Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T04:41:12.094Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Sex hormonal modulation of hemispheric asymmetries in the attentional blink

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 May 2005

ANTJE HOLLÄNDER
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
MARKUS HAUSMANN
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
JEFF P. HAMM
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
MICHAEL C. CORBALLIS
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand

Abstract

The present study examines differences in functional cerebral asymmetries modulated by gonadal steroid hormones during the menstrual cycle in women. Twenty-one right-handed women with regular menstrual cycles performed a double-stream rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) task, with one stream in each visual field, during the low steroid menses and the high steroid midluteal phase. They were required to detect a target item, and then a probe item, each of which could appear in either stream. If the probe item appeared 200 ms after the target, detection of the probe was impaired—a phenomenon known as the “attentional blink.” This occurred in both streams in the midluteal phase, but only in the right visual field during menses. Thus low steroid levels appeared to restrict the attentional blink to the left hemisphere, while high levels of estradiol and progesterone in the midluteal phase appeared to reduce functional asymmetries by selectively increasing the attentional blink in the right hemisphere. This effect appears to be mediated by estradiol rather than progesterone, and it is compatible with the assumption of a hormone-related suppression of right hemisphere functions during the midluteal phase. (JINS, 2005, 11, 263–272.)

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2005 The International Neuropsychological Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Alexander, G.M., Altemus, M., Peterson, B.S., & Wexler, B.E. (2002). Replication of a premenstrual decrease in right-ear advantage on language-related dichotic listening tests of cerebral laterality. Neuropsychologia, 40, 12931299.Google Scholar
Altemus, M., Wexler, B.E., & Boulis, N. (1989). Changes in perceptual asymmetry with menstrual cycle. Neuropsychologia, 27, 233240.Google Scholar
Asthana, S., Baker, L.D., Craft, S., Stanczyk, F.Z. et al. (2001). High-dose estradiol improves cognition for women with AD: Results of a randomised study. Neurology, 57(4), 605612.Google Scholar
Berk, M. & Stein, D. (2002). Reproductive hormones as psychotropic agents? South African Psychiatry Review, 5, 2022.Google Scholar
Bibawi, D., Cherry, B., & Hellige, J.B. (1995). Fluctuations of perceptual asymmetry across time in women and men: Effects related to the menstrual cycle. Neuropsychologia, 33, 131138.Google Scholar
Boucart, M., de Visme, P., & Wagemans, J. (2000). Effect of benzodiazepine on temporal integration in object perception. Psychopharmacology, 52, 249255.Google Scholar
Boyle, G.J. (2002). Prediction of cognitive learning performance from multivariate state-change scores. Australian Educational and Developmental Psychologist, 3, 1721.Google Scholar
Breitmeyer, B.G., Ehrenstein, A., Pritchard, K., Hiscock, M., & Crisan, J. (1999). The roles of location specificity and masking mechanisms in the attentional blink. Perception and Psychophysics, 61(5), 798809.Google Scholar
Broadbent, D.E. & Broadbent, M.H.P. (1987). From detection to identification. Response to multiple targets in rapid serial visual presentation. Perception and Psychophysics, 42, 105113.Google Scholar
Chiarello, C., McMahon, M.A., & Schaefer, K. (1989). Visual cerebral lateralization over phases of the menstrual cycle: A preliminary investigation. Brain and Cognition, 11, 1836.Google Scholar
Chun, M.M. & Potter, M.C. (1995). A two-stage model for multiple target detection in rapid serial visual presentation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 109127.Google Scholar
Cummings, J. & Coffey, C. (1994). Neurobiological basis of behaviour. In C. Coffey & J. Cummings (Eds.), Textbook of geriatric neuropsychiatry (pp. 7196). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Daly, R.C., Schmidt, P.J., Davis, C.L., Danaceau, M.A., & Rubinow, D.R. (2001). Effects of gonadal steroids on peripheral benzodiazepine receptor density in women with PMS and controls. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 26(6), 539549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daniel, J.M. & Dohanich, G.P. (2001). Acetylcholine mediates the estrogen-induced increase in NMDA receptors binding in CA1 of the hippocampus and the associated improvement in working memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 69496956.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erez, A. & Isen, A.H. (2002). The influence of positive affect on the components of expectancy motivation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 10551067.Google Scholar
George, J.M. & Zhou, J. (2002). Understanding when bad moods foster creativity and good ones don't. The role of context and charity of feelings. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 687697.Google Scholar
Hampson, E. (1990a). Variations in sex related cognitive abilities across the menstrual cycle. Brain and Cognition, 14, 2643.Google Scholar
Hampson, E. (1990b). Estrogen-related variations in human spatial and articulatory motor skills. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 15, 97111.Google Scholar
Hausmann, M., Becker, C., Gather, U., & Güntürkün, O. (2002). Functional cerebral asymmetries during the menstrual cycle: A cross-sectional and longitudinal analysis. Neuropsychologia, 40, 808816.Google Scholar
Hausmann, M. & Güntürkün, O. (2000). Steroid fluctuations modify functional cerebral asymmetries: The hypothesis of progesterone-mediated interhemispheric decoupling. Neuropsychologia, 38, 13621374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heister, G., Landis, T., Regard, M., & Schroeder-Heister, P. (1989). Shift of functional cerebral asymmetry during the menstrual cycle. Neuropsychologia, 27, 871880.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holländer, A., Corballis, M.C., & Hamm, J.P. (2004). Visual-field asymmetry in dual-stream RSVP. Neuropsychologia, 43(1), 3540.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janowsky, D.S., Halbreich, U., & Rausch, J. (1998). Association among ovarian hormones, other hormones, emotional disorders, and neurotransmitters. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59 (Suppl. 5), 85106.Google Scholar
Jiang, Y. & Chun, M.M. (2001). The influence of temporal selection on spatial selection and distractor interference: An attentional blink study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Performance and Perception, 27(3), 664679.Google Scholar
Klein, R.M. & Dick, B. (2002). Temporal dynamics of reflexive attention shifts: A dual stream rapid serial visual-presentation exploration. Psychological Science, 13(2), 176179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lambert, A. & Duddy, M. (2002). Visual orienting with central and peripheral precues: Deconfounding the contributions of cue eccentricity, cue discrimination and spatial correspondence. Visual Cognition, 9, 303336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Larisch, R., Meyer, W., Klimke, A., Kehren, F., Vosberg, H., & Müller-Gärtner, H.-W. (1998). Left-right-asymmetry of striatal dopamine D2 receptors. Nuclear Medicine Communications, 19, 781787.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Man, M.S., MacMillan, I., Scott, J., & Young, A.H. (1999). Mood, neuropsychological function and cognitions in premenstrual dysphoric disorders. Psychological Medicine, 29, 727733.Google Scholar
McCourt, M.E., Mark, V.W., Radonovich, K.J., Willison, S.K., & Freeman, P. (1997). The effects of gender, menstrual phase and practice on the perceived location of the midsagittal plane. Neuropsychologia, 35(5), 717724.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McEwen, B.S. (1999). The molecular and neuroanatomical basis for estrogen effects in the central nervous system. Clinical Review, 84, 17901797.Google Scholar
McEwen, B.S. & Alves, S.H. (1999). Estrogen actions in the central nervous system. Endocrinological Review, 20, 278306.Google Scholar
McEwen, B.S. & Woolley, C. (1994). Estradiol and progesterone regulate neuronal structure and synaptic connectivity in adult as well as devleoping brain. Experimental Genrontology, 29, 431436.Google Scholar
Mead, L.A. & Hampson, E. (1996). Asymmetric effects of ovarian hormones on hemispheric activity: Evidence from dichotic and tachistoscopic tests. Neuropsychology, 10, 578587.Google Scholar
Miles, C., Green, R., Sanders, G., & Hines, M. (1998). Estrogen and memory in a transsexual population. Hormones and Behavior, 34(2), 199208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Osterlund, M.K. & Hurd, Y.L. (2001). Estrogen receptors in the human forebrain and the relation to neuropsychiatric disorders. Progress in Neurobiology, 64(3), 251267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oldfield, R.C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ozcan, M.E. & Banoglu, R. (2003). Gonadal hormone in schizophrenia and mood disorders. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 253, 193196.Google Scholar
Payne, J.L. (2003). The role of estrogens in mood disorders in women. International Review of Psychiatry, 15, 280290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, M.S. & Juola, J.F. (2000). Evidence for distinct attentional bottlenecks in attention switching and attentional blink tasks. Journal of General Psychology, 127, 624.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Potter, M.C., Chun, M.M., Banks, B.S., & Muckenhoupt, M. (1998). Two attentional deficits in serial target search: The visual attentional blink and an amodal task-switch deficit. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24(4), 979992.Google Scholar
Purdon, S.E., Klein, S., & Flor-Henry, P. (2001). Menstrual effects on asymmetrical olfactory acuity. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 7, 703709.Google Scholar
Raymond, J.E., Shapiro, K.L., & Arnell, K.M. (1992). Temporary suppression of visual processing in an RSVP task: An attentional blink? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 849860.Google Scholar
Reilly, J. & Kremer, J. (2002). PMS: Moods, measurements and interpretation. Irish Journal of Psychology, 22, 2237.Google Scholar
Rode, C., Wagner, M., & Güntürkün, O. (1995). Menstrual cycle affects functional cerebral asymmetries. Neuropsychologia, 33, 855865.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruch, W., Köhler, G., & van Thriel, C. (1997). To be in good or bad humor: Construction of the state form of the State-Trait-Cheerfulness-Inventory—STCI. Personality and Individual Differences, 22, 477491.Google Scholar
Sanders, G. & Wenmoth, D. (1998). Verbal and music dichotic listening tasks reveal variations in functional cerebral asymmetry across the menstrual cycle that are phase and task dependent. Neuropsychologia, 36(9), 869874.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, L.S., Farlow, M.R., Henderson, V.W., & Pogoda, J.M. (1996). Effects of estrogen replacement therapy on response to tacrine in patients with Alzheimer's disease. Neurology, 46, 15801584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seeman, M.V. (1997). Psychopathology in women and men: Focus on female hormones. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 16411647.Google Scholar
Shapiro, K.L., Raymond, J.E., & Arnell, K.M. (1994). Attention to visual pattern information produces the attentional blink in rapid serial visual presentation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 357371.Google Scholar
Shapiro, K., Hillstrom, A.P., & Husain, M. (2002). Control of visuotemporal attention by inferior parietal and superior temporal cortex. Current Biology, 12, 13201325.Google Scholar
Shaywitz, S.E., Shaywitz, B.A., Pugh, K.R., Fulbright, R.K., Skudlarski, P., Mencl, W.E., Constable, R.T., Naftolin, F., Palter, S.F., Marchione, K.E., Katz, L., Shankweiler, D.P., Fletcher, J.M., Lacadie, C., Keltz, M., & Gore, J.C. (1999). Effect of estrogen on brain activation patterns in postmenopausal women during working memory tasks. JAMA—The Journal of the American Medical Association, 281(13), 11971202.Google Scholar
Smith, S.S. (1991). Progesterone administration attenuates excitatory amino acid responses of cerebellar purkinje cells. Neuroscience, 42, 309320.Google Scholar
Smith, S.S., Waterhouse, B.D., Chapin, J.K., & Woodward, D.J. (1987a). Progesterone alters GABA and glutamate responsiveness: A possible mechanism for its anxiolytic action. Brain Research, 400, 353359.Google Scholar
Smith, S.S., Waterhouse, B.D., & Woodward, D.J. (1987b). Locally applied progesterone metabolites alter neuronal responsiveness in the cerebellum. Brain Research Bulletin, 18, 739747.Google Scholar
Smith, S.S., Waterhouse, B.D., & Woodward, D.J. (1988). Locally applied estrogens potentate glutamate-evoked excitation of cerebral purkinje cells. Brain Research, 475, 272282.Google Scholar
Taylor, T.L. & Hamm, J.P. (1997). Category effects in temporal visual search. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51, 3646.Google Scholar
Van Goozen, S.H.M., Cohen-Kettenis, P.T., Gooren, L.J.G., Frijda, N.H., & Van de Poll, N.E. (1995). Gender differences in behaviour: Activating effects of cross-sex hormones. Psychoendocrinology, 20, 343363.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walker, E.F. (2002). Adolescent neurodevelopment and psychopathology. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 2428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Visser, T.A.W., Bischof, W.F., & Di Lollo, V. (1999). Attentional switching in spatial and nonspatial domains: Evidence from the attentional blink. Psychological Bulletin, 125(4), 458469.Google Scholar
Weichselgartner, E. & Sperling, G. (1987). Dynamics of automatic and controlled visual attention. Science, 238, 778780.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Xu, H., Gouras, G.K., Greenfield, J.P., et al. (1998). Estrogen reduces neuronal generation of Alzheimer [beta]-amyloid peptides. Nature Medicine, 4, 447451.Google Scholar
Zenasni, F. & Lubart, T. (2002). Effects of mood states on creativity. Current Psychology Letters: Behavior, Brain and Cognition, 8, 3350.Google Scholar