Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-r5zm4 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-05T00:18:34.944Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Spatial and temporal order memory in Korsakoff patients

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 May 2006

ALBERT POSTMA
Affiliation:
Psychological Laboratory, Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
MARIEKE VAN ASSELEN
Affiliation:
Psychological Laboratory, Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
OLGA KEUPER
Affiliation:
Psychological Laboratory, Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
ARIE J. WESTER
Affiliation:
Korsakoff Clinic, Vincent van Gogh Institute, Venray, The Netherlands
ROY P.C. KESSELS
Affiliation:
Psychological Laboratory, Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands Department of Neurology, University Medical Center, Utrecht, The Netherlands

Abstract

This study directly compared how well Korsakoff patients can process spatial and temporal order information in memory under conditions that included presentation of only a single feature (i.e., temporal or spatial information), combined spatiotemporal presentation, and combined spatiotemporal order recall. Korsakoff patients were found to suffer comparable spatial and temporal order recall deficits. Of interest, recall of a single feature was the same when only spatial or temporal information was presented compared to conditions that included combined spatiotemporal presentation and recall. In contrast, control participants performed worse when they have to recall both spatial and temporal order compared to when they have to recall only one of these features. These findings together indicate that spatial and temporal information are not automatically integrated. Korsakoff patients have profound problems in coding the feature at hand. Moreover, their lower recall of both features at the same time suggests that Korsakoff patients are impaired in binding different contextual attributes together in memory. (JINS, 2006, 12, 327–336.)

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2006 The International Neuropsychological Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Anderson, J.E. (1976). Short-term retention of the where and when of pictures and words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 105, 378402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brokate, B., Hildebrandt, H., Eling, P., Fichtner, H., Runge, K., & Timm, C. (2003). Frontal lobe dysfunctions in Korsakoff's syndrome and chronic alcoholism: Continuity or discontinuity? Neuropsychology, 17, 420428.Google Scholar
Cabeza, R., Anderson, N.D., Houle, S., Mangels, J.A., & Nyberg, L. (2000). Age-related differences in neural activity during item and temporal-order memory retrieval: A positron emission tomography study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 197206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cave, C.B. & Squire, L.R. (1991). Equivalent impairment of spatial and nonspatial memory following damage to the human hippocampus. Hippocampus, 1, 329340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cave, C.B. & Squire, L.R. (1992). Intact verbal and nonverbal short-term memory following damage to the human hippocampus. Hippocampus, 2, 151164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chalfonte, B.L., Verfaellie, M., Johnson, M.K., & Reiss, L. (1996). Spatial location memory in amnesia: Binding item and location information under incidental and intentional encoding conditions. Memory, 4, 591614.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Constantinidis, C. & Wang, X.J. (2004). A neural circuit basis for spatial working memory. Neuroscientist, 10, 553656.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dent, K. (2002). Representation and capacity in visual-spatial short-term memory. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Lancaster, UK.
Downes, J.J., Mayes, A.R., MacDonald, C., & Hunkin, N.M. (2002). Temporal order memory in patients with Korsakoff's syndrome and medial temporal amnesia. Neuropsychologia, 40, 853861.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fama, R., Marsh, L., & Sullivan, E.V. (2004). Dissociation of remote and anterograde memory impairment and neural correlates in alcoholic Korsakoff Syndrome. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10, 427441.Google Scholar
Feigenbaum, J.D., Polkey, C.E., & Morris, R.G. (1996). Deficits in spatial working memory after unilateral temporal lobectomy in man. Neuropsychologia, 34, 163176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feirrera, C.T., Verin, M., Levy, R., Dubois, B., & Agid, Y. (1998). Spatio-temporal working memory and frontal lobe lesions in man. Cortex, 34, 8398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friedman, H.R. & Goldman-Rakic, P.S. (1994). Coactivation of prefrontal cortex and inferior parietal cortex in working memory tasks revealed by 84 2DG functional mapping in the rhesus monkey. The Journal of Neuroscience, 14, 27752788.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ganzevles, P.G.J., De Geus, B.W.J., & Wester, A.J. (1994). Cognitive and behavioral aspects of Korsakoff's syndrome: The effect of special Korsakoff's, wards in general psychiatric hospital. Tijdschrift voor Alcohol, Drugs en Andere Psychotrope Stoffen, 20, 2031.Google Scholar
Glabus, M.F., Horwitz, B., Holt, J.L., Kohn, P.D., Gerton, B.K., Gallicott, J.H., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., & Berman, K.F. (2003). Interindividual differences in functional interactions among prefrontal, parietal and parahippocampal regions during working memory. Cerebral Cortex, 13, 13521361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hälbig, T.D., Mecklinger, A., Schriefers, H., & Friederici, A.D. (1998). Double dissociation of processing temporal and spatial information in working memory. Neuropsychologia, 36, 305311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heslinga, H., van den Burg, W., & Saan, R.J. (1983). De nieuwe 15- woordentest A en B in een gezonde populatie [The new 15-word test in a healthy population]. Groningen, The Netherlands: University of Groningen, Department of Neuropsychology.
Holdstock, J.S., Mayes, A.R., Cezayirli, E., Aggleton, J.P., & Roberts, N. (1999). A comparison of egocentric and allocentric spatial memory in medial temporal lobe and Korsakoff amnesics. Cortex, 35, 479501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huppert, F.A. & Piercy, M. (1976). Recognition memory in amnesic patients: Effect of temporal context and familiarity of material. Cortex, 12, 320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Igel, A. & Harvey, L.O. (1991). Spatial distortions in visual perception. Gestalt Theory, 13, 210231.Google Scholar
Inoue, M., Mikami, A., Ando, I., & Hideo, T. (2004). Functional brain mapping of the macaque related to spatial working memory as revealed by PET. Cerebral Cortex, 14, 106119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kafer, K.L. & Hunter, M. (1997). On testing the face validity of planning/problem-solving tasks in a normal population. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3, 108119.Google Scholar
Kessels, R.P.C., Postma, A., & De Haan, E.H.F. (1999). Object Relocation: A program for setting up, running, and analyzing experiments on memory for object locations. Behaviour Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 31, 423428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kessels, R.P.C., Postma, A., Wester, A.J., & de Haan, E.H.F. (2000a). Memory for object locations in Korsakoff's amnesia. Cortex, 36, 4757.Google Scholar
Kessels, R.P.C., Postma, A., Wijnalda, E.M., & De Haan, E.H.F. (2000b). Frontal-lobe involvement in spatial memory: Evidence from PET, fMRI, and lesion studies. Neuropsychology Review, 10, 101113.Google Scholar
Kessels, R.P.C., Van Zandvoort, M.J.E., Postma, A., & De Haan, E.H.F. (2000c). The Corsi block-tapping task: Standarisation and normative data. Applied Neuropsychology, 7, 252258.Google Scholar
Kopelman, M.D. (1989). Remote and autobiographical memory, temporal context memory and frontal atrophy in Korsakoff and Alzheimer patients. Neuropsychologia, 27, 437460.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kopelman, M.D. (2002). Disorders of memory. Brain, 125, 21522190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kopelman, M.D., Stanhope, N., & Kingley, D. (1997). Temporal and spatial context memory in patients with focal frontal, temporal lobe, and diencephalic lesions. Neuropsychologia, 12, 15331545.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacAndrew, S.B.G. & Jones, G.V. (1993). Spatial memory in amnesia: Evidence from Korsakoff patients. Cortex, 29, 235249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayes, A.R. (1988). Human Organic Memory Disorders. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Mayes, A.R., Meudell, P.R., & MacDonald, C. (1991). Disproportionate intentional spatial-memory impairments in amnesia. Neuropsychologia, 29, 771784.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayes, A.R., Meudell, P.R., Mann, D., & Pickering, A. (1988). Location of lesions in Korsakoff's syndrome: Neuropsychological and neuropathological data on two patients. Cortex, 24, 367388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayes, A.R., Meudell, P.R., & Pickering, A. (1985). Is organic amnesia caused by a selective deficit in remembering contextual information? Cortex, 21, 167202.Google Scholar
Meudell, P.R., Mayes, A.R., MacDonald, C., Pickering, A., & Fairbairn, A.F. (1991). Korsakoff amnesics are poor at judging the sequence of two tones. Cortex, 27, 431439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meudell, P.R., Mayes, A.R., Ostergaard, A., & Pickering, A. (1985). Recency and frequency judgements in alcoholic amnesics and normal people with poor memory. Cortex, 21, 487511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mimura, M., Kinsbourne, M., & O'Connor, M. (2000). Time estimation by patients with frontal lesions and by Korsakoff amnesics. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6, 517528.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miotto, E.C., Bullock, P., Polkey, C.E., & Morris, R.G. (1996). Spatial working memory and strategy formation in patients with frontal lobe excisions. Cortex, 32, 613630.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nairne, J.S. & Dutta, A. (1992). Spatial and temporal uncertainty in long-term memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 396407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, C.A., Monk, C.S., Lin, J., Carver, L.J., Thomas, K.M., & Truwit, C.L. (2000). Functional neuroanatomy of spatial working memory in children. Developmental Psychology, 36, 109116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nissen, M.J., Willingham, D., & Hartman, M. (1989). Explicit and implicit remembering: When is learning preserved in amnesia? Neuropsychologia, 27, 341352.Google Scholar
Oscar-Berman, M. (1992). The contributions of emotional and motivational abnormalities to cognitive deficits in alcoholism and aging. In L.R. Squire & N. Butters (Eds.), Neuropsychology of memory (2nd ed., pp. 8194). New York: Guilford Press.
Oscar-Berman, M. & Bonner, R.T. (1989). Nonmatching- (oddity) and delayed nonmatching-to-sample performance in aging, alcoholic, and alcoholic Korsakoff individuals. Psychobiology, 17, 424430.Google Scholar
Oscar-Berman, M. & Zola-Morgan, S.M. (1980). Comparative neuropsychology and Korsakoff's syndrome: I—Spatial and visual reversal learning. Neuropsychologia, 18, 499512.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oscar-Berman, M., Clancy, J.P., & Weber, D.A. (1993). Discrepancies between IQ and memory scores in alcoholism and aging. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 7, 281296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oscar-Berman, M., Hutner, N., & Bonner, R.T. (1992). Visual and auditory spatial and nonspatial delayed-response performance by Korsakoff and non-Korsakoff alcoholic and aging individuals. Behavioral Neuroscience, 106, 613622.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oscar-Berman, M., Sahakian, J., & Wikmark, G. (1976). Spatial probability learning by alcoholic Korsakoff patients. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 2, 215222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oslin, D., Atkinson, R.M., Smith, D.M., & Hendrie, H. (1998). Alcohol related dementia: Proposed clinical criteria. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 13, 203212.3.0.CO;2-B>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Owen, A.M., Stern, C.E., Look, R.B., Tracey, I., Rosen, B.R., & Petrides, M. (1998). Functional organization of spatial and nonspatial working memory processing within the human lateral frontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 95, 77217726.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paller, K.A., Acharya, A., Richardson, B.C., Plaisant, O., Shimamura, P., Reed, B.R., & Jagust, W.J. (1997). Functional neuroimaging of cortical dysfunction in alcoholic Korsakoff's syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 277293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Postma, A., Izendoorn, R., & De Haan, E.H.F. (1998). Sex differences and object location memory. Brain and Cognition, 36, 334345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reed, L.J., Lasserson, D., Marsden, P., Stanhope, N., Stevens, T., Bello, F., Kingsley, D., Colchester, A., & Kopelman, M.D. (2003). FDG-PET findings in the Wernicke-Korsakoff syndrome. Cortex, 39, 10271045.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shimamura, A.P., Janowsky, J., & Squire, L.R. (1990). Memory for the temporal order of events in patients with frontal lobe lesions and amnesic patients. Neuropsychologia, 28, 803813.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shoqeirat, M.A. & Mayes, A.R. (1991). Disproportionate incidental spatial-memory and recall in amnesia. Neuropsychologia, 29, 749769.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Asselen, M., Kessels, R.P.C., Neggers, S.F.W., Kappelle, L.J., Frijns, C.J.M., & Postma, A., (in press). Brain areas involved in spatial working memory. Neuropsychologia.
Van Asselen, M., van der Lubbe, R.H.J., & Postma, A. (2006). Are space and time automatically integrated in episodic memory? Memory, 14, 232240.Google Scholar
Verfaellie, M., Milberg, W.O., Cermak, L.S., & Letourneau, L.L. (1992). Priming of spatial configurations in alcoholic Korsakoff's amnesia. Brain and Cognition, 18, 3445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, D., Zhang, X., Sun, X., Li, Z., Wang, Z., He, S., & Hu, X. (2004). Cross-modal temporal order memory for auditory digits and visual locations: An fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping, 22, 280289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar