Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-94fs2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-15T10:11:41.223Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Patterns of neuropsychological impairment in MCI patients with small subcortical infarcts or hippocampal atrophy

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 June 2008

GUIDO GAINOTTI*
Affiliation:
Neuropsychology Service of the UCSC/Policlinico Gemelli, Catholic University of Rome, Rome, Italy
MONICA FERRACCIOLI
Affiliation:
Neuropsychology Service of the UCSC/Policlinico Gemelli, Catholic University of Rome, Rome, Italy
MARIA GABRIELLA VITA
Affiliation:
Neuropsychology Service of the UCSC/Policlinico Gemelli, Catholic University of Rome, Rome, Italy
CAMILLO MARRA
Affiliation:
Neuropsychology Service of the UCSC/Policlinico Gemelli, Catholic University of Rome, Rome, Italy
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Guido Gainotti, Neuropsychology Service, Policlinico Gemelli, Catholic University of Rome, Largo A. Gemelli, 8, 00168 Roma, Italy. E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

We investigated whether MCI patients with hippocampal atrophy or multiple subcortical infarcts demonstrate neuropsychological patterns and markers considered typical of Alzheimer's disease (AD) and of vascular dementia (VD), respectively. An extensive neuropsychological battery, including tests of memory, visual-spatial and executive functions, language, attention, praxis and psychomotor speed, was administered to 36 mild cognitive impairment (MCI) patients with hippocampal atrophy and 41 MCI patients with multiple subcortical infarcts. Both groups of MCI patients were very mildly impaired and well matched in terms of MMSE scores. A clear, disproportionately severe, episodic memory disorder was observed in MCI patients with hippocampal atrophy. A less specific neuropsychological profile, consisting of impairment on an Action Naming task that is sensitive to frontal lobe lesions, was observed in MCI patients with multiple subcortical infarcts. In MCI patients, a disproportionately severe episodic memory impairment strongly points to an Alzheimer's type brain pathology, whereas the prevalence of executive deficits and other frontal lobe symptoms are a much weaker diagnostic marker of small vessel subcortical disease. (JINS, 2008, 14, 611–619.)

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2008

INTRODUCTION

The term Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) is used to identify nondemented patients who have an increased risk of developing a dementia of the Alzheimer's type (DAT). The defining features of the MCI are the presence of subjective memory complaints and objective memory impairment, the absence of obvious impairment in other cognitive domains, the absence of deficits in daily living activities, and an absence of dementia (Dubois & Albert, Reference Dubois and Albert2004; Petersen et al., Reference Petersen, Smith, Waring, Ivnik, Tangalos and Kokmen1999; Ritchie & Touchon, Reference Ritchie and Touchon2000). Studies conducted to date have shown that MCI patients are at increased risk of developing DAT, although the nature of this risk is very different from study to study as a function of clinical, neuroradiological, and neuropsychological variables (e.g., Nordlund et al., Reference Nordlund, Rolstad, Hellstrom, Sjogrem, Hansen and Wallin2005; Ritchie & Touchon, Reference Ritchie and Touchon2000; Shah et al., Reference Shah, Tangalos and Petersen2000). To improve the validity of the MCI construct, Petersen et al. (Reference Petersen, Doody, Kurz, Mohs, Morris, Rabins, Ritchie, Rossor, Thal and Winbland2001) have tried to differentiate various neuropsychological subtypes of MCI, distinguishing “amnestic MCI,” from “multiple cognitive domain MCI” and “single nonmemory domain MCI”). In line with this distinction, Petersen et al. (Reference Petersen, Doody, Kurz, Mohs, Morris, Rabins, Ritchie, Rossor, Thal and Winbland2001) and Dubois and Albert (Reference Dubois and Albert2004) have shown that the “amnestic” type shows the highest accuracy in predicting progression toward DAT. Furthermore, Petersen et al. (Reference Petersen, Doody, Kurz, Mohs, Morris, Rabins, Ritchie, Rossor, Thal and Winbland2001) have hypothesized that “multiple domain MCI” is most likely to progress to vascular dementia (VD). However, Lopez et al. (Reference Lopez, Becker, Jagust, Fitzpatrick, Carlson, DeKosky, Breitner, Lyketos, Jones, Kawas and Kuller2005) maintained that this classification of MCI patients does not capture the nature of the underlying pathology. Supporting this argument, Nordlund et al. (Reference Nordlund, Rolstad, Hellstrom, Sjogrem, Hansen and Wallin2005) claimed that very few subjects have isolated memory impairment and Rasquin et al. (Reference Rasquin, Lodder, Visser, Lousberg and Verhey2005) demonstrated that the “multiple domain MCI” subtype is predictive of later development of both DAT and VD.

Some authors have investigated whether MCI patients with different clinical presentations have different patterns of neuropsychological impairment and tend to evolve toward different types of dementia. In particular, these studies attempted to clarify whether MCI patients showing high risk factors for cerebrovascular diseases have a different pattern of neuropsychological impairment from that of MCI patients with no evidence of vascular risk factors. This line of research followed from the observations that, according to some authors, there are differences between the neuropsychological patterns of DAT patients and those of patients with VD resulting from small-vessel disease. The former consistently show a predominance of episodic memory disorders (Gainotti et al., Reference Gainotti, Monteleone, Parlato and Carlomagno1989, Reference Gainotti, Marra and Villa2001; Graham et al., Reference Graham, Emery and Hodges2004; Ingles et al., Reference Ingles, Boulton, Fisk and Rockwood2007; Looi & Sachdev, Reference Looi and Sachdev1999; Matsuda et al., Reference Matsuda, Saito and Sugishita1998; Mendez & Ashla-Mendez, Reference Mendez and Ashla-Mendez1991), whereas the latter appear to show a prevalence of executive dysfunction (Desmond, Reference Desmond2004; Jokinen et al., Reference Jokinen, Kalska, Mantyla, Ylikoski, Hietanen, Pohjasvaara, Kaste and Erkinjuntti2005; Kertesz & Clydesdale, Reference Kertesz and Clydesdale1994; Kramer et al., Reference Kramer, Reed, Mungas, Weiner and Chui2002; Looi & Sachdev, Reference Looi and Sachdev1999; Padovani et al., Reference Padovani, Di Piero, Dragoni, Iacoboni, Gualdi and Lenzi1995). Furthermore, psychomotor retardation, considered to be a prominent consequence of stroke (Ballard et al., Reference Ballard, Stephens, Kenny, Kalaria, Tovee and O'Brien2003; Rasquin et al., Reference Rasquin, Lodder, Ponds, Winkens, Jolles and Verhey2004) is also found in small-vessel subcortical dementia (Gainotti et al., Reference Gainotti, Marra and Villa2001), in vascular cognitive impairment (Desmond, Reference Desmond2004) and in association with recognized risk factors for vascular dementia, such as white matter hyperintensities (Jokinen et al., Reference Jokinen, Kalska, Mantyla, Ylikoski, Hietanen, Pohjasvaara, Kaste and Erkinjuntti2005). A further difference between DAT and VD could include phonological and semantic fluency, because some authors (e.g., Canning et al., Reference Canning, Leach, Stuss, Ngo and Black2004; Jones et al., Reference Jones, Laukka and Backman2006), but not others (e.g., Bentham et al., Reference Bentham, Jones and Hodges1997; Crossley et al., Reference Crossley, D'Arcy and Rawson1997) have found a greater semantic fluency impairment in DAT patients and a greater phonological fluency impairment in VD patients. On the basis of these findings, it should be logical to expect that different neuropsychological patterns would emerge during the preclinical stages of DAT and VD, respectively. However, the neuropsychological differentiation of DAT from VD is not endorsed by all authors. Almkvist et al. (Reference Almkvist, Backman, Basun and Wahlund1993), Meyer et al. (Reference Meyer, Xu, Thornby, Chowdhury and Quach2002) and Laukka et al. (Reference Laukka, Jones, Small, Fratiglioni and Backman2004) observed, for instance, a similar pattern of cognitive deficits in the preclinical phases of VD and DAT patients. In addition, Reed et al. (Reference Reed, Mungas, Kramer, Ellis, Vinters, Zarow, Jagust and Chui2007) recently reported that in patients with autopsy-defined DAT and cerebrovascular disease, respectively, executive impairment is not a useful diagnostic marker for VD. These observations suggest that ischemic factors may play an important role in the pathophysiology of both vascular and degenerative dementia (see de la Torre, Reference de la Torre2004a,Reference de la Torreb for recent reviews). This ischemic hypothesis is, however, at variance with several studies, most notably the “Nun study” (Snowdon et al., Reference Snowdon, Greiner, Mortimer, Riley, Greiner and Markesbery1997). This study showed that the presence of several subcortical infarcts does not increase the number of senile plaques or neurofibrillary tangles, suggesting that a chronic brain hypoperfusion is not a main determinant of these lesions.

In this study, we were interested in investigating the predictive accuracy of patterns of neuropsychological impairment, as well as other visual-spatial or episodic memory markers of dementia that have previously demonstrated good accuracy in distinguishing DAT from VD patients (Gainotti et al., Reference Gainotti, Parlato, Monteleone and Carlomagno1992) or DAT from other forms of dementia (Gainotti et al., Reference Gainotti, Marra, Villa, Parlato and Chiarotti1998). Therefore, the aim of our study was to evaluate whether different neuropsychological patterns and markers of dementia are apparent in patients with MCI resulting from multiple subcortical infarcts or from hippocampal atrophy, respectively. We advanced the following predictions: MCI patients with hippocampal atrophy (preclinical DAT) should show a predominance of episodic memory disorders, whereas MCI patients with multiple subcortical infarcts resulting from small-vessel disease (preclinical VD) should show significant executive dysfunction and other deficits related to frontal lobe damage, as well as psychomotor retardation. Furthermore, semantic fluency tasks should be more impaired in patients with preclinical DAT (hippocampal atrophy), whereas phonological fluency tasks should be more impaired in patients with preclinical VD (multiple subcortical infarcts). Finally, neuropsychological markers of dementia should be able to differentiate MCI patients with multiple subcortical infarcts from those with hippocampal atrophy.

MATERIAL AND METHODS

Patients

Over a 5-year period, approximately 500 patients were referred to our Neuropsychology Service for the onset of a mental deterioration. From this larger patient sample, we selected 104 patients who satisfied MCI criteria (see below) and who could be considered as preclinical DAT or preclinical VD on the basis of the brain MRI.

Criteria used to identify MCI patients

The clinical criteria suggested by Petersen et al. (Reference Petersen, Smith, Waring, Ivnik, Tangalos and Kokmen1999) were used to identify amnestic MCI patients. These criteria included (1) the presence of subjective memory complaints and objective memory impairment, (2) the absence of impairment in other cognitive domains, and (3) the absence of impairments in activities of daily living. Subjective memory complaints (documenting some form of awareness of the memory deficit) were expected to be confirmed by neuropsychological data. Specifically, at least two scores below cutoff points established for episodic long-term memory tasks (see the neuropsychological battery section) were required to identify objective memory impairment. Other cognitive domains were considered to be impaired only if they had been recognized by patients or by their relatives. Therefore, patients and their relatives were systematically asked whether the patient showed disturbances of language or of judgment, disorders of spatial orientation or difficulties in recognizing objects or persons. The diagnosis of MCI was made only if patients and their caregivers reported an absence of disturbance in these cognitive domains. The absence of functional impairment was systematically documented by reviewing ability to perform various daily living activities (ADL).

MRI criteria used to identify MCI patients with multiple subcortical infarcts or with hippocampal atrophy

All MCI patients underwent an MRI study in which two main criteria, namely: (1) the presence of subcortical brain infarcts less than 2 cm in size and (2) some degree of hippocampal atrophy, documented with hippocampal volumetry, were taken into account. Hippocampal volumetry was evaluated by means of the segmentation protocol proposed by Pruessner et al. (Reference Pruessner, Li, Serles, Pruessner, Collins, Kabani, Lupien and Evans2000) in a study conducted on 40 normal adults. This method is based on a manual segmentation of the hippocampus and on the use of a three-dimensional software that allows simultaneous analysis of sagittal, coronal and horizontal images. The pulse sequence used in the study was the basic sequence of the Siemens (MPRAGE), with acquisition parameters optimized to obtain a good GM/WM contrast and focused on the hippocampal structure. The images were analyzed in stereotactic space.

Because subjects studied by Pruessner et al. (Reference Pruessner, Li, Serles, Pruessner, Collins, Kabani, Lupien and Evans2000) were younger than our patients, and hippocampal volume decreases with advancing age in healthy adults, we adopted the cutoff points derived by a recent study of Lupien et al. (Reference Lupien, Evans, Lord, Miles, Pruessner, Pike and Pruessner2007). In this study, several age ranges were considered. The values of 2860 mm3 and 2240 mm3, corresponding respectively to the mean volumetric values minus two standard deviations for the age ranges of 60–75 and 76–85 were taken as cutoff points for the corresponding age groups of MCI patients.

We thought that, because we did not have a control group of normal elderly adults in our study, the most appropriate cutoff points would be 2 SD below the mean volumetric values of normal adults of corresponding age identified by Lupien et al. (Reference Lupien, Evans, Lord, Miles, Pruessner, Pike and Pruessner2007).

On the other hand, MCI patients with subcortical infarcts were identified using a criterion similar to that adopted in a previous personal work on subcortical vascular dementia (Gainotti et al., Reference Gainotti, Acciarri, Bizzarro, Marra, Masullo, Misciagna, Tartaglione, Valenza and Colosimo2004). MCI patients were included in this group if they presented three or more small subcortical infarcts or two small infarcts and periventricular white matter hyperintensities. The presence of periventricular white matter hyperintensities alone was not considered as a sufficient criterion to be included in the study, even if white matter hyperintensities are often associated with risk factors for vascular disease (Breteler et al., Reference Breteler, van Swieten, Bots, Grobbee, Claus, Vand Den Hout, Van Harskamp, Tanghe, De Jong, Van Gijn and Hofman1994; Longstreth et al., Reference Longstreth, Manolio, Arnold, Burke, Bryan, Jungreis, Enright, O'Leary and Fried1996). Patients with both hippocampal atrophy and subcortical infarcts were excluded from the study. On the basis of these strict MRI criteria, 41 MCI patients were included in the group with small subcortical infarcts (“vascular” group), 36 in the group with hippocampal atrophy (“atrophic” group), and 27 were excluded because they showed both vascular and atrophic lesions. A group of 65 normal controls, matched to MCI patients on age and educational level, were also included in the study. Both MCI patients and normal controls were administered the MMSE and an extensive neuropsychological test battery, that can be considered to be an extended version of the Mental Deterioration Battery (Carlesimo et al., Reference Carlesimo, Caltagirone and Gainotti1996). Data included in this manuscript were obtained in compliance with regulations of our ethics review committees, in accordance with the principles outlined in the Helsinki Declaration.

The Neuropsychological Battery

The neuropsychological battery was based on tests of episodic long-term verbal memory [Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT): immediate (IR) and delayed recall (DR) and delayed recognition], (Rey, Reference Rey and Rey1958), long-term visual-spatial memory [Rey-Osterreith Complex Figure delayed reproduction (ROCFR)] (Lezak, Reference Lezak1995), immediate verbal and visual-spatial memory (Digit and Spatial Span), limb and oral praxis, constructional praxis (Rey-Osterreith Complex Figure copy (ROCFC) and drawing designs with and without landmarks), phonological and semantic/categorical verbal fluency, and the Raven's Standard Progressive Matrices (Raven, Reference Raven1949), considered as a visual deductive reasoning task. Subjects were also given a group of tests sensitive to frontal lobe lesions, including a demanding attentional task (Multiple Features Targets Cancellation/MFTC) (Gainotti et al., Reference Gainotti, Marra and Villa2001), a test of temporal rule induction (Villa et al., Reference Villa, Gainotti, De Bonis and Marra1990) and the Stroop interference test (Perret, Reference Perret1974), each of which explores various facets of executive functions. In the MFTC, the patient is presented with an array of 80 small squares, each containing two variously oriented lines, and is asked to cancel as quickly as possible each of the 13 items that match a model placed immediately above the array, whereas in the “Temporal rule induction” the patient is asked to identify and predict the rule according to which binary sequences of red and blue tokens are presented. A further task, aiming to contrast relative impairments of the frontal and temporal lobes consisted of a naming task based both on objects and actions (Miceli et al., Reference Miceli, Laudanna, Burani and Papasso1994). In this task, the patient must name 30 drawings representing high frequency words (cigarette, radio, monkey, etc.) and 28 drawings representing simple actions (to run, to kiss, to cut, etc.), producing nouns and verbs, respectively. Cappa et al. (Reference Cappa, Binetti, Pezzini, Padovani, Rozzini and Trabucchi1998) demonstrated that frontal lobe dementia patients are more impaired naming actions than objects, whereas DAT patients show the opposite pattern of impairment. Similar results, namely a greater impairment in the generation of verbs than in the generation of nouns, have been obtained by Peran et al. (Reference Peran, Rascol, Démonet, Celsis, Nespoulous, Dubois and Cardebat2003, Reference Peran, Démonet, Pernet and Cardebat2004) in patients with “subcortical” forms of dementia (Huntington's disease and Parkinson's disease).

Timed tasks, namely phonological (PhF) and categorical verbal (CF) fluency tasks, the lines cancellation (LC) test (Albert, Reference Albert1973), the Multiple Features Targets Cancellation (MFTC) task and the naming time of the Stroop interference test were used to evaluate psychomotor speed. In addition to the scores obtained on the various tasks of the battery, we also took into account a certain number of neuropsychological markers, obtained on the Raven's Matrices, and on drawing designs with and without landmarks and in the RAVLT, that had demonstrated ability to distinguish DAT patients from VD patients and from persons with other forms of dementia (Gainotti et al., Reference Gainotti, Parlato, Monteleone and Carlomagno1992, Reference Gainotti, Marra, Villa, Parlato and Chiarotti1998). These markers are (1) the “closing-in” phenomenon on drawing tasks; (2) the tendency to give globalistic or “odd” responses on the Raven's Matrices; (3) the serial position effects (primacy, recency, and recency/primacy ratio) on the immediate recall trials of the RAVLT; (4) the decay of memory traces (absolute memory decay and saving score index) on the Delayed Recall of the RAVLT; (5) the number of false alarms on delayed recognition of the RAVLT. Scores obtained in the various tests and on the putative “neuropsychological markers” of dementia were used to evaluate whether vascular and degenerative forms of MCI can be distinguished on the basis of neuropsychological criteria.

Table 1 reports the main demographic (age, educational level, and gender) characteristics and the mean MMSE values of vascular and atrophic MCI patients and of normal controls.

Table 1. Demographic and MMSE comparisons among control subjects and vascular and atrophic MCI

No difference was found between normal controls and vascular or atrophic MCI patients with respect to demographic characteristics. The MMSE values of MCI patients were, as expected, only mildly impaired (though significantly worse than those obtained by normal controls) and were not significantly different in vascular and atrophic forms of MCI.

Statistics

Data were analyzed using multiple one-way ANOVAs, with the groups representing the independent variable and the neuropsychological variables constituting the neuropsychological tasks. Post hoc comparisons were carried out by means of multiple “Tukey tests for unequal sample size”. Because a large number of variables was analyzed, Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons was performed and a per-comparison α level of p = .002 was considered as significant. Frequency distributions of pathological scores and markers among MCI groups were analyzed by χ2 with Yates correction.

RESULTS

Patterns of Neuropsychological Impairment

To evaluate the patterns of neuropsychological impairment shown by atrophic and vascular forms of MCI, we compared mean scores obtained on the neuropsychological tests by normal controls and the two MCI patient categories, and we identified the frequency of pathological performance obtained on these tests by each MCI group.

Mean scores obtained by normal controls and vascular or atrophic MCI patients on the various tests of the Neuropsychological Battery

Table 2 reports the mean scores obtained by patients with vascular and atrophic forms of MCI and by normal controls on the neuropsychological tests, as well as the time taken to perform the timed tests.

Table 2. One-way ANOVA comparisons between vascular and atrophic MCI and Normal controls

* Bold values p correspond to significant differences among groups after Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons (p:.05 = p:.002).

Consistent with the MMSE data, results reported in Table 2 show that the severity of cognitive impairment of our vascular and atrophic MCI patients was quite mild overall, because most test scores were not significantly worse than those obtained by normal controls. As a group, MCI patients were more impaired than controls on episodic memory, naming, and time-dependent attentional tasks. Furthermore, when a comparison was made between atrophic and vascular MCI patients, the former group was strikingly more impaired on episodic memory tasks, and particularly on verbal (RAVLT) and visual-spatial (ROCFR) measures of delayed recall. In addition, atrophic MCI patients were marginally more impaired than the vascular patients with respect to the number of false alarms shown on the MFTC. On the other hand, vascular MCI patients were significantly more impaired only on a test sensitive to left frontal lesions, namely the “Action Naming” task. On some complex timed tests (namely MFCT − time and Stroop test − Interference time), both vascular and atrophic MCI patients were slower than normal controls, but not significantly different from each-other. Even if it is possible that different mechanisms could account for the slowing shown by vascular and atrophic forms of MCI, the time taken to perform the MFTC and the Stroop tests did not distinguish vascular from atrophic forms of MCI.

Frequency of pathological performances obtained on the tests of the Neuropsychological Battery by each MCI group

When the number of impaired scores obtained by each MCI group on the tests of the Neuropsychological Battery was considered, results similar to those observed when comparing the means of vascular and atrophic MCI patients were obtained. To demonstrate this comparison, we report the cutoff scores obtained in our control group in Table 3. These cutoff scores, set at 1.67 SD from the control means, corresponded to a statistically derived 5th percentile of the control group. The number of atrophic and vascular MCI patients scoring below these cutoff points on the various tests of the Neuropsychological Battery are also reported in Table 3.

Table 3. Frequency of impaired neuropsychological scores among MCI groups

* Cutoff scores were set at 1.67 SD from the control means, according to values corresponding to the 5th percentile of the control groups.

Data reported in Table 3 show that: (1) on most neuropsychological tasks, the proportion of impaired scores is rather low, both in vascular and in atrophic forms of MCI. (2) The proportion of atrophic MCI patients obtaining an impaired score on the verbal (RAVLT) delayed recall and on the visual-spatial (ROCFR) delayed reproduction is, however, very high (approximately 80%), and a rather high frequency of impaired scores is also observed on the Naming tasks. In particular, vascular MCI patients show a significantly higher number of impaired scores (60.9%) on the Action Naming task.

In the statistical comparison between atrophic and vascular MCI patients, the former group demonstrated a significantly greater number of impaired scores on the RAVLT DR, RAVLT Recognition–Accuracy and ROCFR recall, and also showed a significantly higher number of false alarms on the MFTC. On the other hand, a significantly higher number of vascular MCI patients obtained impaired scores on the Action naming task.

Neuropsychological Markers of MCI

Table 4 reports the mean scores (or the frequency of occurrence) of the neuropsychological markers observed in patients with vascular and atrophic forms of MCI. Table 4 also reports the results of the statistical analyses, which were carried out by means of t tests in the comparisons between mean scores and of Yates corrected χ2 in the comparisons between frequencies of occurrence.

Table 4. Comparisons of neuropsychological markers among MCI groups

Results reported in Table 4 are quite consistent with those observed in Tables 2 and 3 from two points of view: first, only the markers of long-term memory impairment (primacy effect; primacy minus recency and absolute decay or decay ratio) distinguished the atrophic from the vascular forms of MCI; second, the visual-spatial markers were very rare in both forms of MCI, suggesting that the severity of cognitive impairment was very mild in this sample.

The markers of memory decay on the RAVLT were particularly helpful in differentiating the two groups, because a complete loss of the memory trace in delayed recall was observed in a high percentage (33%) of atrophic, but only in very few (2.4%) vascular forms of MCI.

DISCUSSION

Results of our study suggest that a pattern of neuropsychological impairment mainly consisting of episodic memory deficits, is typical of MCI patients with hippocampal atrophy, whereas a different neuropsychological profile, consisting of impaired performance on the Action Naming task is observed in vascular forms of MCI. Two methodological features of the study allow us to maintain that these results are not due to confounding variables, such as the presence of mixed forms of MCI or of a different severity of cognitive impairment in the two matched groups. First, by including in the study only MCI patients whose MRI showed three or more subcortical infarcts, in the absence of hippocampal atrophy (“Vascular MCI”) or hippocampal atrophy in the absence of subcortical infarcts (“atrophic MCI”), we excluded all patients with both hippocampal atrophy and subcortical infarcts. Second, we ensured that the severity of cognitive impairment of our vascular and atrophic MCI patients was quite mild overall and that the two groups were well matched in terms of severity of impairment. The generally high MMSE scores and the comparability of mean scores obtained by both MCI groups on most neuropsychological measures confirm that our vascular and atrophic MCI patients were equally mildly impaired.

Returning from the methodological to the factual data, we can say that the severity of episodic memory impairment in MCI patients with hippocampal atrophy was consistently shown: (1) by the very low value of the mean scores obtained by these patients on the on the verbal (RAVLT) and visual-spatial (ROCFR) delayed recall tasks of the Neuropsychological Battery, (2) by the very high percentage of patients obtaining impaired scores on the same tasks, and (3) by results obtained when we took into account the neuropsychological markers drawn from the same tests. This pattern of impairment was expected on the basis of the neuropathological concomitants of DAT, because the first elementary lesions in this disease involve the entorhinal cortex and the hippocampus (Braak & Braak, Reference Braak and Braak1991), disconnecting the Papez circuit from information coming from the outside world and thus hindering the formation of new episodic memories. These data are also consistent with the recent results of van de Pol et al. (Reference Van de Pol, van der Flier, Korf, Fox, Barkhof and Scheltens2007), who showed that in MCI patients, reduced hippocampal volume at the baseline predicts subsequent accelerated rates of hippocampal atrophy, suggestive of the accumulation of Alzheimer-type pathology, which may clinically manifest itself in the future. This finding does not mean that episodic memory was impaired in isolation in our atrophic MCI patients, because executive functions, naming and performance on timed attentional tasks were also significantly impaired. An explanation of this observation, also framed in terms of the neuropathological staging of DAT, could be that the pathological process was spreading from the temporolimbic to the neocortical association areas in some of our atrophic MCI patients. If this interpretation is correct, during the follow-up of these patients, we should observe that the conversion to DAT should be more rapid and more frequent in patients with severe but not isolated impairment of memory functions. Unfortunately, the number of patients followed up for 1 or more years is, to date, insufficient to check this prediction.

If we pass, now, to the pattern of impairment observed in patients with Vascular MCI, we see that this pattern mainly reflects poor scores on a task sensitive to frontal lobe damage (such as the Action Naming task), but is less salient than that observed in degenerative forms of MCI. This neuropsychological marker seen in our vascular MCI patients, suggesting a likely frontal lobe involvement, is consistent with the hypothesis assuming that small vessel subcortical dementia may be due to the disruption of corticostriatal loops that subserve the functions of the frontal lobes, coursing through the frontal white matter (Alexander & Crutcher, Reference Alexander and Crutcher1990; Cummings, Reference Cummings1993). The fact that a greater impairment in the generation of verbs than in the generation of nouns has been reported by Peran et al. (Reference Peran, Rascol, Démonet, Celsis, Nespoulous, Dubois and Cardebat2003, Reference Peran, Démonet, Pernet and Cardebat2004) in patients with other “subcortical” forms of dementia (Huntington's Disease and Parkinson's Disease) also supports this interpretation.

The greater saliency of the neuropsychological pattern of impairment shown by atrophic MCI patients could be due to three main reasons: (1) the distribution of lesions in vascular MCI patients is rather scattered, being due to disruption of different components of the above mentioned corticostriatal loops, such as the basal ganglia, the thalamus, and the white matter tracts connecting the frontal cortex with these subcortical nuclei (Alexander & Crutcher, Reference Alexander and Crutcher1990; Cummings, Reference Cummings1993); (2) the nonhomogeneous nature of the cognitive deficits observed in patients with frontal lobe lesions (Dubois et al., Reference Dubois, Slachevsky, Litvan and Pillon2000; Shallice & Burgess, Reference Shallice and Burgess1991); (3) the observation that in patients with nondegenerative forms of MCI, the pattern of cognitive impairment is less consistent and more fluctuating in time. Lopez et al. (Reference Lopez, Kuller, Becker, Dulberg, Sweet, Gach and DeKosky2007) have, indeed, rightly noted that when the subject's cognitive deficits could be due to nondegenerative conditions (such as a small-vessel subcortical pathology), a greater proportion of MCI patients remain cognitively stable or return to a normal baseline. These reasons could contribute to explain the conflicting results of previous studies dealing with the patterns of cognitive impairment observed in VD and DAT patients (e.g., Desmond, Reference Desmond2004; Laukka et al., Reference Laukka, Jones, Small, Fratiglioni and Backman2004; Looi & Sachdev, Reference Looi and Sachdev1999; Meyer et al., Reference Meyer, Xu, Thornby, Chowdhury and Quach2002; Reed et al., Reference Reed, Mungas, Kramer, Ellis, Vinters, Zarow, Jagust and Chui2007). It is also possible that the absence of a clear neuropsychological pattern in vascular MCI patients was due to their small sample size (and hence to insufficient statistical power) but this hypothesis is at variance with the fact that a clear and consistent neuropsychological pattern was found in the even smaller group of atrophic MCI patients. In any case, our findings in patients with atrophic and vascular forms of MCI are consistent with results recently obtained by Reed et al. (Reference Reed, Mungas, Kramer, Ellis, Vinters, Zarow, Jagust and Chui2007) in DAT and VD patients with autopsy-defined pathologies, because both studies show that a severe memory impairment strongly points to an Alzheimer's type brain pathology, whereas the prevalence of executive deficits, other frontal lobe symptoms or psychomotor slowing are a much weaker diagnostic marker of small vessel subcortical cerebrovascular disease.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We thank Drs. T. Tartaglione and F. Tomaiolo for their precious contribution in the identification of MCI patients showing multiple subcortical infarcts or a hippocampal atrophy. We also want to express our extreme gratitude to Dr. John L. Woodard for his very careful and time consuming editing assistance, under the auspices of the Research Editing Consultant Program of the International Neuropsychological Society International Liaison Committee. We are very indebted to him, because he improved the clarity and the style of our study.

References

REFERENCES

Albert, M.L. (1973). A simple test of visual neglect. Neurology, 23, 658664.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alexander, G.E. & Crutcher, M.D. (1990). Functional architecture of the basal ganglia circuits: Neural substrates of parallel processing. Trends in Neurosciences, 13, 266271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Almkvist, O., Backman, L., Basun, H., & Wahlund, L-O. (1993). Patterns of neuropsychological performance in Alzheimer's disease and vascular dementia. Cortex, 29, 661673.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ballard, C., Stephens, S., Kenny, R., Kalaria, R., Tovee, M., & O'Brien, J. (2003). Profile of neuropsychological deficits in older stroke survivors without dementia. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 16, 5256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bentham, P.W., Jones, S., & Hodges, J.R. (1997). A comparison of semantic memory in vascular dementia and dementia of the Alzheimer's type. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 12, 575580.3.0.CO;2-N>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braak, H. & Braak, E. (1991). Neuropathological staging of Alzheimer's related changes. Acta Neuropathologica, 82, 239259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breteler, M.M., van Swieten, J.C., Bots, M.L., Grobbee, D.E., Claus, J.J., Vand Den Hout, J.H.W., Van Harskamp, F., Tanghe, H.L.J., De Jong, P.T.V.M., Van Gijn, J., & Hofman, A. (1994). Cerebral white matter lesions, vascular risk factors, and cognitive function in a population-based study: The Rotterdam study. Neurology, 44, 12461252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Canning, S.J., Leach, L., Stuss, D., Ngo, L., & Black, S.E. (2004). Diagnostic utility of abbreviated fluency measures in Alzheimer's disease and vascular dementia. Neurology, 62, 556562.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cappa, S.F., Binetti, G., Pezzini, A., Padovani, A., Rozzini, L., & Trabucchi, M. (1998). Object and action naming in Alzheimer's disease and fronto-temporal dementia. Neurology, 50, 351355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlesimo, G.A., Caltagirone, C., Gainotti, G., & the Group for the Standardization of the Mental Deterioration Battery. (1996). The Mental Deterioration Battery: Normative data, diagnostic reliability and qualitative analyses of cognitive impairment. European Neurology, 36, 378384.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crossley, M., D'Arcy, C., & Rawson, N.S. (1997). Letter and category fluency in community dwelling Canadian seniors: A comparison of normal participants to those with dementia of the Alzheimer or vascular type. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 19, 5262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cummings, J.L. (1993). Frontal-subcortical circuits and human behaviour. Archives of Neurology, 50, 5667.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de la Torre, J.C. (2004a). Is Alzheimer's disease a neurodegenerative or a vascular disorder? Data, dogma, and dialectics [Review]. Lancet Neurology, 3, 184190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de la Torre, J.C. (2004b). Alzheimer's disease is a vasocognopathy: A new term to describe its nature [Review]. Neurological Research, 26, 517524.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Desmond, D.W. (2004). The neuropsychology of vascular cognitive impairment: Is there a specific cognitive deficit? Journal of Neurological Science, 226, 37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dubois, B. & Albert, M.L. (2004). Amnestic MCI or prodromal Alzheimer's disease? Lancet Neurology, 3, 246248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dubois, B., Slachevsky, A., Litvan, I., & Pillon, B. (2000). The FAB: A frontal assessment battery at bedside. Neurology, 55, 16211666.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gainotti, G., Acciarri, A., Bizzarro, A., Marra, C., Masullo, C., Misciagna, S., Tartaglione, T., Valenza, A., & Colosimo, C. (2004). The role of brain infarcts and hippocampal atrophy in subcortical ischaemic vascular dementia. Neurological Sciences, 25, 192197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gainotti, G., Marra, C., & Villa, G. (2001). A double dissociation between accuracy and time of execution on attentional tasks in Alzheimer's disease and multi-infarct dementia. Brain, 124, 731738.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gainotti, G., Marra, C., Villa, G., Parlato, V., & Chiarotti, F. (1998). Sensitivity and specificity of some neuropsychological markers of Alzheimer dementia. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 12, 152162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gainotti, G., Monteleone, D., Parlato, V., & Carlomagno, S. (1989). Verbal memory disorders in Alzheimer's disease and multi-infarct dementia. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 3–4, 327345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gainotti, G., Parlato, V., Monteleone, D., & Carlomagno, S. (1992). Neuropsychological markers of dementia on visual-spatial tasks: A comparison between Alzheimer's type and vascular forms of dementia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 2, 239252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graham, N.L., Emery, T., & Hodges, J.R. (2004). Distinctive cognitive profiles in Alzheimer's disease and subcortical vascular dementia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 75, 6171.Google ScholarPubMed
Ingles, J.L., Boulton, D.C., Fisk, J.D., & Rockwood, K. (2007). Preclinical vascular cognitive impairment and Alzheimer disease: Neuropsychological test performance 5 years before diagnosis. Stroke, 38, 11481153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jokinen, H., Kalska, H., Mantyla, R., Ylikoski, R., Hietanen, M., Pohjasvaara, T., Kaste, M., & Erkinjuntti, T. (2005). White matter hyperintensities as a predictor of neuropsychological deficits post-stroke. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 76, 12291233.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, S., Laukka, E.J., & Backman, L. (2006). Differential verbal fluency deficits in the preclinical stages of Alzheimer's disease and vascular dementia. Cortex, 42, 347355.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kertesz, A. & Clydesdale, S. (1994). Neuropsychological deficits in vascular dementia vs Alzheimer's disease. Frontal lobe deficits prominent in vascular dementia. Archives of Neurology, 51, 12261231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kramer, J.H., Reed, B.R., Mungas, D., Weiner, M.W., & Chui, H.C. (2002). Executive dysfunction in subcortical ischaemic vascular disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 72, 217220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laukka, E.J., Jones, S., Small, B.J., Fratiglioni, L., & Backman, L. (2004). Similar patterns of cognitive deficits in the preclinical phases of vascular dementia and Alzheimer's disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10, 382391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lezak, M.D. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment (3rd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Longstreth, W.T., Manolio, T.A., Arnold, A., Burke, G.L., Bryan, N., Jungreis, C.A., Enright, P.L., O'Leary, D., & Fried, L. (1996). Clinical correlates of white matter findings on cranial magnetic resonance imaging of 3301 elderly people: The cardiovascular health study. Stroke, 27, 12741282.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Looi, J.C. & Sachdev, P.S. (1999). Differentiation of vascular dementia from AD on neuropsychological tests. Neurology, 53, 670678.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lopez, O.L., Becker, J.T., Jagust, W.J., Fitzpatrick, A., Carlson, M.C., DeKosky, S.T., Breitner, J., Lyketos, C.G., Jones, B., Kawas, C., & Kuller, L.H. (2005). Neuropsychological characteristics of mild cognitive impairment subgroups. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 77, 159165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lopez, O.L., Kuller, L.H., Becker, J.T., Dulberg, C., Sweet, R.A., Gach, H.M., & DeKosky, S.T. (2007). Incidence of dementia in mild cognitive impairment in the cardiovascular health study cognition study. Archives of Neurology, 64, 416420.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lupien, S.J., Evans, A., Lord, C., Miles, J., Pruessner, M., Pike, B., & Pruessner, J.C. (2007). Hippocampal volume is as variable in young as in older adults: Implications for the notion of hippocampal atrophy in humans. Neuroimage, 34, 479485.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matsuda, O., Saito, M., & Sugishita, M. (1998). Cognitive deficits of mild dementia: A comparison between dementia of the Alzheimer's type and vascular dementia. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 52, 8791.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mendez, M.F. & Ashla-Mendez, M. (1991). Differences between multi-infarct dementia and Alzheimer's disease on unstructured neuropsychological tasks. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 13, 923932.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyer, J.S., Xu, G., Thornby, J., Chowdhury, M.H., & Quach, M. (2002). Is mild cognitive impairment prodromal for vascular dementia like Alzheimer's disease? Stroke, 33, 19811984.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miceli, G., Laudanna, A., Burani, C., & Papasso, R. (1994). Batteria per l'analisi dei deficit afasici (BADA). CEPSAG, UCSC, Roma.Google Scholar
Nordlund, A., Rolstad, S., Hellstrom, P., Sjogrem, L., Hansen, S., & Wallin, A. (2005). The Goteborg MCI study: Mild cognitive impairment is a heterogeneous condition. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 76, 14851490.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Padovani, A., Di Piero, V., Dragoni, M., Iacoboni, M., Gualdi, G.F., & Lenzi, G.L. (1995). Patterns of neuropsychological impairment in mild dementia: A comparison between Alzheimer's disease and multi-infarct dementia. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 92, 433442.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peran, P., Démonet, J.F., Pernet, C., & Cardebat, D. (2004). Verb and noun generation tasks in Huntington's disease. Movement Disorders, 19, 565571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peran, P., Rascol, O., Démonet, J.F., Celsis, P., Nespoulous, J.L., Dubois, B., & Cardebat, D. (2003). Defisit of verb generation in nondemented patients with Parkinson's disease. Movement Disorders, 18, 150156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perret, E. (1974). The left frontal lobe of man and the suppression of habitual responses in verbal categorical behaviour. Neuropsychologia, 12, 323330.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, R.C., Doody, R., Kurz, A., Mohs, R.C., Morris, J.C., Rabins, P.V., Ritchie, K., Rossor, M., Thal, L., & Winbland, B. (2001). Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment. Archives of Neurology, 58, 19851992.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, R.C., Smith, G.E., Waring, S.C., Ivnik, R.J., Tangalos, E.G., & Kokmen, E. (1999). Mild cognitive impairment: Clinical characterization and outcome. Archives of Neurology, 56, 303308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pruessner, J.C., Li, L.M., Serles, W., Pruessner, M., Collins, D.L., Kabani, N., Lupien, S., & Evans, A.C. (2000). Volumetry of hippocampus and amygdala with high-resolution MRI and three-dimensional analysis software: Minimizing the discrepancies between laboratories. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 433442.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rasquin, S.M., Lodder, J., Ponds, R.W., Winkens, J., Jolles, J., & Verhey, F.R. (2004). Cognitive functioning after stroke: A one-year follow-up study. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 18, 138144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rasquin, S.M., Lodder, J., Visser, P.J., Lousberg, R., & Verhey, F.R. (2005). Predictive accuracy of MCI subtypes for Alzheimer's disease and vascular dementia in subjects with mild cognitive impairment: A 2-year follow-up study. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 19, 113119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raven, JC. (1949). Progressive matrices '47, Sets A, Ab, B: Board and book forms. London: Lewis.Google Scholar
Reed, B.R., Mungas, D.M., Kramer, J.H., Ellis, W., Vinters, H.V., Zarow, C., Jagust, W.J., & Chui, H.C. (2007). Profiles of neuropsychological impairment in autopsy-defined Alzheimer's disease and cerebrovascular disease. Brain, 130, 731739.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rey, A. (1958). Mémorisation d'une série de 15 mots en 5 rèpètitions. In Rey, A. (Ed.), L'examen clinique en psychologie. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.Google Scholar
Ritchie, K. & Touchon, J. (2000). Mild cognitive impairment: Conceptual basis and current nosological status. Lancet, 355, 225228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shah, Y., Tangalos, E.G., & Petersen, R.C. (2000). Mild cognitive impairment. When is it a precursor to Alzheimer's disease [Review]? Geriatrics, 55, 62, 65–68.Google ScholarPubMed
Shallice, T. & Burgess, P.W. (1991). Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man. Brain, 114, 727741.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snowdon, D.A., Greiner, L.H., Mortimer, J.A., Riley, K.P., Greiner, P.A., & Markesbery, W.R. (1997). Brain infarction and the clinical expression of Alzheimer disease. The Nun Study. JAMA, 277, 813817.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van de Pol, L.A., van der Flier, W.M., Korf, E.S.C., Fox, N.C., Barkhof, F., & Scheltens, P. (2007). Baseline predictors of rates of hippocampal atrophy in mild cognitive impairment. Neurology, 69, 14911497.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Villa, G., Gainotti, G., De Bonis, C., & Marra, C. (1990). Double dissociation between temporal and spatial pattern processing in patients with frontal and parietal damage. Cortex, 26, 399407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic and MMSE comparisons among control subjects and vascular and atrophic MCI

Figure 1

Table 2. One-way ANOVA comparisons between vascular and atrophic MCI and Normal controls

Figure 2

Table 3. Frequency of impaired neuropsychological scores among MCI groups

Figure 3

Table 4. Comparisons of neuropsychological markers among MCI groups