Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T23:38:59.387Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Patients with Parkinson's disease can successfully remember to execute delayed intentions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 August 2007

MAREIKE ALTGASSEN
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
JACQUELINE ZÖLLIG
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
UTE KOPP
Affiliation:
Klinik für Neurologie, Charité, Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Berlin, Germany
RACHAEL MACKINLAY
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
MATTHIAS KLIEGEL
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland

Abstract

The present study investigated prospective memory in patients with Parkinson's disease (PD) and healthy controls. In addition, the influence of task importance on participants' performance was examined. Experimental settings required participants to focus either on the prospective or the ongoing task. The three main findings are (1) PD patients performed as well on a prospective memory task as healthy controls when the focus was laid on the prospective memory task, (2) their prospective memory performance was impaired when the ongoing activity was stressed, and (3) differences in working memory capacity were related to these differential effects. Results indicate that PD patients can perform event-based prospective memory tasks to a normal degree if the prospective task component is prioritized. Data also suggest that a reduced working memory capacity plays an important role in this process. Findings are discussed in terms of conceptual, methodological, and clinical implications. (JINS, 2007, 13, 888–892.)

Type
BRIEF COMMUNICATION
Copyright
2007 The International Neuropsychological Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Brandimonte, M., Einstein, G.O., & McDaniel, M.A. (1996). Prospective memory: Theory and applications. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Einstein, G.O., Holland, L.J., McDaniel, M.A., & Guynn, M.J. (1992). Age-related deficits in prospective memory: The influence of task complexity. Psychology and Aging, 7, 471478.Google Scholar
Goschke, T. & Kuhl, J. (1993). Representation of intentions: Persisting activation in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 12111226.Google Scholar
Higginson, C.I., King, D.S., Levine, D., Wheelock, V.L., Khamphay, N.O., & Sigvardt, K.A. (2003). The relationship between executive function and verbal memory in Parkinson's disease. Brain and Cognition, 52, 343352.Google Scholar
Katai, S., Maruyama, T., Hashimoto, T., & Ikeda, S. (2003). Event based and time based prospective memory in Parkinson's disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 74, 704709.Google Scholar
Kliegel, M. & Martin, M. (2003). Mechanisms underlying motivational effects in prospective remembering. In S.P. Shohov (Ed.), Advances in psychology research. Huntington, NY: Nova Science Publishers.
Kliegel, M., Martin, M., McDaniel, M.A., & Einstein, G.O. (2001). Varying the importance of a prospective memory task: Differential effects across time- and event-based prospective memory. Memory, 9, 111.Google Scholar
Kliegel, M., McDaniel, M.A., & Einstein, G.O. (2007, in press). Prospective memory: Cognitive, neuroscience, developmental, and applied perspectives. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kliegel, M., Phillips, L., & Fischer, C. (2004, April). Importance effects on age differences in performance in event-based prospective memory. Atlanta, GA: 10th Cognitive Aging Conference.
Kliegel, M., Phillips, L.H., Lemke, U., & Kopp, U.A. (2005). Planning and realisation of complex intentions in patients with Parkinson's disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 76, 15011505.Google Scholar
Lehrl, S., Merz, J., & Burkhard, G.F.S. (1991). Der Mehrfachwahlwortschatztest (MWT-B). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.
Lewis, S.J.G., Cools, R., Robbins, T.W., Dove, A., Barker, R.A., & Owen, A.M. (2003). Using executive heterogeneity to explore the nature of working memory deficits in Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychologia, 41, 645654.Google Scholar
Martin, M., Kliegel, M., & McDaniel, M.A. (2003). The involvement of executive functions in prospective memory performance of adults. International Journal of Psychology, 38, 195206.Google Scholar
McDaniel, M.A., Guynn, M.J., Einstein, G.O., & Breneiser, J. (2004). Cue-focused and reflexive-associative processes in prospective memory retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 605614.Google Scholar
Muslimovic, D., Post, B., Speelman, J.D., & Schmand, B. (2005). Cognitive profile of patients with newly diagnosed Parkinson disease. Neurology, 65, 12391245.Google Scholar
Plohmann, A.M., Kappos, L., Ammann, W., Thordai, A., Wittwer, A., Huber, S., Bellaiche, Y., & Lechner-Scott, J. (1998). Computer assisted retraining of attentional impairments in patients with multiple sclerosis. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 64, 455462.Google Scholar
Salthouse, T.A., Berish, D.E., & Siedlecki, K.L. (2004). Construct validity and age sensitivity of prospective memory. Memory and Cognition, 32, 11331148.Google Scholar
Smith, R.E. & Bayen, U.J. (2004). A multinomial model of event-based prospective memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 756777.Google Scholar
Sommerville, S.C., Wellmann, H.M., & Cultice, J.C. (1983). Young children's deliberate reminding. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 143, 8796.Google Scholar
Wang, L., Kliegel, M., Yang, Z., & Liu, W. (2006). Prospective memory performance across adolescence. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 167, 179188.Google Scholar
West, R., Bowry, R., & Krompinger, J. (2006). The effects of working memory demands on the neural correlates of prospective memory. Neuropsychologia, 44, 197207.Google Scholar
Whittington, C.J., Podd, J., & Kan, M.M. (2000). Recognition memory in Parkinson's disease: Power and meta-analyses. Neuropsychology, 14, 233246.Google Scholar
Woods, S.P. & Troster, A.I. (2003). Prodromal frontal/executive dysfunction predicts incident dementia in Parkinson's disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 9, 1724.Google Scholar
Zimmermann, P. & Fimm, B. (2002). Testbatterie zur Aufmerksamkeitsprüfung, Version 1.7. Herzogenrath, Germany: Vera Fimm.