Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T11:17:23.985Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

NIH Toolbox® Episodic Memory Measure Differentiates Older Adults with Exceptional Memory Capacity from those with Average-for-Age Cognition

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 February 2022

Tatiana Karpouzian-Rogers*
Affiliation:
Mesulam Center for Cognitive Neurology and Alzheimer’s Disease, Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, Chicago, Illinois, USA Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, Chicago, Illinois, USA
Beth Makowski-Woidan
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, Chicago, Illinois, USA
Alan Kuang
Affiliation:
Division of Biostatistics, Department of Preventative Medicine, Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, Chicago, Illinois, USA
Hui Zhang
Affiliation:
Division of Biostatistics, Department of Preventative Medicine, Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, Chicago, Illinois, USA
Angela Fought
Affiliation:
Department of Biostatistics and Informatic, Colorado School of Public Health, University of Colorado-Denver Anschutz Medical Campus, Aurora, CO
Janessa Engelmeyer
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, Chicago, Illinois, USA
M.-Marsel Mesulam
Affiliation:
Mesulam Center for Cognitive Neurology and Alzheimer’s Disease, Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, Chicago, Illinois, USA Department of Neurology, Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, Chicago, Illinois, USA
Sandra Weintraub
Affiliation:
Mesulam Center for Cognitive Neurology and Alzheimer’s Disease, Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, Chicago, Illinois, USA Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, Chicago, Illinois, USA
Emily Rogalski
Affiliation:
Mesulam Center for Cognitive Neurology and Alzheimer’s Disease, Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, Chicago, Illinois, USA Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, Chicago, Illinois, USA
*
*Correspondence and reprint requests to: Tatiana Karpouzian-Rogers, PhD, 320 E. Superior St., Chicago, IL 60611, USA. E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

Older adults with exceptional memory function, designated “SuperAgers,” include individuals over age 80, with episodic memory at least as good as individuals ages 50s–60s. The Northwestern University SuperAging cohort is defined by performance on an established test of verbal memory. The purpose of this study was to determine if superior verbal memory extends to nonverbal memory in SuperAgers by examining differences in the National Institutes of Health Toolbox® (NIHTB) between older adults with exceptional memory and those with average-for-age cognition.

Method:

SuperAgers (n = 46) and cognitively average-for-age older adults (n = 31) completed a comprehensive neuropsychological battery and the NIHTB Cognition module. Multiple linear regressions were used to examine differences on subtests between groups.

Results:

There was a significant effect of group on the Picture Sequence Memory score, (p = .007), such that SuperAgers had higher scores than cognitively average-for-age older adults. There were no other group effects across other non-episodic memory NIHTB Cognition measures.

Conclusions:

Findings from this study demonstrated stronger performance on the memory measure of the NIHTB in SuperAgers compared to cognitively average-for-age older adults demonstrating superior memory in not only verbal but also nonverbal episodic memory in this group. Additionally, this study adds to the literature validating the NIHTB in older adults, particularly in a novel population of adults over age 80 with exceptional memory.

Type
Brief Communication
Copyright
Copyright © INS. Published by Cambridge University Press, 2022

INTRODUCTION

Decline in memory functions is often accepted as part of “normal” aging, with mild changes beginning in mid-life and more accelerated changes occurring over the age of 60 (Nyberg et al., Reference Nyberg, Lövdén, Riklund, Lindenberger and Bäckman2012). However, at the Northwestern Mesulam Center for Cognitive Neurology and Alzheimer’s disease, we have identified a group of individuals that we designated “SuperAgers,” who are over age 80 and able to maintain superior memory performance compared to their same age peers and at a level that is at least “average” for 50- and 60-year-olds (Rogalski et al., Reference Rogalski, Gefen, Shi, Samimi, Bigio, Weintraub and Mesulam2013). Longitudinal follow-up of these individuals suggests that superior memory performance can be maintained over time, providing additional support for their resistance to the typical age-related decline (Gefen et al, Reference Gefen, Shaw, Whitney, Martersteck, Stratton, Rademaker and Rogalski2014; Rogalski et al., Reference Rogalski, Gefen, Mao, Connelly, Weintraub, Geula and Mesulam2019). With respect to psychological factors, SuperAgers report greater levels of social relationships compared to cognitively average-for-age peers (Cook Maher et al., Reference Cook Maher, Kielb, Loyer, Connelley, Rademaker, Mesulam and Rogalski2017). Neuroimaging studies have demonstrated greater cortical integrity and slowed rates of atrophy compared to cognitively average age-matched peers and thicker anterior cingulate cortex compared to 50-–65-year-olds (Harrison et al., Reference Harrison, Weintraub, Mesulam and Rogalski2012; Cook et al., Reference Cook, Sridhar, Ohm, Rademaker, Mesulam, Weintraub and Rogalski2017). Further, post-mortem studies suggest a lower frequency of Alzheimer neuropathology and higher density of von Economo neurons in the anterior cingulate compared to cognitively average older adults and individuals with amnestic mild cognitive impairment (Gefen et al., Reference Gefen, Peterson, Papastefan, Martersteck, Whitney, Rademaker and Geula2015).

The operationalization of memory capacity in SuperAgers was defined on the basis of scores on the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT), a difficult 15-item list-learning test of verbal episodic memory, which is widely used and has good psychometric properties. However, performance on tests of other episodic memory measures, including memory tests that place less emphasis on verbal abilities, has not been systematically investigated in this population. A recent research tool that was designed to measure cognitive functions in adults is the National Institutes of Health Toolbox® for Assessment of Neurological and Behavioral Function (NIHTB; Gershon et al., Reference Gershon, Wagster, Hendrie, Fox, Cook and Nowinski2013; Weintraub et al., Reference Weintraub, Dikmen, Heaton, Tulsky, Zelazo, Slotkin and Gershon2014). Traditionally, the evaluation of cognitive abilities in older adults has included either brief cognitive screening measures or lengthy neuropsychological batteries that often require clinical expertise and these batteries frequently differ across studies, making direct comparisons difficult to conduct. The NIHTB is a computerized suite of tests that measure cognitive, emotional, motor and sensory domains in individuals aged 3–85, and was designed to be used across a variety of settings, particularly in longitudinal research studies so that findings across studies could be conveniently compared. The Cognition Module includes a test of episodic memory, the Picture Sequence Memory test, that relies less heavily on verbal abilities, requiring participants to recall sequences of pictured actors and actions in the order they were originally learned over several trials.

The present study examined differences across all subtests of the Cognition module of the NIHTB between older adults with exceptional memory and those who are cognitively average-for-age. Our particular focus was to determine if the NIHTB episodic memory test specifically, which is less reliant on verbal abilities in comparison to our gold-standard of memory capacity, would be sensitive to differences between SuperAgers and “normal” agers. This is important as it would extend the contexts in which SuperAgers display superior episodic memory performance and opens the possibility of using the NIHTB as an efficient tool for future identification of SuperAgers. This study is also the first characterization of NIHTB Cognition module in an established cohort of older adults with exceptional memory. Given that SuperAgers display superior memory capacity, we hypothesized that the SuperAger group would demonstrate greater performance on the episodic memory test of the NIHTB.

METHODS

Participants

Participants 80 years or older were recruited through the Mesulam Center and Northwestern’s Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center Clinical Core, community lectures, and/or word of mouth. SuperAgers were referred on the basis of high memory scores and the absence of impairment in any other cognitive domain but were not necessarily superior in non-memory domains. Inclusion criteria for SuperAgers included: (1) score at or above the average level for 50–65-year-olds (equivalent to the Superior range for their own age) on the delayed recall condition of the (RAVLT; Schmidt, Reference Schmidt1996), a 15-word list-learning memory test; and (2) performance within one standard deviation of the average range for their age on nonmemory measures including the Trail Making Test Part-B, Category Fluency Test, and 30-item Boston Naming Test according to published normative data (Heaton, Reference Heaton2004, Randolph, Reference Randolph1998, Mack et al., Reference Mack, Freed, Williams and Henderson1992). Inclusion criteria for Cognitively Average-for-Age Older Adults included: performance within the average-for-age normative range on the RAVLT and on all non-memory tests administered in the study. Full scale IQ was measured using the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Third Edition (WAIS-III). Additional inclusion criteria for both groups were that all participants maintained their cognitive status (as measured by neuropsychological battery described above) from their visit to the time the NIH Toolbox® was administered to maintain the integrity of our SuperAger sample. The administration of the NIHTB and collection of the neuropsychological battery occurred no more than three months apart. Additionally, all participants were required to have preserved activities of daily living. Participants with significant neurologic or psychiatric illnesses were excluded. All participants provided written informed consent. The Institutional Review Board at Northwestern University approved all study procedures. Research was completed in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration.

Fig.1. SuperAgers perform significantly better than cognitively average-for-age 80+ year-olds on the NIH Toolbox® Picture Sequence Memory. **p < .01

Study Measures

As described in previous studies (Gefen et al., Reference Gefen, Shaw, Whitney, Martersteck, Stratton, Rademaker and Rogalski2014), all participants underwent a neuropsychological battery, including measures of attention, executive functions, language, and episodic memory. Participants completed the Cognition module of the NIH Toolbox® as part of the biyearly standardized battery. The Cognition Battery consists of tests assessing Executive Function and Attention (Dimensional Change Card Sort Test and Flanker Inhibitory Control and Attention Test), Episodic Memory (Picture Sequence Memory Test), Language (Oral Reading Recognition Test and Picture Vocabulary Test), Processing Speed (Pattern Comparison Processing Speed Test), and Working Memory (List Sorting Working Memory Test) (Weintraub et al., Reference Weintraub, Dikmen, Heaton, Tulsky, Zelazo, Slotkin and Gershon2014). In addition to individual test scores, Cognitive Function, Fluid Cognition, and Crystallized Cognition composite scores are computed. In the Picture Sequence Memory Test, participants are shown a series of pictures depicting a sequence of events, for example, playing in the park. Then, the pictures are assembled in the center of the screen and participants are asked to reproduce the spatial placement of the previously demonstrated sequence of pictures. For additional details on these modules, refer to the original publications (Weintraub et al., Reference Weintraub, Dikmen, Heaton, Tulsky, Zelazo, Bauer and Fox2013; Gershon et al., Reference Gershon, Cella, Fox, Havlik, Hendrie and Wagster2010; Gershon et al., Reference Gershon, Wagster, Hendrie, Fox, Cook and Nowinski2013) and the NIHTB website (nihtoolbox.org).

Statistical Analyses

Differences in participant demographics were assessed using two-sample t-tests. NIH Toolbox® scores were summarized using frequencies and percentages for categorical variables or mean and standard deviation for continuous variables. Histograms and scattered plots of each NIH Toolbox® measure were examined to explore the shape of distributions and identify potential outliers. For the Cognition Module, computed scores were calculated for Flanker, Dimensional Change Card Sort, Pattern Comparison, and Picture Sequence Memory subtests, theta scores were calculated for Reading and Vocabulary, and raw scores were used for List Sorting. For details regarding computation of theta and computed scores, refer to NIHTB Scoring and Interpretation Guide (http://www.healthmeasures.net/images/nihtoolbox/Training-Admin-Scoring_Manuals/NIH_Toolbox_Scoring_and_Interpretation_Manual_9-27-12.pdf). Composite scores were calculated by averaging the normalized scores of each measure, and then deriving scale scores based on this new distribution. For each NIH Toolbox® measure, multiple linear regressions were used to examine differences between groups. Covariate adjustments included Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale -Full Scale Intelligence Quotient (WAIS-FSIQ), sex, age, and education. Linear regression model fit was assessed using measures of collinearity and non-linearity, including residuals versus fits plots, histograms, Q-Q plots of residuals, and Dfbeta statistics. Adjusted R2 values were used to summarize variability explained in the linear models. All analyses were conducted in R 3.5.3 software.

RESULTS

Groups did not differ with respect to age, race, years of education, or sex (ps > .05) (Table 1). SuperAgers had a higher WAIS-FSIQ than cognitively average-for-age older adults (p < .001). Across all linear models, r-squared values ranged from .03–.34. Collinearity was not a concern, with pairwise correlations ranging from 0 to .56 for model covariates. Multicollinearity was not a concern, with variance inflation factor ranging from 1.03 to 1.79. Dfbeta statistics indicated there are some observations of influences. In order to improve linearity for measures that were positively skewed, scores were log-transformed, which included scores on the Picture Sequence Memory test.

Table 1. Study sample characteristics and NIHTB subtest scores

*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001

SD = standard deviation; M = male; F = female; CA = Caucasian; AA = African American.

*Note two participants (1 control, 1 SuperAger) had low Boston Naming Test (BNT) scores at the time of NITB but met qualifying criteria at their initial visit. Four participants (3 Controls, 1 SuperAger) had consistently low BNT scores, for one English was a second language and may have contributed to lower scores. These participants were retained in all analysis as there were no other objective or subjective reports of difficulty with language.

Within the Cognition Module, there was an effect of group on Picture Sequence Memory scores (F (1,63) = 7.7, p = .007, β = .10), such that the SuperAger group had higher scores than the Cognitively Average-for-Age Older Adults (Figure 1). The effects of sex, age, education level, and FSIQ were not significant in this model. There were no other effects of group across all other NIHTB Cognition tests of non-memory domains or on Composite Scores (all ps > .05).

DISCUSSION

This study sought to extend the superiority of episodic memory compared with other cognitive domains in the Northwestern University SuperAging cohort by comparing SuperAgers with cognitively average-for-age older adults on the tests of the NIHTB Cognition Battery. Findings of this study demonstrated greater performance on a test reliant on nonverbal episodic memory, the Picture Sequence Memory Test, in the SuperAging group compared to cognitively average-for-age older adults. Performance across all other measures of cognition on the NIHTB were comparable between groups. These findings confirm the exceptional episodic memory in SuperAgers.

The criteria for inclusion in the Northwestern University SuperAging research program involve completion of neuropsychological measures examining multiple aspects of cognition, with particular emphasis on memory abilities. SuperAging status requires performance at or above the average level of 50–65-year-olds on the RAVLT, a well-established measure of verbal episodic memory shown to be sensitive to early changes in memory and structural brain changes in Alzheimer’s disease (e.g., Estévez-González et al., Reference Estévez-González, Kulisevsky, Boltes, Otermín and García-Sánchez2003). Longitudinal studies of SuperAgers have demonstrated that superior memory performance tends to be stable, suggesting that exceptional memory capacity is not necessarily a function of superior premorbid cognitive abilities, but rather a resistance to age-related cognitive changes (Gefen et al., Reference Gefen, Shaw, Whitney, Martersteck, Stratton, Rademaker and Rogalski2014; Rogalski et al., Reference Rogalski, Gefen, Mao, Connelly, Weintraub, Geula and Mesulam2019). The finding that performance on the NIHTB Picture Sequence Memory, a measure of episodic memory that places less emphasis on verbal memory abilities, also differentiates the SuperAging group and cognitively average-for-age older adults is further confirmation of the memory superiority in SuperAgers. This suggests that superior memory capacity in SuperAgers may not be specific to the list learning of the RAVLT, but is more general to episodic memory. Additionally, scores on all other NIHTB measures were similar between groups, which mirrors our criteria that SuperAgers may score in at least the average range on all other measures of cognition, including object naming, semantic fluency, and executive attention. Although it is unclear why there were no differences at the group level across other NIHTB subtests, it is possible there may be nuanced profiles at the individual level; this is similar to what was observed in recent work from the SuperAging Research Program, which demonstrated significant intragroup variability on multiple cognitive domains (Maher et al., Reference Maher, Makowski-Woidan, Kuang, Zhang, Weintraub, Mesulam and Rogalski2021). Additional explanations include differences in the specific domains assessed by our neuropsychological measures versus the NIHTB (i.e. verbal fluency), as well as differences in tests used in this study compared to the neuropsychological tests used to validate the NIHTB (Weintraub et al., Reference Weintraub, Dikmen, Heaton, Tulsky, Zelazo, Bauer and Fox2013).

The NIHTB has been validated in older adults without cognitive impairment and has also been investigated in older adults with varying degrees of cognitive impairment. In particular, one study examined the psychometric properties of the NIHTB cognition module in cognitively intact older adults and found acceptable test-retest reliability over a one-year period, as well as a relationship between NIHTB Fluid Composite and cerebral volumes, and a strong correlation between Fluid and Crystallized Composites with their respective gold standard composites (Scott et al., Reference Scott, Sorrell and Benitez2019). In a study of older adults with subjective decline, mild cognitive impairment, or mild dementia, performance on the NIHTB Cognition module was consistent with performance on traditional neuropsychological tests and had greater discriminative ability when supplemented with RAVLT delayed recall performance (Hackett et al., Reference Hackett, Krikorian, Giovannetti, Melendez-Cabrero, Rahman, Caesar and Isaacson2018). Further, neuroimaging studies have demonstrated relationships between NIHTB performance and hippocampal volume and tau deposition in older adults (O’Shea et al. Reference O’Shea, Cohen, Porges, Nissim and Woods2016; Snitz et al., Reference Snitz, Tudorascu, Yu, Campbell, Lopresti, Laymon and Cohen2020). One important limitation is that the sample was a predominantly white, well-educated group, and therefore replication with a more diverse sample is needed. The present study is one of the first, to our knowledge, to examine performance in the NIHTB in an established cohort of adults over age 80 with exceptional memory and adds to the utility of using the NIHTB to measure cognitive functioning in the oldest of old age groups.

FINANCIAL SUPPORT

Research reported in this publication was supported, in part, by the following National Institutes of Health (NIH): the National Institute on Aging [NIA; award numbers R01AG045571, R56AG045571, R01AG067781, E. Rogalski; P30 AG13854, R. Vassar), the Institutional National Research Service Award (T32 AG020506, R. Vassar, trainee: T. Karpouzian-Rogers), and the National Center for Advancing Translational Science (NCATS; award number U54 NS092089, R. D’Aquila). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIH.

CONFLICTS OF INTEREST

The authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose.

References

REFERENCES

Cook Maher, A., Kielb, S., Loyer, E., Connelley, M., Rademaker, A., Mesulam, M.M., … & Rogalski, E. (2017). Psychological well-being in elderly adults with extraordinary episodic memory. PloS one, 12(10), e0186413.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cook, A.H., Sridhar, J., Ohm, D., Rademaker, A., Mesulam, M.M., Weintraub, S., & Rogalski, E. (2017). Rates of cortical atrophy in adults 80 years and older with superior vs average episodic memory. Jama, 317(13), 13731375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Estévez-González, A., Kulisevsky, J., Boltes, A., Otermín, P., & García-Sánchez, C. (2003). Rey verbal learning test is a useful tool for differential diagnosis in the preclinical phase of Alzheimer’s disease: comparison with mild cognitive impairment and normal aging. International journal of geriatric psychiatry, 18(11), 10211028.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gefen, T., Peterson, M., Papastefan, S.T., Martersteck, A., Whitney, K., Rademaker, A., … & Geula, C. (2015). Morphometric and histologic substrates of cingulate integrity in elders with exceptional memory capacity. Journal of Neuroscience, 35(4), 17811791.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gefen, T., Shaw, E., Whitney, K., Martersteck, A., Stratton, J., Rademaker, A., … & Rogalski, E. (2014). Longitudinal neuropsychological performance of cognitive SuperAgers. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 62(8), 15981600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gershon, R.C., Cella, D., Fox, N.A., Havlik, R.J., Hendrie, H.C., & Wagster, M.V. (2010). Assessment of neurological and behavioural function: the NIH Toolbox. The Lancet Neurology, 9(2), 138139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gershon, R.C., Wagster, M.V., Hendrie, H.C., Fox, N.A., Cook, K.F., & Nowinski, C.J. (2013). NIH toolbox for assessment of neurological and behavioral function. Neurology, 80(11 Supplement 3), S2S6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hackett, K., Krikorian, R., Giovannetti, T., Melendez-Cabrero, J., Rahman, A., Caesar, E.E., … & Isaacson, R.S. (2018). Utility of the NIH Toolbox for assessment of prodromal Alzheimer’s disease and dementia. Alzheimer’s & Dementia: Diagnosis, Assessment & Disease Monitoring, 10, 764772.Google ScholarPubMed
Harrison, T.M., Weintraub, S., Mesulam, M.M., & Rogalski, E. (2012). Superior memory and higher cortical volumes in unusually successful cognitive aging. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society: JINS, 18(6), 1081.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heaton, R.K. (2004). Revised comprehensive norms for an expanded Halstead-Reitan Battery: Demographically adjusted neuropsychological norms for African American and Caucasian adults, professional manual. Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Mack, W.J., Freed, D.M., Williams, B.W., & Henderson, V.W. (1992). Boston Naming Test: shortened versions for use in Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of gerontology, 47(3), P154P158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maher, A.C., Makowski-Woidan, B., Kuang, A., Zhang, H., Weintraub, S., Mesulam, M.M., & Rogalski, E. (2021). Neuropsychological Profiles of Older Adults with Superior versus Average Episodic Memory: The Northwestern “SuperAger” Cohort. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 111.Google Scholar
Nyberg, L., Lövdén, M., Riklund, K., Lindenberger, U., & Bäckman, L. (2012). Memory aging and brain maintenance. Trends in cognitive sciences, 16(5), 292305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Shea, A., Cohen, R., Porges, E.C., Nissim, N.R., & Woods, A.J. (2016). Cognitive aging and the hippocampus in older adults. Frontiers in aging neuroscience, 8, 298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Randolph, C. (1998). Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google ScholarPubMed
Rogalski, E., Gefen, T., Mao, Q., Connelly, M., Weintraub, S., Geula, C., … & Mesulam, M.M. (2019). Cognitive trajectories and spectrum of neuropathology in Super Agers: The first 10 cases. Hippocampus, 29(5), 458467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogalski, E.J., Gefen, T., Shi, J., Samimi, M., Bigio, E., Weintraub, S., … & Mesulam, M.M. (2013). Youthful memory capacity in old brains: anatomic and genetic clues from the Northwestern SuperAging Project. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 25(1), 2936.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, M. (1996). Rey auditory verbal learning test: A handbook (p. 1996). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
Scott, E.P., Sorrell, A., & Benitez, A. (2019). Psychometric Properties of the NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery in Healthy Older Adults: Reliability, Validity, and Agreement with Standard Neuropsychological Tests. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 25(8), 857867.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snitz, B.E., Tudorascu, D.L., Yu, Z., Campbell, E., Lopresti, B.J., Laymon, C.M., … & Cohen, A.D. (2020). Associations between NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery and in vivo brain amyloid and tau pathology in non-demented older adults. Alzheimer’s & Dementia: Diagnosis, Assessment & Disease Monitoring, 12(1), e12018.Google ScholarPubMed
Weintraub, S., Dikmen, S.S., Heaton, R.K., Tulsky, D.S., Zelazo, P.D., Bauer, P.J., … & Fox, N.A. (2013). Cognition assessment using the NIH Toolbox. Neurology, 80(11 Supplement 3), S54S64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weintraub, S., Dikmen, S.S., Heaton, R.K., Tulsky, D.S., Zelazo, P.D., Slotkin, J., … & Gershon, R. (2014). The cognition battery of the NIH toolbox for assessment of neurological and behavioral function: validation in an adult sample. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 20(6), 567578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Fig.1. SuperAgers perform significantly better than cognitively average-for-age 80+ year-olds on the NIH Toolbox® Picture Sequence Memory. **p < .01

Figure 1

Table 1. Study sample characteristics and NIHTB subtest scores