We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
An abstract is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)
References
Bertoncini, Josiane & Mehler, Jacques. 1981. Syllables as units in infant speech perception. In Strange, Winifred (ed.), Speech perception and linguistic experience, 171–206. Baltimore, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Hauser, Marc D., Chomsky, Noam & Fitch, W. Tecumseh. 2002. The faculty of language: What is it, who has it and how did it evolve?Science298, 1569–1579.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hornstein, Norbert. 2009. A theory of syntax: Minimal operations and Universal Grammar. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Jakobson, Roman. 1941. Kindersprache, Aphasie und allgemeine Lautgesetze. English edition 1968, Child language, aphasia and phonological universals (translated by Keiler, Allan). The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Jusczyk, Peter. 1997/2000. The discovery of spoken language. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Stampe, David. 1979. A dissertation on natural phonology. New York: Garland.Google Scholar
Thelen, Esther & Smith, Linda B.. 1994. A Dynamic Systems approach to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Werker, Janet & Tees, Richard. 1984. Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behaviour and Development7, 49–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar