C., Mazzocco, K., & Dickert, S. (2006). Numeracy and decision making. Psychological Science, 17, 407413.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reyna, V. F. & Brainerd, C. J.. (1989). Fuzzy-trace theory of framing effects in choice. Presented at the 30th Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Atlanta, GA.Google Scholar
Reyna, V.F., & Brainerd, C.J. (1991). Furzy-trace theory and framing effects in choice: Gist extraction, truncation, and conversion. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 4, 249262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reyna, V., & Brainerd, C. (1995). Fuzzy-trace theory: An interim synthesis. Learning and Individual Differences, 7, 175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reyna, V. F., & Ellis, S. C. (1994). Fuzzy-trace theory and framing effects in children’s risky decision making. Psychological Science, 5, 275279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reyna, V. F., & Farley, F. (2006). Risk and rationality in adolescent decision making: Implications for theory, practice, and public policy. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 7, 144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simon, A. F., Fagley, N.S., & Halleran, J.G. (2004). Decision framing: Moderating effects of individual differences and cognitive processing. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 17, 7793.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sloman, S. A. (1996). The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, S. M., & Levin, I.P. (1996). Need for cognition and choice in framing effects. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 9, 283290.3.0.CO;2-7>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. (1999). Who is rational?: Studies of individual differences in reasoning. Mahwah, NJ US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (2008). Higher-order preferences and the master rationality motive. Thinking & Reasoning, 14, 111127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1998). Individual differences in rational thought. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127, 161188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1999). Discrepancies between normative and descriptive models of decision making and the understanding/acceptance principle. Cognitive Psychology, 38, 349385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2000). Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate? Behavioral and Brain Science, 23, 645726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2008). On the relative independence of thinking biases and cognitive ability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 672695.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turley-Ames, K., & Whitfield, M. M. (2003). Strategy training and working memory task performance. Journal of Memory & Language, 49, 446468.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turner, M. L., & Engle, R. W. (1989). Is working memory capacity task dependent? Journal of Memory & Language, 28, 127154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211, 453458.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Unsworth, N., Heitz, R. P., Schrock, J. C., & Engle, R. W. (2005). An automated version of the operation span task. Behavior Research Methods, 37, 498505.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, Y., & Buda, R. (1999). Moderating effects of need for cognition on responses to positively versus negatively framed advertising messages. Journal of Advertising, 28, 115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1: The Asian disease problem framed as gains and losses.

Figure 1

Figure 1: Modeled decision for high and low WMC participants (calculated as 1 SD from the mean) in the Asian Disease problem.

Supplementary material: File

Corbin et al. supplementary material

Corbin et al. supplementary material
Download Corbin et al. supplementary material(File)
File 1.5 KB