Introduction
Early-onset dementia (EOD), the onset of the disease at the age of 65 years or younger, is a diagnostic challenge. As there is a wider range of possible etiologies to consider in EOD than in late-onset dementia (LOD; Masellis et al., Reference Masellis, Sherborn, Neto, Sadovnick, Hsiung, Black, Prasad, Williams and Gauthier2013), EOD patients are more likely to end up being misdiagnosed than LOD patients (van Vliet et al., Reference van Vliet, de Vugt, Bakker, Pijnenburg, Vernooij-Dassen, Koopmans and Verhey2013; Woolley et al., Reference Woolley, Khan, Murthy, Miller and Rankin2011). One characteristic of EOD is early and prominent executive dysfunction (ED), which refers to the impairment of cognitive functions essential to fluent performance and goal-directed behaviors, such as in working memory operations, inhibition, and task-switching (Diamond, Reference Diamond2013; Miyake et al., Reference Miyake, Friedman, Emerson, Witzki, Howerter and Wager2000). ED is more prominent in both early-onset Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and early-onset vascular dementia (VaD) than in later-onset forms of such diseases (Jang et al., Reference Jang, Kwon, Kim, Jung, Lee, Lee, Chin, Im, Jeon, Lee, Seong, Kim, Kim, Choe, Lee, Kim, Kim, Lee, Na and Kim2016; Mendez, Reference Mendez2019; Smits et al., Reference Smits, Pijnenburg, Koedam, Van Der Vlies, Reuling, Koene, Teunissen, Scheltens and Van Der Flier2012). The core feature of the behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD) is ED, which is more common in EOD than in LOD (Rascovsky et al., Reference Rascovsky, Hodges, Knopman, Mendez, Kramer, Neuhaus, van Swieten, Seelaar, Dopper, Onyike, Hillis, Josephs, Boeve, Kertesz, Seeley, Rankin, Johnson, Gorno-Tempini, Rosen and Miller2011). ED is also often present in other common and potentially treatable conditions underlying cognitive impairment in younger patients, including depression, sleeping disorders, pain problems, and drug abuse (Alves et al., Reference Alves, Yamamoto, Arias-Carrion, Rocha, Nardi, Machado and Cardoso2014; Brownlow et al., Reference Brownlow, Miller and Gehrman2020; Bunk et al., Reference Bunk, Preis, Zuidema, Lautenbacher and Kunz2019; Lautenbacher et al., Reference Lautenbacher, Hoos, Hajak, Trapp and Kunz2021; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Mattick, Jamadar and Iredale2014). Detecting ED also has clinical value in identifying mild cognitive impairment (MCI) patients who either remain stable (Ganguli et al., Reference Ganguli, Jia, Hughes, Snitz, Chang, Berman, Sullivan and Kamboh2019) or progress to dementia (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Kang, Yu and Lee2016; Kirova et al., Reference Kirova, Bays and Lagalwar2015; Reinvang et al., Reference Reinvang, Grambaite and Espeseth2012).
Given the emphasis on ED in early-onset cognitive impairment, it is important to include the assessment of executive functions in cognitive evaluations conducted at an early stage. Typically, ED is assessed by standard neuropsychological tests, such as the Trail Making Test-Part B (TMT-B) (Reitan, Reference Reitan1958) and the Stroop test (Golden, Reference Golden1978), to measure working memory, set shifting, and inhibition. Considering the multifaceted nature of the executive functions, a normal performance on a single task is not sufficient evidence of intact executive functions (Burgess & Stuss, Reference Burgess and Stuss2017). As evidence of ED, clinical neuropsychologists often observe also qualitative aspects of performance that may not be presented on any test scores (Andrewes, Reference Andrewes2016). A comprehensive neuropsychological assessment is highly recommended but time-consuming (Sitek et al., Reference Sitek, Barczak and Harciarek2015). Cognitive screening tests can be useful in initial assessments and aid primary and occupational healthcare in identifying patients who need more detailed assessments. However, widely used screening tests for dementia, such as the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD; Morris et al., Reference Morris, Heyman, Mohs, Hughes, van Belle, Fillenbaum, Mellits and Clark1989) or the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE; Folstein et al., Reference Folstein, Folstein and McHugh1975), are designed for evaluating features typical of late-onset Alzheimer’s disease, including memory impairment and global cognitive performance deficits (O’Malley et al., Reference O’Malley, Parkes, Stamou, LaFontaine, Oyebode and Carter2019; Sitek et al., Reference Sitek, Barczak and Harciarek2015). Some screening tests include tasks that assess aspects of executive functioning, e.g., the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA; Nasreddine et al., Reference Nasreddine, Phillips, Bédirian, Charbonneau, Whitehead, Collin, Cummings and Chertkow2005) or the Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination-Revised (Mioshi et al., Reference Mioshi, Dawson, Mitchell, Arnold and Hodges2006).
To identify neuropsychological profiles with a prominent ED, it may be relevant to measure executive functions in more detail, as evidence has shown that a method specifically designed for detecting ED can better identify patients with dysexecutive profiles than general screening methods (Broche-Pérez et al., Reference Broche-Pérez, Bartuste-Marrer, Batule-Domínguez and Toledano-Toledano2019; Fiorentino et al., Reference Fiorentino, Gleichgerrcht, Roca, Cetkovich, Manes and Torralva2013). Related to this, the Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB; Dubois et al., Reference Dubois, Slachevsky, Litvan and Pillon2000) and the Institute of Cognitive Neurology (INECO) Frontal Screening (IFS; Torralva et al., Reference Torralva, Roca, Gleichgerrcht, Lopez and Manes2009) are tools designed to assess executive functions and may be particularly useful in the early detection of EOD, especially considering the prominence of ED in EOD. The FAB and the IFS have good psychometric properties in terms of internal consistency and concurrent validity (Bahia et al., Reference Bahia, Cecchini, Cassimiro, Viana, Lima-Silva, de Souza, Carvalho, Guimarães, Caramelli, Balthazar, Damasceno, Brucki, Nitrini and Yassuda2018; Dubois et al., Reference Dubois, Slachevsky, Litvan and Pillon2000; Torralva et al., Reference Torralva, Roca, Gleichgerrcht, Lopez and Manes2009). The clinical utilities of both tests have been evaluated mostly in older study populations, and both tests have shown the ability to distinguish healthy controls from patients with neurodegenerative diseases (Bahia et al., Reference Bahia, Cecchini, Cassimiro, Viana, Lima-Silva, de Souza, Carvalho, Guimarães, Caramelli, Balthazar, Damasceno, Brucki, Nitrini and Yassuda2018; Ihnen et al., Reference Ihnen, Antivilo, Muñoz-Neira and Slachevsky2013; Moreira et al., Reference Moreira, Lima and Vicente2014; Oguro et al., Reference Oguro, Yamaguchi, Abe, Ishida, Bokura and Kobayashi2006; Torralva et al., Reference Torralva, Roca, Gleichgerrcht, Lopez and Manes2009) or MCI (Goh et al., Reference Goh, Chan, Ali, Chew, Chuo, Chan and Lim2019; Moreira et al., Reference Moreira, Costa, Machado, Castro, Lima, Vicente and Ginsberg2019; Yamao et al., Reference Yamao, Nagata, Shinagawa, Nukariya, Ochiai, Kasahara and Nakayama2011). Furthermore, these tests can effectively distinguish AD from other dementia subtypes (Custodio et al., Reference Custodio, Herrera-Perez, Lira, Roca, Manes, Báez and Torralva2016; Iavarone et al., Reference Iavarone, Ronga, Pellegrino, Loré, Vitaliano, Galeone and Carlomagno2004; Nakaaki et al., Reference Nakaaki, Murata, Sato, Shinagawa, Matsui, Tatsumi and Furukawa2007; Slachevsky et al., Reference Slachevsky, Villalpando, Sarazin, Hahn-Barma, Pillon and Dubois2004; Torralva et al., Reference Torralva, Roca, Gleichgerrcht, Lopez and Manes2009). However, conflicting results have also been reported (Bahia et al., Reference Bahia, Cecchini, Cassimiro, Viana, Lima-Silva, de Souza, Carvalho, Guimarães, Caramelli, Balthazar, Damasceno, Brucki, Nitrini and Yassuda2018; Boban et al., Reference Boban, Malojcic, Mimica, Vukovic and Zrilic2012; Castiglioni et al., Reference Castiglioni, Pelati, Zuffi, Somalvico, Marino, Tentorio and Franceschi2006; Lipton et al., Reference Lipton, Ohman, Womack, Hynan, Ninman and Lacritz2005).
When comparing the two tests, the IFS seems more accurate in differentiating AD from bvFTD (Custodio et al., Reference Custodio, Montesinos, Lira, Herrera-Perez, Bardales and Valeriano-Lorenzo2017; Gleichgerrcht et al., Reference Gleichgerrcht, Roca, Manes and Torralva2011), as well as AD or FTD from healthy controls (Custodio et al., Reference Custodio, Herrera-Perez, Lira, Roca, Manes, Báez and Torralva2016; Moreira et al., Reference Moreira, Lima and Vicente2014). Furthermore, the IFS has been reported to identify MCI more accurately than the FAB (Fernández-Fleites et al., Reference Fernández-Fleites, Jiménez-Puig, Broche-Pérez, Morales-Ortiz, Luzardo and Crespo-Rodríguez2021) and to have stronger correlations with standard neuropsychological executive tests (Gleichgerrcht et al., Reference Gleichgerrcht, Roca, Manes and Torralva2011). Dementia diagnosis often requires clinical follow-up; therefore, the ability of a method to detect possible changes in executive functions is essential. There are only a few follow-up studies on the respective performances of the IFS and the FAB and they have shown partly contradictory results (Custodio et al., Reference Custodio, Montesinos, Lira, Herrera-Perez, Bardales and Valeriano-Lorenzo2017; da Silva et al., Reference Da Silva, Ordonez, Bregola, Bahia, Cecchini, Guimarães, Gambogi, Caramelli, Balthazar, Damasceno, Brucki, de Souza, Nitrini and Yassuda2021).
Despite the diagnostic challenges of EOD, there is limited literature about the utility of the IFS and the FAB in initial-level diagnostics with patients under 65 years of age. Thus, the current study aimed to extend previous studies by evaluating these tests for detecting ED in a younger clinical population, including multiple subgroups with different levels of cognitive impairment. We compared the performances of EOD, MCI due to neurological causes (MCI-n), MCI due to other causes such as depression or sleep disturbances (MCI-o) and subjective cognitive decline (SCD) groups. The MCI-n and MCI-o groups were analyzed separately because distinguishing between potentially reversible and nonreversible conditions is highly relevant in clinical settings. Given that ED is a prominent symptom in many EOD subtypes, we expected other patient groups to outperform EOD patients in both tests. Furthermore, considering previous studies, we hypothesized that the IFS has better discriminatory properties than the FAB in distinguishing patient groups (EOD/MCI-n vs others and EOD vs SCD) and detecting ED, as well as stronger correlations with standard neuropsychological tests. We also studied the test scores in a longitudinal setting and hypothesized that EOD patients would show a greater decline in performance on both tests than other groups, even though the results are not fully consistent in previous studies.
Materials and methods
Participants
Participants (n = 210) of the Cognitive Impairment and Work Ability study were recruited between 1.3.2019 and 31.3.2021 from among patients referred to the specialized memory outpatient clinics of Oulu or Kuopio University hospitals in Finland. These clinics are responsible for diagnosing neurodegenerative diseases in catchment areas in patients with symptom onset at the ages of 65 years or younger. All participants were native Finnish speakers. The specific inclusion criteria and patient groups are shown in Figure 1.
Experienced neurologists specializing in dementia and memory disorders diagnosed the patients in accordance with current diagnostic guidelines (Gorno-Tempini et al., Reference Gorno-Tempini, Hillis, Weintraub, Kertesz, Mendez, Cappa, Ogar, Rohrer, Black, Boeve, Manes, Dronkers, Vandenberghe, Rascovsky, Patterson, Miller, Knopman, Hodges, Mesulam and Grossman2011; McKhann et al., Reference McKhann, Knopman, Chertkow, Hyman, Jack, Kawas, Klunk, Koroshetz, Manly, Mayeux, Mohs, Morris, Rossor, Scheltens, Carrillo, Thies, Weintraub and Phelps2011; Mckeith et al., Reference McKeith, Boeve, Dickson, Halliday, Taylor, Weintraub, Aarsland, Galvin, Attems, Ballard, Bayston, Beach, Blanc, Bohnen, Bonanni, Bras, Brundin, Burn, Chen-Plotkin and Kosaka2017; O’Brien et al., Reference O'Brien, Erkinjuntti, Reisberg, Roman, Sawada, Pantoni, Bowler, Ballard, DeCarli, Gorelick, Rockwood, Burns, Gauthier and DeKosky2003; Rascovsky et al., Reference Rascovsky, Hodges, Knopman, Mendez, Kramer, Neuhaus, van Swieten, Seelaar, Dopper, Onyike, Hillis, Josephs, Boeve, Kertesz, Seeley, Rankin, Johnson, Gorno-Tempini, Rosen and Miller2011; Winblad et al., Reference Winblad, Palmer, Kivipelto, Jelic, Fratiglioni, Wahlund, Nordberg, Backman, Albert, Almkvist, Arai, Basun, Blennow, de Leon, DeCarli, Erkinjuntti, Giacobini, Graff, Hardy and Petersen2004) based on a comprehensive diagnostic workup that included medical history, neuropsychological assessment, magnetic resonance imaging of the brain, laboratory tests, and neurological examinations. The cerebrospinal fluid markers of Aβ42, tau and phosphotau (n = 77), or FDG-PET (n = 12) from a subset of patients were analyzed. When diagnostics required evidence of symptom progression, the patients’ conditions up to two years before receiving the diagnosis were assessed. All patients received oral and written information on the study and provided written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The ethics committees of the Northern Ostrobothnia and Northern Savo Hospital districts approved the study.
Diagnostic outcomes
The EOD, MCI-n, MCI-o, and SCD groups were formed based on the final diagnoses made at the memory clinic (Figure 1). The EOD group (n = 49) included patients diagnosed with neurodegenerative disorders, VaD or alcohol-related dementia. The MCI-n (n = 34) patients had vascular cognitive impairment or MCI due to a suspected neurodegenerative disease. The MCI-o group (n = 99) included individuals diagnosed with a cognitive impairment related to conditions other than a degenerative or vascular condition. In accordance with the research guidelines and recent findings (Jessen et al., Reference Jessen, Amariglio, Buckley, van der Flier, Han, Molinuevo, Rabin, Rentz, Rodriguez-Gomez, Saykin, Sikkes, Smart, Wolfsgruber and Wagner2020; Molinuevo et al., Reference Molinuevo, Rabin, Amariglio, Buckley, Dubois, Ellis, Ewers, Hampel, Klöppel, Rami, Reisberg, Saykin, Sikkes, Smart, Snitz, Sperling, van der Flier, Wagner and Jessen2017; Wolfsgruber et al., Reference Wolfsgruber, Kleineidam, Guski, Polcher, Frommann, Roeske, Spruth, Franke, Priller, Kilimann, Teipel, Buerger, Janowitz, Laske, Buchmann, Peters, Menne, Fuentes Casan, Wiltfang and Wagner2020), SCD (n = 14) patients reported subjective cognitive symptoms with a rather sudden or insidious onset and a concern associated with the symptoms. Their performances in the neuropsychological assessment were within a normal or subtle impairment range (i.e. not meeting the criteria of MCI). All the SCD patients were evaluated by the neurologists as not expressing any signs of an incipient neurodegenerative disease.
Neuropsychological assessment and classification of cognitive impairment levels
As part of the diagnostic workup for the participants, neuropsychological assessment was conducted by experienced psychologists specializing in assessing patients with memory disorders. Table 1 summarizes the neuropsychological tests used and the cognitive functions assessed by these tests. As with an advanced level neuropsychological assessment, the impairment level on each cognitive domain was determined based on clinical evaluations (i.e., integrative interpretation of the cognitive test results considering normative data, premorbid intelligence level, and qualitative and quantitative test performance) (Jacova et al., Reference Jacova, Kertesz, Blair, Fisk and Feldman2007; Weintraub, Reference Weintraub2022). The scoring was as follows: normal cognitive functioning (0), subtle (1), mild (2), moderate (3) or severe (4) cognitive impairment. The general impairment level was calculated as the mean of all cognitive domains.
Abbreviations: WMS, Wechsler Memory Scale; ROCFT, Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test; WAIS, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale; TMT-B, Trail Making Test Part B; TMT-A, Trail Making Test Part A.
a The scale on each cognitive domain was as follows: 0 = normal cognition, 1 = subtle impairment, 2 = mild impairment, 3 = moderate impairment, and 4 = severe impairment.
The IFS and the FAB
The participants attended another session for a more specific assessment of executive functions, including novel computer-based tests and the IFS and FAB tests. The assessment was conducted at baseline (average of 25 days after the neuropsychological assessment), 6 months and 12 months.
The FAB includes six subtests: Conceptualization, Verbal Fluency, Motor Programming, Conflicting Instructions, Motor Inhibitory Control, and Prehension Behavior (Dubois et al., Reference Dubois, Slachevsky, Litvan and Pillon2000). The score range for all the subtests is 0–3 points, resulting in a maximum score of 18 points. The IFS consists of eight subtests (Torralva et al., Reference Torralva, Roca, Gleichgerrcht, Lopez and Manes2009), including three identical subtests with the FAB (Motor Programming, Conflicting Instructions, Motor Inhibitory Control), along with Verbal Inhibitory Control (0–6 points), Abstraction Capacity (0–3 points), Backward Digit Span (0–6 points), Verbal Working Memory (0–2 points), and the Spatial Working Memory (0–4 points). The maximum score for the IFS is 30 points, and a higher score reflects better functioning in both tests. Officially translated (Finnish) versions of the original tests were used in the study. The tests were administered in the same order to all participants: first the shared subtests, followed by the FAB only and then the IFS subtests only.
Data analysis
Group differences for gender, education, and work status were identified using the chi-square test, while those for age and the IFS and FAB scores were examined using the non-parametric Kruskal-Wallis test. The IFS and the FAB results were also analyzed using the quantile regression, with age and diagnostic group as the explanatory variables. The eta-squared (η 2) was calculated as an effect size measure and interpreted as small (.01–.059), medium (.06–.139) or large (≥.14) (Fritz et al., Reference Fritz, Morris and Richler2012). For correlation analyses, the Spearman’s correlation coefficient (r) was used, and the results were interpreted as very weak (≤.19), weak (.20–.39), moderate (.40–.59), strong (.60–.79) or very strong (≥.80) (Evans, Reference Evans1996). For comparing the IFS and the FAB follow-up data, the Wilcoxon signed-rank test and r-values as effect size measures were calculated (Tomczak & Tomczak, Reference Tomczak and Tomczak2014). Receiver operating characteristic (ROC) analysis with area under the curve (AUC) values was used for evaluating the IFS and the FAB and distinguishing the following groups: (a) EOD and MCI-n patients from other patient groups, (b) EOD from SCD subjects, and (c) patients exhibiting at least a mild impairment in the executive function and/or working memory domains from those who perform within a normal range. The discriminatory accuracies of the IFS and the FAB were compared following the method described by DeLong et al. (Reference DeLong, DeLong and Clarke-Pearson1988).
A p-value < .05 was considered statistically significant. The Bonferroni correction was used to adjust the significance values in pairwise comparisons. All statistical analyses were performed using the IBM SPSS version 27.0 software.
Results
Demographics
The demographics for the total study population and the diagnostic groups are summarized in Table 2. The results showed that the EOD, MCI-n, MCI-o, and the SCD groups did not differ in terms of gender (p = .943) or education level (p = .455). However, significant (p < .001) differences in age were observed: the EOD and MCI-n patients were older than the MCI-o and SCD subjects. Moreover, the proportion of SCD subjects currently working was significantly higher than in any of the other patient groups (p < .001). Thus, patients diagnosed with a cognitive impairment, irrespective of its etiology, were likelier to be outside of working life.
Abbreviations: EOD = Early-onset dementia; MCI-n = Mild cognitive impairment-neurological; MCI-o = Mild cognitive impairment-other; SCD = Subjective cognitive decline; SD, standard deviation. Statistically significant (p < .05) p-values are highlighted. a,b,c,dLetter-pair (e.g., a–a) denotes a statistically significant pairwise group comparison (p < .05, after correction with the Bonferroni method). *All pairwise comparisons to the other diagnostic group are significant (p < .05, after correction with the Bonferroni method).
Group differences in the IFS and the FAB performance
The IFS and the FAB results of the total sample and diagnostic groups are summarized in Table 3. The EOD and MCI-n groups had significantly lower scores on the IFS than the MCI-o and SCD groups. The SCD group also performed at a higher level than the MCI-o patients; therefore, the SCD group outperformed all other groups in the IFS total score. The FAB score results followed a similar pattern; however, the difference between the SCD and MCI-o groups was not significant.
Abbreviations: EOD = Early-onset dementia; MCI-n = Mild cognitive impairment-neurological; MCI-o = Mild cognitive impairment-other; SCD = Subjective cognitive decline; η 2 = eta-squared estimate of an effect size. Statistically significant (p < .05) p-values are highlighted. a,b,c,dLetter-pair (e.g., a–a) denotes a statistically significant pairwise group comparison (p < .05, after correction with the Bonferroni method). *All pairwise comparisons to the other diagnostic groups are significant (p < .05, after correction with the Bonferroni method).
Age (p < .05) and diagnostic group (p < .01) contributed significantly to the IFS and the FAB regression models. The diagnostic group had a stronger relative contribution than the age in the IFS and FAB regression models, as the model fit decreased by 11% and 12% for the IFS and FAB, respectively, after removing the diagnostic group, and by 3% and 0.3%, respectively, after removing age.
Shared subtests of the IFS and the FAB
In the Motor Programming subtest, the SCD group performed better than the other groups (Figure 2). In Conflicting Instructions, the EOD group performed at a lower level than the MCI-n and MCI-o groups. Significant group differences were also observed in Motor Inhibitory Control, where the MCI-o and SCD groups performed better than the EOD and MCI-n groups.
Subtests included only in the IFS
Statistically significant group differences were observed in all the subtests included only in the IFS. The SCD group performed better than the EOD and MCI-n groups in Backward Digit Span, whereas the EOD group scored significantly lower than the MCI-o and SCD groups in Verbal Working Memory. The SCD group outperformed all other groups in Spatial Working Memory. There were also statistically significant group differences in Abstraction Capacity, in which the EOD group had weaker results than the SCD group. Finally, the MCI-n group had a lower score on Verbal Inhibitory Control than the MCI-o and SCD groups.
Subtests included only in the FAB
In the subtests unique to the FAB, the only statistically significant group difference was found in the Conceptualization task, wherein the EOD group performed worse than the MCI-o group. All subjects scored a full 3 points in Prehension Behavior, reflecting a notable ceiling effect for this subtest.
Correlations between the IFS/FAB scores and neuropsychological measures
In the complete sample, there were several significant associations between the IFS and the FAB test scores and the clinical neuropsychological impairment levels (Table 4). In particular, the general cognitive impairment correlated strongly with the IFS (r = −.63) and moderately with the FAB (r = −.56). Moderate correlations were observed between the scores for the executive function impairment and frontal screening tests (for IFS, r = −.57; for FAB, r = −.48) and the TMT-B test (for IFS, r = −.59; for FAB, r = −.52). The correlations between the neuropsychological measures and the IFS total score and subtests were more often statistically significant and were also slightly stronger than those of the FAB total score and subtests. The correlations for the Prehension Behavior subtest could not be calculated due to the lack of variance in the results.
Abbreviations: IFS = INECO Frontal Screening; FAB = Frontal Assessment Battery; MP = Motor Programming; CI = Conflicting Instructions; MIC = Motor Inhibitory Control; BDS = Backward Digit Span; VWM = Verbal Working Memory; SWM = Spatial Working Memory; AC = Abstraction Capacity; VIC = Verbal Inhibitory Control; C = Conceptualization; PB = Prehension Behavior; VF = Verbal Fluency.
Note. When r ≤ −0.15, p < .05. When r ≤ −0.20, p < .01. Correlations ≤ −0.40 are highlighted.
Results related to the longitudinal setting
The IFS and the FAB scores and the results of the statistical analyses at baseline, 6 months and 12 months are shown in Table 5 and Figure 3. The performance of the EOD group in the IFS test did not change from baseline to follow-up. When examining only those EOD patients diagnosed with AD, we found significantly (p < .05) lower scores at the latter measurement points than at baseline (mean scores for baseline: 18.2; 6 months: 17.3; 12 months: 16.9). The MCI-n group’s scores increased in the follow-up: scores after 6 and 12 months were higher than at baseline (p < .05). The MCI-o group’s IFS scores increased from baseline to 12 months (p = .017). No significant changes in the SCD test scores were observed. The FAB scores increased significantly (p < .05) for the MCI-n group from baseline to 6 months and for the MCI-o group from baseline to 12 months. No other statistically significant differences were observed. The AD group’s FAB scores did not change during the follow-up.
Abbreviations: EOD = Early-onset dementia; MCI-n = Mild cognitive impairment-neurological; MCI-o = Mild cognitive impairment-other; SCD = Subjective cognitive decline. Statistically significant (p < .05) p-values are highlighted.
a The calculated baseline mean and standard deviation (SD), including those who participated at 6 and 12-months follow-up.
Discrimination accuracy of the IFS and the FAB
The ability to distinguish EOD and MCI-n patients from MCI-o and SCD patients was studied (Figure 4). The IFS (AUC = .74) and the FAB (AUC = .75) showed moderate and similar (p = .689) accuracies in differentiating between EOD and MCI-n patients from MCI-o and SCD patients. The optimal cutoff values for the group discriminations were defined by visually inspecting the ROC curves and selecting the highest combination of sensitivity and specificity when the sensitivity value exceeded 80%. For the IFS, a cutoff score of 22.5 produced a sensitivity and a specificity of 86.7% and 43.4%, respectively, whereas the corresponding values for a score of 23 were 91.6% and 39.8%. For the FAB, a cutoff score of 16 produced a sensitivity of 73.5% and a specificity of 59.5%, whereas the corresponding values for a cutoff score of 17 were 90.4% and 38.1%, respectively.
Furthermore, both tests showed similar (p = .409) abilities to differentiate EOD patients from the SCD group (IFS AUC = .92, FAB AUC = .89). With 22.5 points as a cutoff value for the IFS, the sensitivity was 83.7%, and the specificity was 85.7%. Considering the FAB, a cutoff value of 17 points produced a sensitivity of 89.8%; however, specificity was notably lower (64.3%).
In differentiating patients with ED from those who showed normal performance, the difference in discriminatory accuracy between the IFS and the FAB was statistically significant (p = .036, for IFS, AUC = .80 and for FAB, AUC = .75). With the same cutoff values (i.e. 22.5 and 17 points), the sensitivity and specificity of the IFS in detecting ED were 83.3% and 55.7%, respectively, whereas the corresponding values for the FAB were 81.7% and 40.0%.
Discussion
To our knowledge, this is the first study to evaluate the IFS and the FAB tests in assessing patients younger than 65 years in a memory clinic setting. The main finding reveals that both tests are applicable in detecting EOD patients from memory clinic patients who do not have neurological causes behind their cognitive problems. We also found that the IFS is more accurate than the FAB in differentiating individuals with ED from those with intact executive functioning. These findings have important clinical application potential because short and accurate cognitive tests targeted to capture ED are of great value for initial assessment in primary healthcare.
The results of the current study are in line with previous reports on older patient cohorts reporting that the IFS and the FAB can effectively differentiate dementia (Custodio et al., Reference Custodio, Herrera-Perez, Lira, Roca, Manes, Báez and Torralva2016) and MCI patients (Fernández-Fleites et al., Reference Fernández-Fleites, Jiménez-Puig, Broche-Pérez, Morales-Ortiz, Luzardo and Crespo-Rodríguez2021) from healthy controls. The patient groups differed significantly in the total scores and in many individual subtests. In particular, the subtests included in both tests seem to tap into relevant aspects of ED in younger patients. In general, the EOD group scored lower than the other groups, whereas the SCD group tended to score the highest. Specifically, the Motor Programming subtest can differentiate SCD from other patient groups, while the Conflicting Instructions subtest can distinguish patients with EOD from other memory clinic patients.
In line with previous findings, there were more significant group differences in the subtests included only in the IFS than in the FAB (Gleichgerrcht et al., Reference Gleichgerrcht, Roca, Manes and Torralva2011). In the subtests unique to the FAB, only one significant group difference was observed. The Prehension Behavior subtest seemed unsuitable for assessing younger memory clinic patients with cognitive symptoms, as all participants performed without errors. Our result is consistent with earlier findings suggesting that this subtest has low sensitivity and that the clinical usefulness of the FAB might be better without this specific subtest (Ilardi et al., Reference Ilardi, Chieffi, Scuotto, Gamboz, Galeone, Sannino, Garofalo, La Marra, Ronga and Iavarone2022). In fact, the scores of all of our patient groups on the FAB-only subtests were close to the maximum scores. Thus, these subtests may not be challenging enough for this patient population, or, alternatively, the scoring should be stricter. Furthermore, considering the stronger total score effect sizes, it may be more reasonable to use the total scores when making clinical evaluations of ED. As the IFS has a wider score range than the FAB, it may be less prone to the ceiling effect and more likely to capture differences in the mild impairment range.
To analyze concurrent validity, we examined the correlations between the IFS and the FAB performances and the patients’ cognitive impairment levels. The strongest correlations were found between the IFS and the FAB total scores and the general cognitive impairment level. Such a result can be interpreted as an indicator of low specificity. Conversely, we argue that our patient cohort’s characteristics (i.e. high prevalence of ED) and the operationalization of general cognitive impairment (i.e. based on thorough neuropsychological assessment instead of a narrow battery, such as MMSE) explain these results. Moreover, the correlation between the frontal screening tests’ total scores and the impairment of language skills was moderate, reflecting that many tests of language processing require executive functions, and vice versa (O’Callaghan et al., Reference O’Callaghan, Hodges and Hornberger2013). A single neuropsychological test rarely succeeds in strictly assessing one domain of cognition, and a poor test result may be due to several reasons (Sitek et al., Reference Sitek, Barczak and Harciarek2015). For example, difficulties in verbal fluency may be due to language processing, executive functions or processing speed deficits (Aita et al., Reference Aita, Beach, Taylor, Borgogna, Harrell and Hill2019; Stolwyk et al., Reference Stolwyk, Bannirchelvam, Kraan and Simpson2015; Whiteside et al., Reference Whiteside, Kealey, Semla, Luu, Rice, Basso and Roper2016). In our study, verbal fluency and similarity tests were used to assess language abilities. However, these can also be used to measure executive functions, and in fact, versions of them are included in the FAB. This likely explains the relatively high (r = −.52) correlation between the verbal fluency subtest and the language skills domain.
Some differences in the IFS and the FAB correlations with the neuropsychological variables were observed in favor of the IFS. For example, we found a moderate correlation between working memory impairment level and the IFS, whereas the correlation was weak between the FAB and working memory impairment. In accordance with a previous comparison study (Gleichgerrcht et al., Reference Gleichgerrcht, Roca, Manes and Torralva2011), the correlations were more often higher for the IFS than for the FAB. However, the differences are very small, so their clinical significance is likely to be minor.
Considering the assessment of symptom progression, Da Silva et al. (Reference Da Silva, Ordonez, Bregola, Bahia, Cecchini, Guimarães, Gambogi, Caramelli, Balthazar, Damasceno, Brucki, de Souza, Nitrini and Yassuda2021) found that the scores of both tests remained unchanged at 12-months follow-up for bvFTD and AD patients. Custodio et al. (Reference Custodio, Montesinos, Lira, Herrera-Perez, Bardales and Valeriano-Lorenzo2017) reported that the IFS scores of stroke patients with and without vascular cognitive impairment decreased at 12 months follow-up. In the present study, the IFS and the FAB scores of the EOD group were already abnormal at baseline, and against our hypothesis, no significant changes in follow-up were observed. When examining patients diagnosed with AD, the IFS scores decreased at follow-up. This may be due to AD patients typically having a greater impairment of memory functions than other patients with dementia, resulting in a lower practice effect in retesting. However, such an effect was not observed for the FAB scores of AD patients. The IFS and the FAB scores of the MCI-n group tended to increase at the follow-up, and the same effects were also observed in the MCI-o group, although only for the IFS. This indicates that MCI patients may have benefitted from retesting more than EOD patients.
Interestingly, no significant practice effect was observed in the SCD group, which may be due to a ceiling effect, i.e., high performance already at baseline (IFS = 24.7 and FAB = 16.9). Taken together, our results reveal that both tests should be used with caution as a follow-up measure in younger patients, at least when the test-retest interval is as short as 6 months. The results also suggest that the unchanged scores in relatively short-term retesting should not be considered as an exclusion criterion for EOD. In general, the assessment of ED in a longitudinal setting might be problematic because retesting might reduce the task’s ability to capture the target executive process. This might be a general feature of executive tasks (Calamia et al., Reference Calamia, Markon and Tranel2013; Lemay et al., Reference Lemay, Bédard, Rouleau and Tremblay2004) rather than a specific weakness of the present methods.
Finally, we evaluated and compared the discriminatory accuracy of the IFS and the FAB in different settings. Given the importance of early diagnosis for patients and their caregivers, one of the main purposes of a screening test is to recognize potential dementia patients. Therefore, it was important to evaluate the tests’ accuracy in distinguishing EOD and MCI-n patients from the MCI-o and SCD groups, and we found it to be moderate for both tests. With cutoff scores of 22.5 for the IFS (sensitivity = 0.87, specificity = 0.43) and 17 for the FAB (sensitivity = 0.90, specificity = 0.38), the tests had higher sensitivity but lower specificity. In the case of screening tests, the emphasis on sensitivity rather than specificity does not lead to wrong diagnosis, because the tests are used to assess the need for referral to a more detailed diagnostic evaluation. Furthermore, sensitivity would likely have been higher had we compared the patient groups to healthy controls instead of other memory clinic patients. Notably, the cutoff score of 17 is only one point lower than the maximum score of the FAB, indicating that the participants performed well on the test. Thus, in clinical practice, the discrimination power of the FAB might turn out to be weak. Moreover, when examining the ability to differentiate subjects with ED from those with intact executive functioning, the IFS proved to be superior to the FAB. This might be related to the fact that the former covers a wider range of executive functions and includes tests for verbal inhibitory control and working memory. These subtests have demonstrated high sensitivity in detecting subtle EDs also in a previous study (Gleichgerrcht et al., Reference Gleichgerrcht, Roca, Manes and Torralva2011).
One of the strengths of our study was the prospective study design and the nonselective nature of the study population, as all patients referred to the memory clinics were offered the opportunity to join the study, regardless of the diagnosis they received. By having no exclusion criteria other than age at symptom onset, we wanted to capture the whole spectrum of patients with unclear etiologies to cognitive symptoms. Furthermore, we used a comprehensive neuropsychological assessment in the study and measured the correlations between the screening tests and cognitive domains more extensively than most of the earlier studies (Gleichgerrcht et al., Reference Gleichgerrcht, Roca, Manes and Torralva2011; Ihnen et al., Reference Ihnen, Antivilo, Muñoz-Neira and Slachevsky2013; Kugo et al., Reference Kugo, Terada, Ata, Ido, Kado, Ishihara, Hikiji, Fujisawa, Sasaki and Kuroda2007). The screening tests were completed at a later study visit, and the results were not given to the neurologist. Therefore, there was no risk that the screening test results would influence the neuropsychological assessment or the diagnosis. As the IFS and the FAB tests were conducted before the patients received a diagnosis, the results reflect the early phase of the evaluation process.
The major limitations of the study are its relatively small group sizes and the lack of a healthy population-level representative reference group. Instead, the SCD group served as a reference group, because it is clinically relevant to compare groups of patients referred for a memory clinic rather than to compare patients with healthy individuals. Due to the small group sizes, we were unable to analyze the dementia subgroups separately. However, the aim of the study was to compare the IFS and the FAB performances of cognitively symptomatic patients within the early-onset category and with different etiologies of cognitive impairment. Although our results do not tap into the exact differential diagnostics, we provide clinically relevant information for initial assessment purposes in differentiating patient groups and in identifying those who might need a referral towards a more thorough evaluation due to ED. Another weakness of the study is the lack of general screening tests. Having for example MoCA in the test battery would have enabled us to evaluate the possible additional benefits of the joint use of a specific ED assessment and global screening tools in assessing younger memory clinic patients. Finally, as all patients were native speakers of Finnish, the generalisability of the results to other groups remains to be addressed in future studies.
In conclusion, the IFS and the FAB seem to be useful in assessing younger memory clinic patients whose cognitive symptoms are due to different underlying causes. In line with previous reports on older or mixed patient cohorts, the IFS seems to have a slightly better ability to differentiate subjects with ED from those with intact executive functioning and to correlate with standard neuropsychological tests than the FAB. In the future, further research on the IFS and the FAB is needed in larger study samples and with separate disease groups and healthy controls in the study setting.
Acknowledgements
The authors have no conflicts of interest to report. The Cognitive Impairment and Work Ability study was funded by the Finnish Work Environment Fund (Grant Number: 180038). The first authors received personal research grants from the Finnish Brain Foundation sr (A-LH, VT) and the University of Oulu Scholarship Foundation (A-LH). This study was also supported by a grant from the Academy of Finland (Grant Number: 315460, AMR). No funding source had any role in the design, analysis or interpretation of the results in this study. We would like to thank Kia Gluschkoff for her support in the statistical analyses. We are also grateful to all the volunteer participants who made this study possible.