Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7czq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-30T23:25:47.585Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Changes in articulation accompanying functional changes in word usage

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 July 2010

Mária Gósy
Affiliation:
Department of Phonetics, Research Institute for Linguistics, Hungarian Academy of Sciences [email protected], [email protected]
Viktória Horváth
Affiliation:
Department of Phonetics, Research Institute for Linguistics, Hungarian Academy of Sciences [email protected], [email protected]

Abstract

Two words in present-day Hungarian, the conjunction tehát ‘that is’ and ‘consequently’ and the pronoun ilyen ‘like this’ seem to be undergoing a functional change, acquiring the function of fillers, while retaining their former lexical function, too. Twenty narratives were drawn from the Hungarian Spontaneous Speech Corpus (BEA), to analyze the acoustic-phonetic patterns of these words. Both words showed significant differences in duration depending on function. The first and second formant values of the conjunction tehát showed significant differences depending on whether it was used as a filler or in its original function as a conjunction. The formants of the stressed vowel in the pronoun ilyen did not show any differences with either males or females, but the second formant of the unstressed vowel, depending on function, showed significant variations with male subjects. Apparently, females make an unconscious distinction between the two functions only by varying the time structure of the word. Our data confirmed that these words are indeed undergoing a functional change, which is manifested in changes of their temporal patterns and, to some extent, in the articulation of their vowels.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © International Phonetic Association 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adamikné Jászó, Anna. 2004. Hangtan [Phonetics]. In Jászó, Anna Adamikné (ed.), A magyar nyelv könyve [The book of Hungarian], 137198. Budapest: Trezor.Google Scholar
Amir, Noam & Amir, Ofer. 2007. Novel measures for vowel reduction. In Trouvain, Jürgen & Barry, William J. (eds.), ICPhS XVI, 849852. Saarbrücken.Google Scholar
Bailey, Karl G. D. & Ferreira, Fernanda. 2007. The processing of filled pause disfluencies in the visual world. In van Gompel, Roger P. G., Fischer, Martin H., Murray, Wayne S. & Hill, Robin L. (eds.), Eye movements: A window on mind and brain, 487502. Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beke, András. 2008. Az alapfrekvencia-eloszlás modellezése a beszélőfelismeréshez [Modeling fundamental frequency distributions for automatic speech recognition]. Alkalmazott Nyelvtudomány 1/2, 5779.Google Scholar
Boersma, Paul & Weenink, David. 2004. Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (version 4.4). http://www.praat.org (18 September 2005).Google Scholar
Brennan, Susan & Schober, Michael. 2001. How listeners compensate for disfluencies in spontaneous speech. Journal of Memory and Language 44, 274296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan L. 2000. The phonology of the lexicon: Evidence from lexical diffusion. In Barlow, Michael & Kemmer, Susanne (eds.), Usage-based models of language, 6585. Stanford, CA: CSLI.Google Scholar
Bybee, Joan L. 2001. Phonology and language use. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan L. 2003. Mechanisms of change in grammaticization: The role of frequency. In Joseph, Brian D. & Janda, Richard D. (eds.), The handbook of historical linguistics, 602623. Malden, MA: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, Herbert H. 1994. Discourse in production. In Gernsbacher, Morton Anne (ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics, 9851022. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Clark, Herbert H. & Tree, Jean E. Fox. 2002. Using uh and um in spontaneous speaking. Cognition 84, 73111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corley, Martin, MacGregor, Lucy J. & Donaldson, David I.. 2007. It's the way that you, er, say it: Hesitations in speech affect language comprehension. Cognition 105, 658668.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curl, Traci S., Local, John & Walker, Gareth. 2006 . Repetition and the prosody–pragmatics interface. Journal of Pragmatics 38, 17211751.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Czuczor, Gergely & Fogarasi, János. 1865. A magyar nyelv szótára [Dictionary of Hungarian]. Pest: Emrich Gusztáv kiadása.Google Scholar
Dér, Csilla Ilona. 2004. Határok nélkül: a grammatikalizáció státusáról [Without frontiers: On the status of grammaticalization]. Nyelvtudományi Közlemények 101, 182194.Google Scholar
Dér, Csilla Ilona. 2006. Diskurzusszerveződés és grammatikalizáció [Discourse organization and grammaticalization]. Nyelvtudományi Közlemények 102, 247264.Google Scholar
Dér, Csilla Ilona & Markó, Alexandra. 2010. A pilot study of Hungarian discourse markers. Language and Speech 53 (2), 135180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dömötör, Adrienne. 2008. Az úgy mondtól az úgymondig. Egy diskurzusjelölő elem története az ómagyar kortól napjainkig. [From úgy mond ‘quoth he’ to úgymond ‘so to speak’: The story of a discourse marker from Old Hungarian to the present]. Magyar Nyelvőr 132, 3752.Google Scholar
Duez, Danielle. 1982. Silent and non-silent pauses in three speech styles. Language and Speech 25, 1125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fabulya, Márta. 2007. Izé, hogyishívják, hogymondjam. Javítást kezdeményező lexikális kitöltőelemek [Thingummy, whatsisname, howtoputit: Fillers initiating repair]. Magyar Nyelvőr 131, 324342.Google Scholar
Fehringer, Carol & Fry, Christina. 2007. Hesitation phenomena in the language production of bilingual speakers: The role of working memory. Folia Linguistica 41, 3772.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferreira, Fernanda & Bailey, Karl G. D.. 2004. Disfluencies and human language comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8, 231237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox Tree, Jean E. 2001. Listeners’ uses of um and uh in speech comprehension. Memory and Language 29, 320326.Google ScholarPubMed
Fox Tree, Jean E. 2002. Interpreting pauses and ums at turn exchanges. Discourse Processes 34, 3755.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox Tree, Jean E. & Clark, Herbert H.. 1997. Pronouncing ‘the’ as ‘thee’ to signal problems in speaking. Cognition 62, 151167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox Tree, Jean E. & Schrock, John C.. 2002. Basic meanings of you know and I mean. Journal of Pragmatics 34, 727747.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fraser, Bruce. 1999. What are discourse markers? Journal of Pragmatics 31, 931952.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gahl, Susanne. 2008. Time and thyme are not homophones: The effect of lemma frequency on word durations in spontaneous speech. Language 84, 474496.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gósy, Mária. 2004. Fonetika, a beszéd tudománya [Phonetics: The science of speech]. Budapest: Osiris Kiadó.Google Scholar
Gósy, Mária. 2005a. Pszicholingvisztika [Psycholinguistics]. Budapest: Osiris Kiadó.Google Scholar
Gósy, Mária. 2005b. Age-dependent processing of pauses in spontaneous speech. The Phonetician 2, 518.Google Scholar
Gósy, Mária & Gyarmathy, Dorottya. 2008. A nyelvhasználati változás egy jelensége [On how language use changes over time]. Magyar Nyelvőr 132, 206222.Google Scholar
Hentschel, Elke & Weydt, Harald. 2002. Die Wortart ‘Partikel’. In Cruse, Alan D., Hundsmurscher, Franz, Job, Michael & Lutzeier, Peter Rolf (eds.), Lexikologie. Lexicology. Ein internationales Handbuch zur Natur und Struktur von Wörtern und Wortschätzen. Lexicology: An international handbook on the nature and structure of words and vocabularies, 646653. Berlin & New York: De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Horváth, Viktória. 2009. Funkció és kivitelezés a megakadásjelenségekben [Function and execution in disfluent phenomena]. Ph.D. dissertation, ELTE University in Budapest.Google Scholar
Howell, Peter & Au-Yeung, James. 2002. The EXPLAN theory of fluency control and the diagnosis of stuttering. In Fava, Elisabetta (ed.), Clinical linguistics: Language pathology, speech therapy, and linguistic theory (Current Issues in Linguistic Theory), 7595. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jakobson, Roman. 1960. Closing statements: Linguistics and poetics. In Sebeok, Thomas A. (ed.), Style in language, 350377. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jasperson, Robert. 2002. Some linguistic aspects if closure cut-off. In Ford, Cecilia E., Fox, Barbara A. & Thompson, Sandra A. (eds.), The language of turn and sequence, 257286. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jefferson, Gail. 1974. Error correction as an interactional resource. Language in Society 2, 181199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jucker, Andreas H. 1993. The discourse marker well: A relevance-theoretical account. Journal of Pragmatics 19, 435452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lease, Matthew & , Mark Johnson. 2006. Early detection of fillers in processing conversational speech. The Human Language Technology Conference of the North American Chapter of the ACL, 7377. New York: Association for Computational Linguistics.Google Scholar
Leeuw, Esther de. 2007. Hesitation markers in English, German, and Dutch. Journal of Germanic Linguistics 19, 85114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levelt, Willem J. M. 1983. Monitoring and self-repair in speech. Cognition 14, 41104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levelt, Willem J. M. 1989. Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lindblom, Björn. 1990. Explaining phonetic variation: A sketch of the H&H theory. In Hardcastle, William J. & Marchal, Alain (eds.), Speech production and speech modeling, 403439. Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Local, John. 2003. Variable domains and variable relevance: Interpreting phonetic exponents. Journal of Phonetics 31, 321339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Local, John. 2004. Getting back to prior talk: And-uh(m) as a back-connecting device in British and American English. In Couper-Kuhlen, Elizabeth & Ford, Cecilia E. (eds.), Sound patterns in interaction: Cross-linguistic studies from conversation (Typological Studies in Language 62), 377400. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Local, John. 2007. Phonetic detail in talk-in-interaction: On the deployment and interplay of sequential context and phonetic resources. Nouveaux cahiers de linguistique française 28, 6786.Google Scholar
Local, John & Walker, Gareth. 2005. Methodological imperatives for investigating the phonetic organization and phonological structures of spontaneous speech. Phonetica 62, 120130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Louwerse, Matt M. & Mitchell, Heather Hite. 2003. Toward a taxonomy of a set of discourse markers in dialog: A theoretical and computational linguistic account. Discourse Processes 35, 199239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maekawa, Kikuo. 2003. Corpus of spontaneous Japanese: Its design and evaluation. ISCA [International Speech Communication Association] and IEEE [Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers] Workshop on Spontaneous Speech Processing and Recognition (SSPR 2003), 7–12. Tokyo.Google Scholar
Markó, Alexandra. 2004. Megakadások vizsgálata különféle monologikus szövegekben [The analysis of disfluencies in various monologues of Hungarian]. Beszédkutatás 2004, 209–222.Google Scholar
Merlo, Sandra & Mansur, Letícia Lessa. 2004. Descriptive discourse: Topic familiarity and disfluencies. Journal of Communication Disorders 37, 489503.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Misono, Yasuko & Kiritani, Shigeru. 1990. The distribution pattern of pauses in lecture-style speech. Logopedics and Phoniatrics 2, 110113.Google Scholar
Müller, Simone. 2005. Discourse markers in native and non-native English discourse. Philadelphia & Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Connell, Daniel C. & Kowal, Sabine. 2005. Uh and um revisited: Are they interjections for signaling delay? Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 34, 555576.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Keeffe, Anne & Adolphs, Svenja. 2008. Response tokens in British and Irish discourse: Corpus, context and variational pragmatics. In Schneider, Klaus P. & Barron, Anne (eds.), Variational pragmatics, 6998. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pierrehumbert, Janet B. 2001. Exemplar dynamics: Word frequency, lenition, and contrast. In Bybee, Joan [L.] & Hopper, Paul [J.] (eds.), Frequency effects and the emergence of lexical structure, 137157. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schachter, Stanley, Christenfeld, Nicholas, Ravina, Bernard & Bilous, Frances. 1991. Speech disfluency and the structure of knowledge. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 60, 362367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schegloff, Emanuel A. 1982. Discourse as an interactional achievement: Some uses of ‘uh huh’ and other things that come between sentences. In Tannen, Deborah (ed.), Analyzing discourse: Text and talk (32nd Georgetown University Round Table on Languages and Linguistics 1981), 7193. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Schegloff, Emanuel A. 2007. Sequence organisation in interaction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schegloff, Emanuel A., Sacks, Harvey & Jefferson, Gail. 1977. The preference for self-correction in the organization of repair in conversation. Language 53, 361382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schourup, Lawrence. 1999. Discourse markers. Lingua 107, 227265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shriberg, Elizabeth. 2001. To ‘errrr’ is human: Ecology and acoustics of speech disfluencies. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 31, 153169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swerts, Marc. 1998. Filled pauses as markers of discourse structure. Journal of Pragmatics 30, 485496.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watanabe, Michiko, Hirose, Keikichi, Den, Yasuharu & Minematsu, Nobuaki. 2008. Filled pauses as cues to the complexity of upcoming phrases for native and non-native listeners. Speech Communication 50, 8194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhao, Yuan & Jurafsky, Dan. 2005. A preliminary study of Mandarin filled pauses. DiSS’05: Disfluency in Spontaneous Speech Workshop, 10–12 September 2005, 179–182. Aix-en-Provence.Google Scholar