Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-767nl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-09T11:17:51.310Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effects of habitat, group-size, sex-age class and seasonal variation on the behavioural responses of the mountain nyala (Tragelaphus buxtoni) in Munessa, Ethiopia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 November 2013

Solomon A. Tadesse*
Affiliation:
Mitriani Department for Desert Ecology, Blaustein Institutes for Desert Research, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Midreshet Ben-Gurion 84990, Israel
Burt P. Kotler
Affiliation:
Mitriani Department for Desert Ecology, Blaustein Institutes for Desert Research, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Midreshet Ben-Gurion 84990, Israel
*
1Corresponding author. Email: [email protected]

Abstract:

Activity patterns of animals are generally influenced by many factors. We hypothesized that the behavioural responses (i.e. activity time-budget allocated to vigilance, feeding and moving) of mountain nyala (Tragelaphus buxtoni) should vary with habitat type, season, group-size and sex-age class. We randomly established a total of 12 permanent walking transects with the aid of a GPS device across three major habitat types used by the mountain nyala (i.e. four transects in each habitat). Following each transect, we conducted focal-animal observations to quantify the time-budget allocated to vigilance, feeding and moving. A total of 119 and 116 focal-animals were assessed in the wet and dry season respectively. Moreover, along each transect, seven habitat variables were collected in systematically laid 109 circular plots each with a 5-m radius (i.e. 31, 41 and 37 plots in the cleared vegetation, plantation and natural forest respectively) in the wet and dry season. We developed behavioural models by correlating the time-budget (i.e. proportion of time vigilance, feeding and moving) of the focal-animals in accordance with habitat variables, group-size and sex-age class. In the wet season, mountain nyala devoted most of their time to vigilance, but they allocated the largest proportion of their time to moving in the dry season. Vigilance differed among the three habitats and was highest in the cleared vegetation during the dry season. Contrary to expectations, adult males were more vigilant than both adult females and sub-adults during the dry season. The behavioural models based on time-budget help to predict how the mountain nyala perceive their environment and trade-off between food acquisition and safety in the wet and dry season. The study also improves our understanding of the adaptive behavioural ecology of the endangered mountain nyala.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

LITERATURE CITED

ABEBE, G. T. 2008. Ecology of regeneration and phenology of seven indigenous tree species in a dry tropical Afromontane forest, Southern Ethiopia. PhD Dissertation, Addis Ababa University, Addis Ababa, Ethiopia. 150 pp.Google Scholar
ALTMANN, J. 1974. Observation study of behaviour: sampling methods. Behaviour 49:227265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
ARENZ, C. L. & LEGER, D. W. 1999. Thirteen-lined ground squirrel (Sciuridae: Spermophilus tridecemlineatus) anti-predator vigilance decreases as vigilance cost increases. Animal Behaviour 57:97103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
BROWN, J. S. 1999. Vigilance, patch use, and habitat selection: foraging under predation risk. Journal of Evolution and Ecological Research 1:4971.Google Scholar
BROWN, J. S. & KOTLER, B. P. 2004. Hazardous duty pay and the foraging cost of predation. Journal of Ecology Letters 7:9991014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
BROWN, J. S., KOTLER, B. P. & BOUSKILA, A. 2001. Ecology of fear: foraging games between predators and prey with pulsed resources. Annales Zoologici Fennici 38:7187.Google Scholar
BROWN, L. 1969. Observation of the status, habitat, and behaviour of Mountain Nyala, Tragelaphus buxtoni in Ethiopia. Mammalia 33:545597.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
BURGER, J. & GOCHFELD, M. 1994. Vigilance in African mammals: differences amongst mothers, other females and males. Behaviour 131:153169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
CHILDRESS, M. J. & LUNG, M. A. 2003. Predation risk, gender and the group-size effect: does elk vigilance depend upon the behaviour of conspecifics? Animal Behaviour 66:389398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
CRESSWELL, W. 2008. Non-lethal effects of predation in birds. Ibis 150:317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DRUCE, D. J. 2005. Species requirements, coexistence and habitat partitioning at the community level: Rock Hyrax and Klipspringer. PhD thesis, University of KwaZulu-Natal, Durban, South Africa. 122 pp.Google Scholar
EVANGELISTA, P., SWARTZINSKI, P. & WALTERMIRE, R. 2007. A profile of the Mountain Nyala (Tragelaphus buxtoni). African Indaba 5:148.Google Scholar
EVANGELISTA, P., NORMAN, J., LAKEW, B., KUMAR, S. & ALLEY, N. 2008. Predicting habitat suitability for the endemic Mountain Nyala in Ethiopia. Journal of Wildlife Research 35:409416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
FRID, A. 1997. Vigilance by female Dall's sheep: interactions between predation risk factors. Animal Behaviour 53:799808.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
GEBREKIDAN, W. G. 1996. The status of the Mountain Nyala (Tragelaphus buxtoni) in Bale Mountains National Park 1986–1994. Walia 17:2737.Google Scholar
GILL, J. A., SUTHERLAND, W. J. & WATKINSON, A. R. 1996. A method to quantify the effects of human disturbance on animal populations. Journal of Applied Ecology 33:786792.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
HILLMAN, J. C. 1985. The Mountain Nyala. Acacia 6:3946.Google Scholar
HILLMAN, J. C. & HILLMAN, S. M. 1987. Mountain Nyala (Tragelaphus buxtoni) and Simien fox (Canis simensis) in the Bale Mountains National Park. Walia 10:36.Google Scholar
HOCHMAN, V. & KOTLER, B. P. 2006. Patch use, apprehension, and vigilance behaviour of Nubian Ibex under perceived risk of predation. Behavioural Ecology l8:368374.Google Scholar
HUNTER, T. B. & SKINNER, J. D. 1998. Vigilance behaviour in African ungulates: the role of predation pressure. Behaviour 135:195211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
KOTLER, B. P., GROSS, J. E. & MITCHELL, W. A. 1994. Applying patch use to assess aspects of foraging behaviour in Nubian Ibex. Journal of Wildlife Management 58:299307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LAGORY, K. E. 1986. Habitat, group size, and the behaviour of White-tailed Deer. Behaviour 98:168179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LAUNDRÉ, J. W., HERNANDEZ, L. & ALTENDORF, K. B. 2001. Wolves, Elk, and Bison: reestablishing the ‘‘landscape of fear’’ in Yellowstone National Park, USA. Canadian Journal of Zoology 79:14011409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LIMA, S. L. 1998. Non-lethal effects in the ecology of predator-prey interactions. BioScience 48:2534.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LIMA, S. L. & DILL, L. M. 1990. Behavioural decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus. Canadian Journal of Zoology 68:619640.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MAIN, M. B., WECKERLY, F. W. & BLEICH, V. C. 1996. Sexual segregation in ungulates: new directions for research. Journal of Mammalogy 77:449461.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MAMO, Y. 2007. Ecology and conservation of mountain nyala (Tragelaphus buxtoni) in Bale Mountains National Park, Ethiopia. PhD dissertation, University of Aberdeen, United Kingdom. 201 pp.Google Scholar
MANOR, R. & SALTZ, D. 2003. Impact of human nuisance disturbance on vigilance and group-size of a social ungulate. Ecological Applications 13:18301834.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MARTIN, P. & BATESON, P. 1993. Measuring behaviour: an introductory guide. (Second edition). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. 222 pp.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MCCAULEY, S. J., ROWE, L. & FORTIN, M. J. 2011. The deadly effects of “nonlethal” predators. Ecology 92:20432048.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MECH, L. D. 1977. Wolf-pack buffer zones as prey reservoirs. Science 198:320321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MORRIS, D. W., KOTLER, B. P., BROWN, J. S., SUNDARARAJ, V. & ALE, S. B. 2009. Behavioural indicators for conserving mammal diversity. Annals of the New York Academy of Science 1162:334356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MULLER, D. M., KOHLMANN, S. G. & ALKON, P. U. 1995. Nubian Ibex nursery: creche or natural trap. Israel Journal of Zoology 41:163174.Google Scholar
PRINS, H. H. T. 1996. Ecology and behaviour of the African Buffalo – social inequality and decision making. (First edition). Chapman and Hall, London. 320 pp.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
REFERA, B. & BEKELE, A. 2004. Population status and structure of Mountain Nyala in the Bale Mountains National Park, Ethiopia. African Journal of Ecology 42:17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
REID, C. 2005. Habitat suitability and behaviour of Springbok (Antidorcas marsupialis) at Augrabies National Park, South Africa. Masters Thesis, University of Port Elizabeth, Port Elizabeth, South Africa. 106 pp.Google Scholar
RIEUCAU, G. & MARTIN, J. G. A. 2008. Many eyes or many ewes: vigilance tactics in female bighorn sheep (Ovis canadensis) vary according to reproductive status. Oikos 117:501506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
RIPPLE, W. J. & BESCHTA, R. L. 2003. Wolf reintroduction, predation risk, and cottonwood recovery in Yellowstone National Park. Forest Ecology and Management 184:299313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
RIPPLE, W. J. & BESCHTA, R. L. 2004. Wolves and the ecology of fear: can predation risk structure ecosystems? Bioscience 54:755766.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
ROBERTS, G. 1996. Why individual vigilance declines as group-size increases. Animal Behaviour 51:10771086.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
ROSENZWEIG, M. L. 1974. On the optimal aboveground activity of banner-tail kangaroo rats. Journal of Mammalogy 55:193199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
RUCKSTUHL, K. E. 1998. Foraging behaviour and sexual segregation in bighorn sheep. Animal Behaviour 56:99106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
RUCKSTUHL, K. E. 2007. Sexual segregation in vertebrates: proximate and ultimate causes. Integrative and Comparative Biology 47:245257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
RUCKSTUHL, K. E. & KOKKO, H. 2002. Modeling sexual segregation in ungulates: effects of group size, activity budgets, and synchrony. Animal Behaviour 64:909914.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
SHETTLEWORTH, S. J., HAMPTON, R. R. & WESTWOOD, R. P. 1995. Effects of season and photoperiod on food storing by black-capped chickadees, Parus atricapillus. Animal Behaviour 49:989998.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
SIEGFRIED, W. R. 1980. Vigilance and group-size in Springbok. Madoqua 12:151154.Google Scholar
SIH, A. 1980. Optimal behaviour: can foragers balance two conflicting demands? Science 210:10411043.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
STANKOWICH, T. 2008. Ungulate flight responses to human disturbances: a review and meta-analysis. Biological Conservation 141:21592173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
STEPHENS, D. W. & KREBS, J. R. 1986. Foraging theory. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ. 262 pp.Google Scholar
SUKUMAR, R. & GADGIL, M. 1988. Male–female differences in foraging on crops by Asian Elephants. Animal Behaviour 36:12331235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
TADESSE, S. A. & KOTLER, B. P. 2011. The seasonal responses of habitat use by Nubian Ibex (Capra nubiana) evaluated with behavioural indictors. Israel Journal of Ecology and Evolution 57:223246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
TADESSE, S. A. & KOTLER, B. P. 2013. The impacts of humans and livestock encroachments on the habitats of mountain nyala (Tragelaphus buxtoni) in Munessa, Ethiopia. International Journal of Biodiversity and Conservation 5:560571.Google Scholar
TCHABOVSKY, A. V., KRASNOV, R. B., KHOKHLOVA, I. S. & SHENBROT, G. I. 2001. The effects of vegetation cover on vigilance and foraging tactics in the fat sand rat (Psammomys obesus). Journal of Ethology 19:105113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
TEKETAY, D. 1992. Human impact on a natural montane forest in south-eastern Ethiopia. Mountain Resources Development 12:393400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
TEKETAY, D. & GRANSTRÖM, A. 1995. Soil seed banks in dry afro-montane forests of Ethiopia. Journal of Vegetation Science 6:777786.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
TREVES, A. 2000. Theory and method in studies of vigilance and aggregation. Animal Behaviour 60:711722.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
YOUNG, T. P. & ISBELL, L. A. 1991. Sex differences in giraffe feeding ecology: energetic and social constraints. Journal of Ethology 87:7989.CrossRefGoogle Scholar