Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T05:06:16.813Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Positive and Negative Consequences of Making Coffee among Breakfast Related Irrelevant Objects: Evidence from MCI, Dementia, and Healthy Ageing

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 May 2017

María Rodríguez-Bailón*
Affiliation:
Departament of Physioterapy (Occupational Therapy) University of Malaga, Málaga, Spain Mind, Brain and Behavior Research Center, University of Granada, Granada, Spain
Tamara García-Morán
Affiliation:
Departament of Physioterapy (Occupational Therapy) University of Malaga, Málaga, Spain Asociación Granadina de Familias para la Rehabilitación del Daño Cerebral Adquirido “AGREDACE”, Granada, Spain
Nuria Montoro-Membila
Affiliation:
Mind, Brain and Behavior Research Center, University of Granada, Granada, Spain
Estrella Ródenas-García
Affiliation:
Mind, Brain and Behavior Research Center, University of Granada, Granada, Spain
Marisa Arnedo Montoro
Affiliation:
Department of Psychobiology, Mind, Brain and Behavior Research Center, University of Granada, Granada, Spain
María Jesús Funes Molina
Affiliation:
Department of Experimental Psychology, Mind, Brain and Behavior Research Center, University of Granada, Granada, Spain
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: María Rodríguez Bailón, Departamento de Fisioterapia (Terapia Ocupacional), Universidad de Málaga,Facultad de Ciencias de la Salud, C/ Arquitecto Francisco Peñalosa, 3, 29071 Málaga, Spain. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Objectives: Previous studies have reported impairments in activities of daily living (ADL) performance in the presence of irrelevant but physically/functionally related objects in dementia patients. The aim of the present study was to increase our knowledge about the impact of the presence of contextually related non-target objects on ADL execution in patients with multi-domain mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and dementia. Methods: We compared ADL execution in patients with MCI, dementia, and healthy elderly participants under two experimental conditions: One in which the target objects were embedded with contextually related non-target items that constituted the object set necessary to complete two additional (but unrequired) ADL tasks related to the target task, and a second, control condition where target objects were surrounded by isolated objects (they never constituted a whole set needed to complete an alternative ADL task). Results: Separate analysis of ADL errors associated with the target task versus errors involving the non-target objects revealed that, although the presence of contextually related objects facilitated the accomplishment of the target task, such a condition also led to errors involving the use of irrelevant objects in dementia and MCI. Conclusions: The presence of contextually related non-target items produces both positive and negative effects on ADL performance. These types of non-target objects might help to cue the retrieval of the action schema related to the target task, particularly in patients with MCI. In contrast, the presence of these objects might also lead to distraction in dementia and MCI. (JINS, 2017, 23, 481–492)

Type
Special Section: Mild Cognitive Impairment
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Albert, M.S., DeKosky, S.T., Dickson, D., Dubois, B., Feldman, H.H., Fox, N.C., & Phelps, C.H. (2011). The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease: Recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers & Dementia, 7(3), 270279. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-IV-TR. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Ardila, A., Ostrosky-Solis, F., & Bernal, B. (2006). Cognitive testing toward the future: The example of Semantic Verbal Fluency (ANIMALS). International Journal of Psychology, 41(5), 324332. doi: 10.1080/00207590500345542 Google Scholar
Arrighi, H.M., Gelinas, I., McLaughlin, T.P., Buchanan, J., & Gauthier, S. (2013). Longitudinal changes in functional disability in Alzheimer’s disease patients. International Psychogeriatrics / IPA, 25(6), 929937. doi: 10.1017/s1041610212002360 Google Scholar
Bettcher, B.M., Giovannetti, T., Macmullen, L., & Libon, D.J. (2008). Error detection and correction patterns in dementia: A breakdown of error monitoring processes and their neuropsychological correlates. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14(2), 199208. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1355617708080193 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bozeat, S., Ralph, M.A., Patterson, K., & Hodges, J.R. (2002). The influence of personal familiarity and context on object use in semantic dementia. Neurocase, 8(1-2), 127134. http://doi.org/10.1093/neucas/8.1.127 Google Scholar
Buxbaum, L.J., Schwartz, M.F., & Montgomery, M.W. (1998). Ideational apraxia and naturalistic action. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15(6-8), 617643. doi: 10.1080/026432998381032 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chrysikou, E.G., Giovannetti, T., Wambach, D.M., Lyon, A.C., Grossman, M., & Libon, D.J. (2011). The importance of multiple assessments of object knowledge in semantic dementia: The case of the familiar objects task. Neurocase, 17(1), 5775. http://doi.org/10.1080/13554794.2010.497156 Google Scholar
Chun, M.M., & Jian, Y.H. (1998). Contextual cueing: Implicit learning and memory of visual context guides spatial attention. Cognitive Psychology, 36(1), 2871. http://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1998.0681 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooke, K.Z., Fisher, A.G., Mayberry, W., & Oakley, F. (2000). Differences in activities of daily living process skills of persons with and without Alzheimer’s disease. Occupational Therapy Journal of Research, 20(2), 87105.Google Scholar
Cooper, R., & Shallice, T. (2000). Contention scheduling and the control of routine activities. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17(4), 297338.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fastenau, P.S., Denburg, N.L., & Mauer, B.A. (1998). Parallel short forms for the Boston Naming Test: Psychometric properties and norms for older adults. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 20(6), 828834. doi: 10.1076/jcen.20.6.828.1105 Google Scholar
Folstein, M.F., Folstein, S.E., & McHugh, P.R. (1975). Mini-Mental State. Practical method for grading cognitive state of patients for clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6 Google Scholar
Galleguillos, C., & Belongie, S. (2010). Context based object categorization: A critical survey. Computer Vision and Image Understanding, 114(6), 712722. doi: 10.1016/j.cviu.2010.02.004 Google Scholar
Giovannetti, T., Bettcher, B.M., Brennan, L., Libon, D.J., Kessler, R.K., & Duey, K. (2008). Coffee with jelly or unbuttered toast: Commissions and omissions are dissociable aspects of everyday action impairment in Alzheimer´s disease. Neuropsychology, 224(2), 235245.Google Scholar
Giovannetti, T., Bettcher, B.M., Brennan, L., Libon, D.J., Wambach, D., & Seter, C. (2010). Target-related distractors disrupt object selection in everyday action: Evidence from participants with dementia. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 16(3), 484494. doi: 10.1017/s1355617710000081 Google Scholar
Giovannetti, T., Libon, D.J., Buxbaum, L.J., & Schwartz, M.F. (2002). Naturalistic action impairments in dementia. Neuropsychologia, 40(8), 12201232. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(01)00229-9 Google Scholar
Giovannetti, T., Sestito, N., Libon, D.J., Schmidt, K.S., Gallo, J.L., Gambino, M., && Chrysikou, E.G. (2006). The influence of personal familiarity on object naming, knowledge, and use in dementia. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 21(7), 607614. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2006.05.005 Google Scholar
Gold, D.A., Park, N.W., Troyer, A.K., & Murphy, K.J. (2015). Compromised naturalistic action performance in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology, 29(2), 320333. http://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000132 Google Scholar
Gorelick, P.B., Scuteri, A., Black, S.E., DeCarli, C., Greenberg, S.M., & Iadecola, C., American Heart Association Stroke Council. (2011). Vascular contributions to cognitive impairment and dementia a statement for healthcare professionals from the American Heart Association/American Stroke Association. Stroke, 42(9), 26722713. doi: 10.1161/STR.0b013e3182299496 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humphreys, G.W., & Forde, E.M.E. (1998). Disordered action schema and action disorganisation syndrome. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15(6-8), 771811.Google Scholar
Levine, B., Robertson, I.H., Clare, L., Carter, G., Hong, J., Wilson, B.A., & Stuss, D.T. (2000). Rehabilitation of executive functioning: An experimental-clinical validation of goal management training. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6(3), 299312.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LaPointe, M.R.P., Lupianez, J., & Milliken, B. (2013). Context congruency effects in change detection: Opposing effects on detection and identification. Visual Cognition, 21(1), 99122. doi: 10.1080/13506285.2013.787133 Google Scholar
Macbeth, G., Razumiejczyk, E., & Ledesma, R.D. (2011). Cliff’s Delta Calculator: A non-parametric effect size program for two groups of observations. Universitas Psychologica, 10(2), 545555.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mack, W.J., Freed, D.M., Williams, B.W., & Henderson, V.W. (1992). Boston Naming Test - Shortened versions for use in Alzheimer’s disease. Journals of Gerontology, 47(3), P154P158.Google Scholar
McKhann, G.M., Knopman, D.S., Chertkow, H., Hyman, B.T., Jack, C.R. Jr., Kawas, C.H., & Phelps, C.H. (2011). The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: Recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers & Dementia, 7(3), 263269. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.005 Google Scholar
McLaughlin, P.M., Wright, M.J., LaRocca, M., Nguyen, P.T., Teng, E., Apostolova, L.G., & Woo, E. (2014). The “Alzheimer’s Type” profile of semantic clustering in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 20(4), 402412. doi: 10.1017/s135561771400006x Google Scholar
Mioshi, E., Hodges, J.R., & Hornberger, M. (2013). Neural correlates of activities of daily living in frontotemporal dementia. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 26(1), 5157. doi: 10.1177/0891988713477474 Google Scholar
Moores, E., Laiti, L., & Chelazzi, L. (2003). Associative knowledge controls deployment of visual selective attention. Nature Neuroscience, 6(2), 182189. doi: 10.1038/nn996 Google Scholar
Morady, K., & Humphreys, G.W. (2009). Comparing action disorganization syndrome and dual-task load on normal performance in everyday action tasks. Neurocase, 15(1), 112. http://doi.org/10.1080/13554790802524214 Google Scholar
Niki, C., Maruyama, T., Muragaki, Y., & Kumada, T. (2009). Disinhibition of sequential actions following right frontal lobe damage. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 26(3), 266285. doi: 10.1080/02643290903028484 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oltra-Cucarella, J., Perez-Elvira, R., & Duque, P. (2014). Benefits of deep encoding in Alzheimer Disease. Analysis of performance on a memory task using the Item Specific Deficit approach. Neurologia, 29(5), 286293. doi: 10.1016/j.nrl.2013.06.006 Google Scholar
Padilla, R. (2011). Effectiveness of occupational therapy services for people with Alzheimer’s disease and related dementias. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 65(5), 487489.Google Scholar
Palmer, T.E. (1975). The effects of contextual scenes on the identification of objects. Memory & Cognition, 3(5), 519526. doi: 10.3758/bf03197524 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peña Casanova, J. (1990). Programa integrado de exploración neuropsicológica. Test Barcelona. Barcelona: Masson.Google Scholar
Rainville, C., Lepage, E., Gauthier, S., Kergoat, M.J., & Belleville, S. (2012). Executive function deficits in persons with mild cognitive impairment: A study with a Tower of London task. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 34(3), 306324. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2011.639298 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rascovsky, K., Hodges, J.R., Knopman, D., Mendez, M.F., Kramer, J.H., Neuhaus, J., & Miller, B.L. (2011). Sensitivity of revised diagnostic criteria for the behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia. Brain, 134, 24562477. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr179 Google Scholar
Remy, F., Saint-Aubert, L., Bacon-Mace, N., Vayssiere, N., Barbeau, E., & Fabre-Thorpe, M. (2013). Object recognition in congruent and incongruent natural scenes: A life-span study. Vision Research, 91, 3644. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2013.07.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rey, A. (1964). L’examen clinique en Psychologia. Paris: Pressee Universitaires de France.Google Scholar
Rodriguez-Bailon, M., Montoro-Membila, N., Garcia-Moran, T., Luisa Arnedo-Montoro, M., & Funes Molina, M.J. (2015). Preliminary cognitive scale of basic and instrumental activities of daily living for dementia and mild cognitive impairment. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 37(4), 339353. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2015.1013022 Google Scholar
Schmitter-Edgecombe, M., McAlister, C., & Weakley, A. (2012). Naturalistic assessment of everyday functioning in individuals with mild cognitive impairment: The Day-Out Task. Neuropsychology, 26(5), 631641. doi: 10.1037/a0029352 Google Scholar
Schmitter-Edgecombe, M., & Parsey, C.M. (2014). Assessment of functional change and cognitive correlates in the progression from healthy cognitive aging to dementia. Neuropsychology, 28(6), 881893. doi: 10.1037/neu0000109 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, M.F., Buxbaum, L.J., Montgomery, M.W., Fitzpatrick-DeSalme, E., Hart, T., Ferraro, M., & Coslett, H.B. (1999). Naturalistic action production following right hemisphere stroke. Neuropsychologia, 37(1), 5166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, M.F., Montgomery, M.W., Buxbaum, L.J., Lee, S.S., Carew, T.G., Coslett, H.B., & Mayer, N. (1998). Naturalistic action impairment in closed head injury. Neuropsychology, 12(1), 1328. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.12.1.13 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, M.F., Segal, M., Veramonti, T., Ferraro, M., & Buxbaum, L.J. (2002). The Naturalistic Action Test: A standardised assessment for everyday action impairment. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 12(4), 311339. doi: 10.1080/09602010244000084 Google Scholar
Seligman, S.C., Giovannetti, T., Sestito, J., & Libon, D.J. (2013). A new approach to the characterization of subtle errors in everyday action: Implications for mild cognitive impairment. Clinical Neuropsychologist doi: 10.1080/13854046.2013.852624 Google Scholar
Snowden, J., Griffiths, H., & Neary, D. (1994). Semantic dementia - Autobiographical contribution to preservation of meaning. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 11(3), 265288. doi: 10.1080/02643299408251976 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suh, G.H., Ju, Y.S., Yeon, B.K., & Shah, A. (2004). A longitudinal study of Alzheimer’s disease: Rates of cognitive and functional decline. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 19(9), 817824. doi: 10.1002/gps.1168 Google Scholar
Sun, H.M., Simon-Dack, S.L., Gordon, R.D., & Teder, W.A. (2011). Contextual influences on rapid object categorization in natural scenes. Brain Research, 1398, 4054. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2011.04.029 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Torralva, T., Roca, M., Gleichgerrcht, E., Lopez, P., & Manes, F. (2009). INECO Frontal Screening (IFS): A brief, sensitive, and specific tool to assess executive functions in dementia. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15(5), 777786. doi: 10.1017/s1355617709990415 Google Scholar
Traykov, L., Raoux, N., Latour, F., Gallo, L., Hanon, O., Baudic, S., & Rigaud, A.-S. (2007). Executive functions deficit in mild cognitive impairment. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 20(4), 219224.Google Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Rodríguez-Bailón supplementary material

Rodríguez-Bailón supplementary material 1

Download Rodríguez-Bailón supplementary material(File)
File 204.3 KB