Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T00:53:50.646Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The perception of word-initial consonant length: Pattani Malay*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 February 2009

Arthur S. Abramson
Affiliation:
Dept. of Linguistics, U-145, University of Connecticut, 341 Mansfield Rd., Rm. 230, Storrs, CT 06268, U.S.A., and Haskins Laboratories, New Haven, CT 06511-6695, U.S.A.

Extract

In experimental phonetic research, we often come across cases of multiple perceptual cues to a phonemic distinction, even though this distinction may be seen traditionally as dependent on some single phonetic feature. The question arises as to the relative power of these cues. How equally do they share the burden of communicative relevance? For example, among the several cues that emanate from the timing of the valvular action of the larynx (Lisker and Abramson (1965); Abramson (1977)), fundamental-frequency perturbations (House and Fairbanks (1953)) have been shown by some studies, apparently starting with Haggard, Ambler and Callow (1970) and Fujimura (1971), to help in the perceptual differentiation of voiced and voiceless stop consonants; however, recent work (Abramson and Lisker (1985)) suggests that this cue has very limited efficacy compared with other acoustic consequences of voice timing.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Journal of the International Phonetic Association 1986

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abramson, A.S. (1962). The Vowels and Tones of Standard Thai: Acoustical Measurements and Experiments. Bloomington: Indiana University Research Center in Anthropology, Folklore, and Linguistics.Google Scholar
Abramson, A.S. (1977). Laryngeal timing in consonant distinctions. Phonetica 34, 295303.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abramson, A.S. (1986). Distinctive length in initial consonants: Pattani Malay. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 79, S27 (abstract).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abramson, A.S. (1987). Word-initial consonant length in Pattani Malay. Proceedings of the XIth International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. Tallinn: Academy of Sciences of the Estonian S.S.R., 6870.Google Scholar
Abramson, A.S. and Lisker, L. (1985). Relative power of cues: F0 shift versus voice timing. In V., Fromkin (editor), Phonetic Linguistics: Essays in Honor of Peter Ladefoged. New York: Academic Press, 2533.Google Scholar
Chaiyanara, P. Masmintra. (1983). Dialek Melayu Patani dan Bahasa Malaysia: Satu Kajian Perbandingan dari segi Fonologi, Morfologi dan Syntaksis. Master's thesis, University of Malaya.Google Scholar
Fujimura, O. (1971). Remarks on stop consonants: Synthesis experiments and acoustic cues. In Hammerich, L.L., Jakobson, R. and Zwirner, E. (editors), Form and Substance: Phonetic and Linguistic Papers Presented to Eli Fischer-Jørgensen. Copenhagen: Akademisk Forlag, 221231.Google Scholar
Haggard, M.P., Ambler, S. and Callow, M. (1970). Pitch as a voicing cue. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 47, 613617.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
House, A.S. and Fairbanks, G. (1953). The influence of consonant environment upon the secondary acoustical characteristics of vowels. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 25, 105113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lehiste, I. (1970). Suprasegmentals. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lisker, L. and Abramson, A.S. (1965). Stop categorization and voice onset time. Proceedings of the Vth International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. Basel: S. Karger, 389391.Google Scholar