Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-r5fsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T19:17:53.313Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Nonliteral Language in Alzheimer Dementia: A Review

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 January 2011

Alexander M. Rapp*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Tuebingen, Tuebingen, Germany
Barbara Wild
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Tuebingen, Tuebingen, Germany
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Alexander M. Rapp, MD, Department of Psychiatry, University of Tuebingen, Osianderstrasse 26, 72076 Tuebingen, Germany. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

The use of nonliteral language in clinical assessment, especially testing the patients’ ability to interpret proverbs, has a long tradition in psychiatry. However, its diagnostic sensitivity and specificity in dementias is not yet clear. The aim of this review article is to examine the current evidence on nonliteral/figurative language (proverb, metaphor, metonymy, idiom, irony, sarcasm) comprehension in Alzheimer's disease and related disorders. A comprehensive literature search identified 25 studies (16 proverb, 3 metaphor, 0 metonymy, 5 idiom, 3 sarcasm) on nonliteral language comprehension in dementia. Studies predominantly indicate a deficit. Most studies investigated Alzheimer's dementia. Applied correctly, nonliteral language is a worthwhile diagnostic tool to evaluate language and abstract thinking in dementias. During assessment, familiarity testing (e.g., by asking “are you familiar with the proverb XY”) is obligatory. Still, future research is needed in several areas: evidence on decline of nonliteral language over the course of the illness is limited. So far, almost no studies delineated proverb comprehension in high risk populations such as patients with mild cognitive impairment. Currently, there is a lack of studies addressing performance in direct comparison to relevant differential diagnosis like older-age depression, delirium, brain lesion, or other psychiatric conditions. (JINS, 2011, 17, 207–218)

Type
Critical Review
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Ahrens, K., Liu, H., Lee, C., Gong, S., Fang, S., Hsu, Y.Y. (2007). Functional MRI of conventional and anomalous metaphors in Mandarin Chinese. Brain and Language, 100, 163171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amanzio, M., Geminiani, G., Leotta, D., Cappa, S. (2008). Metaphor comprehension in Alzheimer's disease: Novelty matters. Brain and Language, 107, 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anaki, D., Faust, M., Kravetz, S. (1998). Cerebral hemispheric asymmetries in processing lexical metaphors. Neuropsychologia, 36, 691700.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andreasen, N.C. (1977). Reliability and validity of proverb interpretation to assess mental status. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 18(5), 465472.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Annaz, D., Van Herwegen, J., Thomas, M., Fishman, R., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Rundblad, G. (2009). Comprehension of metaphor and metonymy in children with Williams syndrome. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 44(6), 962978.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Báez, S., Mendoza, L., Reyes, P., Matallana, D., Montañés, P. (2009). Interpretación de refranes y enfermedad de Alzheimer. Revista de Neurologia, 49(11), 566572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barth, A., Küfferle, B. (2001). Die Entwicklung eines Sprichworttestes zur Erfassung konkretistischer Denkstörungen bei schizophrenen Patienten. Nervenarzt, 72, 853858.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blasko, D.G., Kazmerski, V.A. (2006). ERP correlates of individual differences in the comprehension of nonliteral language. Metaphor and Symbol, 21(4), 267284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benjamin, J.D. (1944). A method for distinguishing and evaluating formal thinking disorders in schizophrenia. In J. Kasanin (Ed.), Language and thought in schizophrenia (pp. 6590). Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Brattemo, C.E. (1962). Interpretations of proverbs in schizophrenic patients: Further studies. Acta Psychologica, 20(3), 254263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brundage, S.B. (1993). Comparison of proverb interpretations provided by non-brain-damaged adults, aphasic adults, right-hemisphere-damaged adults, and adults with probable dementia. Dissertation, University of Minnesota.Google Scholar
Brundage, S.B., Brookshire, R.H. (1995). A system for scoring proverb interpretations provided by non-brain-damaged adults and aphasic adults. Clinical Aphasiology, 23, 165177.Google Scholar
Burgess, C., Chiarello, C. (1996). Neurocognitive mechanisms underlying metaphor comprehension and other figurative language. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 11, 6784.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burgos, L. (1986). Inter-rater reliability and the use of proverb interpretation in the detection of disordered thinking. Dissertation Abstracts International, 46(7), 2453B.Google Scholar
Campanha, A.C., Lira, J.O., Diniz, A., Ortiz, K.Z., Bertolucci, P.H.F., Minett, T. (2008). IC-P1-007: Performance comparison of Alzheimer disease and healthy subjects in interpretation of popular proverbs. Alzheimer's & Dementia, 4(Suppl.), T12.Google Scholar
Campbell, K.M., Schubert, D.S. (1992). Reversal of delirium associated with essential thrombocythemia by antiplatelet therapy. Psychosomatics, 33(2), 213215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carter, M.L. (1986). The assessment of thought deficit in psychotic unipolar depression and chronic paranoid schizophrenia. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 174(6), 336341.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Channon, S., Pellijeff, A., Rule, A. (2005). Social cognition after head injury: Sarcasm and theory of mind. Brain and Language, 93(2), 123134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Channon, S., Rule, A., Maudgil, D., Martinos, M., Pellijeff, A., Frankl, J., Shieff, C. (2007). Interpretation of mentalistic actions and sarcastic remarks: Effects of frontal and posterior lesions on mentalising. Neuropsychologia, 45(8), 17251734.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chapman, S.B., Highley, A.P., Thompson, J.L. (1998). Discourse in fluent aphasia and Alzheimer's disease: Linguistic and pragmatic considerations. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 11(1–2), 5578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chapman, S.B., Ulatowska, H.K., Franklin, L.R., Shobe, A.E., Thompson, J.L., McIntire, D.D. (1997). Proverb interpretation in fluent aphasia and Alzheimer's disease: Implications beyond abstract thinking. Aphasiology, 11(4–5), 337350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, E., Widick, P., Chatterjee, A. (2008). Functional-anatomical organization of predicate metaphor processing. Brain and Language, 107, 194202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Code, C., Lodge, B. (1987). Language in dementia of recent referral. Age and Ageing, 16, 366372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colston, H.L., Gibbs, R.W. Jr. (2002). Are irony and metaphor understood differently? Metaphor and Symbol, 17(1), 5780.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cuerva, A.G., Sabe, L., Kuzis, G., Tiberti, C., Dorrego, F., Starkstein, S.E. (2001). Theory of mind and pragmatic abilities in dementia. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychology and Behavioral Neurology, 14, 153158.Google ScholarPubMed
Davis, G.A. (2007). Cognitive pragmatics of language disorders in adults. Seminars in Speech and Language, 28(2), 111121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delis, D.C., Kaplan, E., Kramer, J.H. (2001). Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Delis, D.C., Kramer, J., Kaplan, E. 1984. The California proverb test. Unpublished protocol.Google Scholar
Elmore, C.M., Gorham, D.R. (1957). Measuring the impairment of the abstracting function with the proverbs test. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 13, 263266.3.0.CO;2-C>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Engel, G.L., Romano, J. (1959). Delirium, a syndrome of cerebral insufficiency. Journal of Chronic Diseases, 9, 260277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eviatar, Z., Just, M.A. (2006). Brain correlates of discourse processing: An fMRI investigation of irony and conventional metaphor comprehension. Neuropsychologia, 44(12), 23482359.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Faust, M., Mashal, N. (2007). The role of the right cerebral hemisphere in processing novel metaphoric expressions taken from poetry: A divided visual field study. Neuropsychologia, 45(4), 860870.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farrar, C.B. (1906). The making of psychiatric records. American Journal of Psychiatry, 62, 479509.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Filippova, E., Astington, J.W. (2008). Further development in social reasoning revealed in discourse irony understanding. Child Development, 79(1), 126138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Finckh, J. (1906). Zur Frage der Intelligenzprüfung. Zentralblatt für Nervenheilkunde und Psychiatrie, 29, 945957.Google Scholar
Fogel, M.L. (1965). The proverbs test in the appraisal of cerebral disease. The Journal of General Psychology, 72, 269275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Folstein, M.F., Folstein, S.E., McHugh, P.R. (1975). Mini-Mental State: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gagnon, L., Goulet, P., Giroux, F., Joanette, Y. (2003). Processing of metaphoric and non-metaphoric alternative meanings of words after right- and left-hemispheric lesion. Brain and Language, 87, 217226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibbs, R.W. Jr. (1980). Spilling the beans on understanding and memory for idioms in conversation. Memory and Cognition, 8, 149156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giora, R., Zaidel, E., Soroker, N., Batori, G., Kasher, A. (2000). Differential effects of right- and left-hemisphere damage on understanding sarcasm and metaphor. Metaphor and Symbol, 15(1&2), 6383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giora, R. (2003). On our mind: Salience, context, and figurative language. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giora, R. (2007). Is metaphor special? Brain and Language, 100(2), 111114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glucksberg, S. (2003). The psycholinguistics of metaphor. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(2), 9296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldstein, R.H., Salzman, L.F. (1965). Proverb word counts as a measure of overinclusiveness in delusional schizophrenics. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 70(4), 244245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gorham, D.R. (1956). Use of the proverbs test for differentiating schizophrenics from normals. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 20, 435440.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gorham, D.R. (1961). Verbal abstraction in psychiatric illness: Assay of impairment utilizing proverbs. Journal of Mental Science, 107, 5259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hadlich, H. (1931). Schizophrene Denkstörung. Psychologische Forschung, 15, 359373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Happé, F.G. (1996). Understanding minds and metaphors: Insights from the study of figurative language in autism. Metaphor & Symbol, 10, 275295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haynes, R.M., Resnick, P.J., Dougherty, K.C., Althof, S.E. (1993). Proverb familiarity and the mental status examination. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 57(4), 523528.Google ScholarPubMed
Heinik, J., Aharon-Peretz, J. (1993). Abstract thinking in dementia: A preliminary report. Clinical Gerontologist, 12(4), 5761.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hensler, M. (2009). Sind konkretistische Denkstörungen eine homogene Entität? Untersuchungen zum Verständnis nicht-wörtlicher Sprache bei schizophrenen Patienten. Thesis, University of Tuebingen, Germany. http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bsz:21-opus-40670Google Scholar
Hillekamp, U., Knobloch, J., Buelau, P. (1996). Metaphorische Sprachverarbeitung bei Hirngeschädigten: Anwendung und Analyse eines Metapherntests. Neurologie & Rehabilitation, 4, 232236.Google Scholar
Hillert, D.G., Buracas, G.T. (2009). The neural substrates of spoken idiom comprehension. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24(9), 13701391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horowitz, L.M., Manelis, L. (1973). Recognition and cued recall of idioms and phrases. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 100, 291296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iakimova, G., Passerieux, C., Hardy-Baylé, M.C. (2006). The understanding of metaphors in schizophrenia and depression. An experimental approach. Encephale, 32(6 Pt 1), 9951002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jäger, A.O., Althoff, K. (1983). WIT: Der Wilde- Intelligenz- Test. Ein Strukturdiagnostikum. Göttingen: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Kacinik, N.A., Chiarello, C. (2007). Understanding metaphoric language: Is the right hemisphere uniquely involved? Brain and Language, 100, 188207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Katz, A.N., Blasko, D.G., Kazmerski, V.A. (2004). Saying what you don't mean: Social influences on sarcastic language processing. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13(5), 186189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kempler, D., Van Lancker, D., Read, S. (1988). Proverb and idiom comprehension in Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 2(1), 3849.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kipps, C.M., Nestor, P.J., Acosta-Cabronero, J., Arnold, R., Hodges, J.R. (2009). Understanding social dysfunction in the behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia: The role of emotion and sarcasm processing. Brain, 132(Pt 3), 592603.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kosmidis, M.H., Aretouli, E., Bozikas, V.P., Giannakou, M., Ioannidis, P. (2008). Studying social cognition in patients with schizophrenia and patients with frontotemporal dementia: Theory of mind and the perception of sarcasm. Behavioural Neurology, 19(1–2), 6569.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lafleche, G., Albert, M. (1995). Executive functions in mild Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychology, 9(3), 313320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G., Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lieberman, P. (1963). Some effects of semantic and grammatic context on the production and perception of speech. Language and Speech, 6, 172187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacKenzie, C., Begg, T., Brady, M., Lees, K.R. (1997). The effects on verbal communication skills of right hemisphere stroke in middle age. Aphasiology, 11, 929945.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, I., McDonald, S. (2005). Exploring the causes of pragmatic language deficits following traumatic brain injury. Aphasiology, 19, 712730.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mashal, N., Faust, M., Hendler, T., Jung-Beeman, M. (2005). The role of the right hemisphere in processing nonsalient metaphorical meanings: Application of principal component analysis to fMRI data. Neuropsychologia, 43, 20842100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDonald, S., Bornhofen, C., Shum, D., Long, E., Saunders, C., Neulinger, K. (2006). Reliability and validity of The Awareness of Social Inference Test (TASIT): A clinical test of social perception. Disability and Rehabilitation, 28(24), 15291542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDonald, S., Flanagan, S., Rollins, J., Kinch, J. (2003). TASIT: A new clinical tool for assessing social perception after traumatic brain injury. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 18, 219238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moretti, M., Torre, P., Antonello, R.M., Cazzato, G. (2000). Ten-point clock test: A correlation analysis with mini-mental state and proverb interpretation score in Alzheimer's disease. European Journal of Neurology, 7, 746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moretti, M., Torre, P., Antonello, R.M., Cazzato, G. (2001a). Fronto-temporal dementia vs. Alzheimer disease. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics. Supplement, 7, 273278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moretti, M., Torre, P., Antonello, R.M., Cazzato, G. (2001). Is isolated vitamin B12 deficiency a sufficient causative factor of dementia? European Journal of Neurology, 8, 8788.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moretti, M., Torre, P., Antonello, R.M., Cazzato, G., Bava, A. (2002). Ten-point clock test: A correlation analysis with other neuropsychological tests in dementia. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 17, 347353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moretti, M., Torre, P., Antonello, R.M., Cazzato, G., Bava, A., Manos, P.J. (2005). Use of the ten-point clock test to compare executive functioning across 24 months in patients with subcortical vascular dementia. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 100, 207216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, L.L. (2010). Distinguishing clinical depression from early Alzheimer's disease in elderly people: Can narrative analysis help? Aphasiology, Epub 29 January 2010. doi:10.1080/02687030903422460CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nippold, M.A., Taylor, C.L. (2002). Judgments of idiom familiarity and transparency: A comparison of children and adolescents. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45(2), 384391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nippold, M.A., Haq, F.S. (1996). Proverb comprehension in youth: The role of concreteness and familiarity. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 166176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Novelli, G., Papagno, C., Capitani, E., Laiacona, M., Vallar, G., Cappa, S.F. (1986). Tre test clinici di ricerca e produzione lessicale: taratura su soggetti normali. Archivio di Psicologia, Neurologia e Psichiatria, 47, 477505.Google Scholar
Osgood, C.E., Housain, R. (1974). Salience of the word as a unit in the perception of language. Perception and Psychophysics, 15, 168192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Panther, K.U., Radden, G. (1999). Metonymy in language and thought. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Papagno, C. (2001). Comprehension of metaphors and idioms in patients with Alzheimer's disease. A longitudinal study. Brain, 124, 14501460.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Papagno, C., Caporali, A. (2007). Testing idiom comprehension in aphasic patients: The effects of task and idiom type. Brain and Language, 100(2), 208220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Papagno, C., Cappa, S.F., Forelli, A., Garavaglia, G., Laiacona, M., Capitani, E., Vallar, G. (1995). La comprensione non letterale del linguaggio: taratura di un test di comprensione di metafore e di espressioni idiomatiche. Archivio di Psicologia, Neurologia e Psichiatria, 56, 402420.Google Scholar
Papagno, C., Lucchelli, F., Muggia, S., Rizzo, S. (2003). Idiom comprehension in Alzheimer's disease: The role of the central executive. Brain, 126, 24192430.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penn, N.E., Jacob, T.C., Brown, M. (1988). Familiarity with proverbs and performance of a black population on Gorham's Proverbs Test. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 66(3), 847854.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Qualls, C.D., Harris, J.L. (2003). Age, working memory, figurative language type, and reading ability: Influencing factors in African American adults’ comprehension of figurative language. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 12(1), 92102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rankin, K.P., Salazar, A., Gorno-Tempini, M.L., Sollberger, M., Wilson, S.M., Pavlic, D., Miller, B.L. (2009). Detecting sarcasm from paralinguistic cues: Anatomic and cognitive correlates in neurodegenerative disease. Neuroimage, 47(4), 20052015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rapp, A. (2009). The role of the right hemisphere for language in schizophrenia. In I.E. Sommer & R.S. Kahn (Eds.), Language lateralization in psychosis (pp. 147156). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rapp, A.M. (2011). The brain behind nonliteral language: Insights from brain imaging. In M. Faust (Ed.), Neuropsychology of language: Advances in the neural substrates of language: Towards a synthesis of basic research and clinical research (in press). New York: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Rapp, A.M., Leube, D.T., Erb, M., Grodd, W., Kircher, T.T. (2004). Neural correlates of metaphor processing. Brain Research Cognitive Brain Research, 20(3), 395402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rapp, A.M., Leube, D.T., Erb, M., Grodd, W., Kircher, T.T. (2007). Laterality in metaphor processing: Lack of evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging for the right hemisphere theory. Brain and Language, 100(2), 142149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rapp, A.M., Markert, K., Erb, M., Grodd, W. (2010). Neural correlates of metonymy resolution. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
Rapp, A.M., Mutschler, D.E., Wild, B., Erb, M., Lengsfeld, I., Saur, R., Grodd, W. (2010). Neural correlates of irony comprehension: The role of schizotypal personality traits. Brain and Language, 113(1), 112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rapp, A.M., Schmierer, P. (2010). Proverbs and nonliteral language in schizophrenia: A systematic methodological review of all studies published 1931–2010. Schizophrenia Research, 117(2–3), 422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rassiga, C., Lucchelli, F., Crippa, F., Papagno, C. (2009). Ambiguous idiom comprehension in Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 31(4), 402411.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reich, J.H. (1981). Proverbs and the modern mental status exam. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 22(5), 528531.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rinaldi, M.C., Marangolo, P., Baldassarri, F. (2004). Metaphor comprehension in right brain-damaged patients with visuo-verbal and verbal material: A dissociation (re)considered. Cortex, 40, 479490.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roos, P., Lewis, J.M. (1962). Differential abstraction deficits in a normal population. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 134(6), 535538.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rundblad, G., Annaz, D. (2010). Development of metaphor and metonymy comprehension: Receptive vocabulary and conceptual knowledge. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28(3), 547563.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sander, F.M., Greenberg, H.R. (1968). A proverbial excursion: On the hazards of administering proverbs to test the capacity to abstract. The Psychiatric Quarterly, 42(4), 696697.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Santos, M.T.F., Carvalho, T.L., Bastos, O., Sougey, E.B. (2005). Estudo piloto de desempenho mnêmico com “Jogo de Memória de Provérbios” criado para idosos. Neurobiologia, 68(2), 106.Google Scholar
Santos, M.T.F., Guerra, G., Menezes, T.L., Carvalho, T.L., Alchieri, J.C., Sougey, E.B. (2008). Preliminary data on a mnemonic instrument with proverbs for tracking Alzheimer's disease. Dementia & Neuropsychologia, 2, 333338.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Santos, M.T.F., Sougey, E.B., Alchieri, J.C. (2009). Validity and reliability of the Screening Test for Alzheimer's disease with Proverbs (STADP) for the elderly. Arquivos de Neuro-psiquiatria, 67(3-B), 836842.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, G.L., DeBuse, C.J., Seger, C.A. (2007). Right hemisphere metaphor processing? Characterizing the lateralization of semantic processes. Brain and Language, 100, 127141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, G.L., Kranjec, A., Cardillo, E.R., Chatterjee, A. (2010). Beyond laterality: A critical assessment of research on the neural basis of metaphor. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 16(1), 15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, G.L., Seger, C.A. (2009). Neural correlates of metaphor processing: The roles of figurativeness, familiarity and difficulty. Brain and Cognition, 71(3), 375386.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shamay-Tsoory, S.G., Aharon-Peretz, J. (2007). Dissociable prefrontal networks for cognitive and affective theory of mind: A lesion study. Neuropsychologia, 45(13), 30543067.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shamay-Tsoory, S.G., Tomer, R., Aharon-Peretz, J. (2005). The neuroanatomical basis of understanding sarcasm and its relationship to social cognition. Neuropsychology, 19(3), 288300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shamay-Tsoory, S.G., Tomer, R., Aharon-Peretz, J. (2002). Deficit in understanding sarcasm in patients with prefrontal lesion is related to impaired empathic ability. Brain and Cognition, 48(2–3), 558563.Google Scholar
Shibata, M., Abe, J., Terao, A., Miyamoto, T. (2007). Neural mechanisms involved in the comprehension of metaphoric and literal sentences: An fMRI study. Brain Research, 1166, 92102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shibata, M., Toyomura, A., Itoh, H., Abe, J. (2010). Neural substrates of irony comprehension: A functional MRI study. Brain Research, 1308, 114123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stringaris, A.K., Medford, N.C., Giampietro, V., Brammer, M.J., David, A.S. (2007). Deriving meaning: Distinct neural mechanisms for metaphoric, literal, and non-meaningful sentences. Brain and Language, 100(2), 150162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taler, V., Phillips, N.A. (2008). Language performance in Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment: A comparative review. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 30(5), 501556.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thoma, P., Daum, I. (2006). Neurocognitive mechanisms of figurative language processing – evidence from clinical dysfunctions. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30(8), 11821205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thoma, P., Hennecke, M., Mandok, T., Wähner, A., Brüne, M., Juckel, G., Daum, I. (2009). Proverb comprehension impairments in schizophrenia are related to executive dysfunction. Psychiatry Research, 170(2–3), 132139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treves, T.A., Ragolsky, M., Gelernter, I., Korczyn, A.D. (1990). Evaluation of a short mental test for the diagnosis of dementia. Dementia, 1, 102108.Google Scholar
Tseng, W.S., Streltzer, J. (2008). Cultural aspects of consultation-liaison psychiatry. In H. Leigh & J. Streltzer (Eds.), Handbook of consultation-liaison psychiatry (p. 275). Berlin, New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Uekermann, J., Thoma, P., Daum, I. (2008). Proverb interpretation changes in aging. Brain and Cognition, 67(1), 5157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ulatowska, H.K., Chapman, S.B., Highley, A.P., Prince, J. (1998). Discourse in healthy old-elderly adults: A longitudinal study. Aphasiology, 12(7–8), 619633.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Lancker, D. (1990). The neurology of proverbs. Behavioral Neurology, 3, 169187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (1976). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Revised. San Antonio, Texas: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wegrocki, H.J. (1940). Generalizing ability in schizophrenia; an inquiry into the disorders of problem thinking in schizophrenia. Archives of Psychology (Columbia University), 254, 76.Google Scholar
Winner, E., Gardner, H. (1977). The comprehension of metaphor in brain damaged patients. Brain, 100, 717729.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winner, E., Gardner, H. (1993). Metaphor and irony: Two levels of understanding. In A. Ortony (Ed.), Metaphor and thought (pp. 425446). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, F.G., Edens, J., Simpson, C., Krawczyk, D.C. (2009). Differences in task demands influence the hemispheric lateralization and neural correlates of metaphor. Brain and Language, 111(2), 114124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, F.P., Fuller, J., Khodaparst, N., Krawczyk, D.C. (2010). Figurative language processing after traumatic brain injury in adults: A preliminary study. Neuropsychologia, 48(7), 19231929.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zaidel, E., Kasher, A., Soroker, N., Batori, G. (2002). Effects of right and left hemisphere damage on performance of the “right hemisphere communication battery”. Brain and Language, 80(3), 510535.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed