Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gvvz8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T17:37:21.906Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Priming for novel between-word associations in patients with organic amnesia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 August 2005

G.A. CARLESIMO
Affiliation:
Clinica Neurologica, Università Tor Vergata, Roma, Italy I.R.C.C.S. Fondazione S. Lucia, Roma, Italy
R. PERRI
Affiliation:
I.R.C.C.S. Fondazione S. Lucia, Roma, Italy
A. COSTA
Affiliation:
I.R.C.C.S. Fondazione S. Lucia, Roma, Italy
L. SERRA
Affiliation:
I.R.C.C.S. Fondazione S. Lucia, Roma, Italy
C. CALTAGIRONE
Affiliation:
Clinica Neurologica, Università Tor Vergata, Roma, Italy I.R.C.C.S. Fondazione S. Lucia, Roma, Italy

Abstract

Ten amnesic patients of various etiologies and 10 matched normal controls participated in this study. On 2 consecutive days, subjects studied 30 novel word–word associations 6 times. Using a cued recall task, we assessed episodic learning and delayed retention of the study material immediately after each study phase and again 24 hr after the final study phase. Further, we evaluated implicit memory for new between-word associations by means of an automatic relational priming paradigm immediately after the delayed cued recall trial. Amnesic patients performed poorly on the cued recall task. Moreover, in the overall group of amnesics the priming effect failed to reach statistical significance. When the overall group of amnesics was split according to mean performance on the cued recall task, those in the low performer subgroup—comprised of 6 patients with direct or indirect involvement of the hippocampi—were particularly poor at episodically remembering the associations and did not reveal any relational priming. These data support the hypothesis of similar impairment of new episodic and implicit learning in amnesic patients and suggest that the hippocampus is crucial for both kinds of new learning. (JINS, 2005, 11, 566–573.)

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2005 The International Neuropsychological Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Aggleton, J.P., Keith, A.B., Rawlins, J.N., Hunt, P.R., & Sahgal, A. (1992). Removal of the hippocampus and transection of the fornix produce comparable deficits on delayed non-matching to position by rats. Behavioural Brain Research, 52, 6171.Google Scholar
Anderson, J.R. & Ross, B.H. (1980). Evidence against a semantic–episodic distinction. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 441465.Google Scholar
Bonanni, R., Carlesimo, G.A., Tomaiuolo, F., & Caltagirone, C. (2004). Amnesia following endoscopic third ventriculostomy: A single case study. European Neurology, 51, 118120.Google Scholar
Bortolini, V., Tagliavini, C., & Zampulli, A. (1971). Lessico di frequenze della lingua italiana contemporanea [Frequency of words in the contemporary Italian language]. Milan, Italy: Garzanti.
Brokate, B., Hildebrandt, H., Eling, P., Fichtner, H., Runge, K., & Timm, C. (2003). Frontal lobe dysfunctions in Korsakoff's syndrome and chronic alcoholism: Continuity or discontinuity? Neuropsychology, 17, 420428.Google Scholar
Cermak, L.S., Bleich, R.P., & Blackford, S.P. (1988). Deficits in the implicit retention of new associations by alcoholic Korsakoff patients. Brain and Cognition, 7, 312323.Google Scholar
Cohen, N.J., Poldrack, R.A., & Eichenbaum, H. (1997). Memory for items and memory for relations in the procedural/declarative memory framework. Memory, 5, 131178.Google Scholar
Dagenbach, D., Horst, S., & Carr, T.H. (1990). Adding new information to semantic memory: How much learning is enough to produce automatic priming? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 16, 581591.Google Scholar
Gabrieli, J.D.E., Keane, M.M., Zarella, M.M., & Poldrack, R.A. (1997). Preservation of implicit memory for new associations in global amnesia. Psychological Science, 8, 326329.Google Scholar
Gade, A. (1982). Amnesia after operations on aneurysms of the anterior communicating artery. Surgical Neurology, 18, 4649.Google Scholar
Gade, A. & Mortensen, E.L. (1990). Temporal gradient in the remote memory impairment of amnesic patients with lesions in the basal forebrain. Neuropsychologia, 28, 9851001.Google Scholar
Gadian, D.G., Aicardi, J., Watkins, K.E., Porter, D.A., Mishkin, M., & Vargha-Khadem, F. (2000). Developmental amnesia associated with early hypoxic-ischaemic injury. Brain, 123, 499507.Google Scholar
Gooding, P.A., Mayes, A.R., & van Eijk, R. (2000). A meta-analysis of indirect memory tests for novel material in organic amnesics. Neuropsychologia, 38, 666676.Google Scholar
Gooding, P.A., Mayes, A.R., van Eijk, R., Meudell, P.R., & McDonald, F.L. (1999). Do novel associative word stem completion and cued recall share the same memory retrieval processes? Memory, 7, 323343.Google Scholar
Goshen-Gottstein, Y., Moscovitch, M., & Melo, B. (2000). Intact implicit memory for newly formed verbal associations in amnesic patients following single study trials. Neuropsychology, 14, 570578.Google Scholar
Graf, P. & Schacter, D.L. (1985). Implicit and explicit memory for new associations in normal and amnesic subjects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 11, 501518.Google Scholar
Henke, K., Buck, A., Weber, B., & Wieser, H.G. (1997). Human hippocampus establishes associations in memory. Hippocampus, 7, 249256.Google Scholar
Holdstock, J.S., Mayes, A.R., Isaac, C.L., Gong, Q., & Roberts, N. (2002a). Differential involvement of the hippocampus and tem-poral lobe cortices in rapid and slow learning of new semantic information. Neuropsychologia, 40, 748768.Google Scholar
Holdstock, J.S., Mayes, A.R., Roberts, N., Cezayirli, E., Isaac, C.L., O'Reilly, R.C., & Norman, K.A. (2002b). Under what conditions is recognition spared relative to recall after hippocampus damage in humans? Hippocampus, 12, 341351.Google Scholar
Hopkins, R.O., Kesner, R.P., & Goldstein, M. (1995). Item and order recognition memory in subjects with hypoxic brain injury. Brain and Cognition, 27, 180201.Google Scholar
Mayes, A.R. & Gooding, P. (1989). Enhancement of word completion priming in amnesics by cueing with previously novel associates. Neuropsychologia, 271, 10571072.Google Scholar
McKone, E. & Slee, J.A. (1997). Explicit contamination in ‘implicit’ memory for new associations. Memory and Cognition, 25, 352366.Google Scholar
Neely, J.H. (1977). Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited-capacity attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 106, 226254.Google Scholar
Neely, J.H. (1991). Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories. In D. Besner & G.W. Humphreys (Eds.), Basic processing in reading (pp. 264236). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Paller, K.A. & Mayes, A.R. (1994). New-association priming of word identification in normal and amnesic subjects. Cortex, 30, 5373.Google Scholar
Pecher, D. & Raaijmakers, J.G. (1999). Automatic priming effects for new associations in lexical decision and perceptual identification. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52A, 593614.Google Scholar
Pruessner, J.C., Li, L.M., Serles, W., Pruessner, M., Collins, D.L., Kabani, N., Lupien, S., & Evans, A.C. (2000). Volumetry of hippocampus and amygdala with high-resolution MRI and three-dimensional analysis software: Minimizing the discrepancies between laboratories. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 433442.Google Scholar
Rempel-Clower, N.L., Zola, S.M., Squire, L.R., & Amaral, D.G. (1996). Three cases of enduring memory impairment after bilateral damage limited to the hippocampal formation. Journal of Neuroscience, 16, 52335255.Google Scholar
Schacter, D.L. & Buckner, R.L. (1998). Priming and the brain. Neuron, 20, 8595.Google Scholar
Schacter, D.L. & Graf, P. (1986). Preserved learning in amnesic patients: Perspectives from research on direct priming. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 8, 727743.Google Scholar
Schrijnemakers, J.M. & Raaijmakers, J.G. (1997). Adding new word associations to semantic memory: Evidence for two interactive learning components. Acta Psychologica, 96, 103132.Google Scholar
Shimamura, A.P. & Squire, L.R. (1989). Impaired priming of new associations in amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 15, 721728.Google Scholar
Squire, L.R. & Knowlton, B. (1995). Memory, hippocampus, and brain systems. In M.S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 825837). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Squire, L.R. & Zola, S.M. (1998). Episodic memory, semantic memory, and amnesia. Hippocampus, 8, 205211.Google Scholar
Tekkok, S.B., Godfraind, J.M., & Krnjevic, K. (2002). Moderate hypoglycemia aggravates effects of hypoxia in hippocampal slices from diabetic rats. Neuroscience, 113, 1121.Google Scholar
Tulving, E. & Markowitsch, H.J. (1998). Episodic and declarative memory: Role of the hippocampus. Hippocampus, 8, 198204.Google Scholar
Turriziani, P., Fadda, L., Caltagirone, C., & Carlesimo, G.A. (2004). Recognition memory for single items and associations in amnesic patients. Neuropsychologia, 42, 426433.Google Scholar