Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-mzp66 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-01-21T07:00:59.919Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Neuropsychiatric symptoms predict rate of change in executive function in Alzheimer’s disease and related dementias

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 January 2025

Grace J. Goodwin*
Affiliation:
Department of Brain Health, University of Nevada, Las Vegas, NV, USA
D.A. Briley
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign, IL, USA
Katie Singsank
Affiliation:
Department of Brain Health, University of Nevada, Las Vegas, NV, USA
Denise Tanner
Affiliation:
Department of Brain Health, University of Nevada, Las Vegas, NV, USA
Myjae Maloy-Robertson
Affiliation:
Department of Brain Health, University of Nevada, Las Vegas, NV, USA
Samantha E. John
Affiliation:
Department of Brain Health, University of Nevada, Las Vegas, NV, USA
*
Corresponding author: Grace J. Goodwin; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

Neuropsychiatric symptoms (NPS) are considered diagnostic and prognostic indicators of dementia and are attributable to neurodegenerative processes. Little is known about the prognostic value of early NPS on executive functioning (EF) decline in Alzheimer’s disease and related dementias (ADRD). We examined whether baseline NPS predicted the rate of executive function (EF) decline among older adults with ADRD.

Method:

Older adults (n = 1625) with cognitive impairment were selected from the National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center database. EF was estimated with a latent factor indicated by scores on Number Span Backward, Letter Fluency, and Trail Making-Part B. A curve of factors (CUFF) latent growth curve model was estimated to examine rate of change over four years. Baseline NPS severity was entered as a predictor in the model to examine its influence on the rate of change in EF over time.

Results:

The CUFF models exhibited good fit. EF significantly declined over four waves (slope = −.16, p < .001). Initial visit NPS severity predicted decline in EF (slope = .013, p < .001), such that those with greater baseline NPS severity demonstrated a more rapid decline in EF performance over time. Presence of 2 NPS significantly predicted EF decline, and those with medium total NPS severity (NPS score of 2–4) at baseline exhibited a sharper decline in EF.

Conclusions:

Findings underscore the importance of targeting NPS early across ADRD syndromes to minimize EF decline, offering novel insights into how early NPS treatment may alter cognitive trajectories. We provide an innovative, user-friendly web-based application that may be helpful for personalized treatment planning.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of International Neuropsychological Society

Introduction

Alzheimer’s disease and related dementias (ADRD) are neurodegenerative disorders characterized by declines in memory, thinking skills, and day-to-day functioning. Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is the most common cause of dementia, followed by vascular, Lewy body (DLB), and frontotemporal dementias (Goodman et al., Reference Goodman, Lochner, Thambisetty, Wingo, Posner and Ling2017). Approximately 5 million people in the United States age 85 years and older have ADRD, and this prevalence is expected to more than double by 2025 (Office of the Assistant Secretary for Planning and Evaluation, Reference Goodman, Lochner, Thambisetty, Wingo, Posner and Lingn.d.). As of 2019, the global economic burden of ADRD was $2.8 trillion, and this is expected to increase to $16.9 trillion by 2050 (Nandi et al., Reference Nandi, Counts, Chen, Seligman, Tortorice, Vigo and Bloom2022). This looming societal health and economic burden provides the impetus for research on sensitive, patient-specific methods of early disease identification and monitoring.

There has been increased attention on executive functioning (EF) decline and neuropsychiatric symptoms (NPS) in ADRD, given their importance for differential diagnosis (Guarino et al., Reference Guarino, Favieri, Boncompagni, Agostini, Cantone and Casagrande2019; Pakzad et al., Reference Pakzad, Ringuette, Borque and Sepehry2018), and functional decline (Cahn-Weiner et al., Reference Cahn-Weiner, Ready and Malloy2003; Farias et al., Reference Farias, Harrell, Neumann and Houtz2003; Gallo et al., Reference Gallo, Schmidt and Libon2008; Razani et al., Reference Razani, Casas, Wong, Lu, Alessi and Josephson2007), and conversion from MCI to dementia (Acosta et al., Reference Acosta, Borges, Aguirre‐Hernandez, Sosa and Prince2018; David et al., Reference David, Lin and Porsteinsson2016; Edwards et al., Reference Edwards, Spira, Barnes and Yaffe2009; Jung et al., Reference Jung, Park, Jang, Cho, Kim, Kim, Kim, Na, Seo and Kim2020; Teng et al., Reference Teng, Lu and Cummings2007). EF represents “higher-order” cognitive processes (e.g., inhibitory control, working memory, initiation, cognitive flexibility, problem solving, decision-making, emotion regulation) (Diamond, Reference Diamond2013; Suchy, Reference Suchy2015) that subserve complex, goal-directed tasks and behaviors (e.g., preparing food, managing medications and finances, etc.) (Gauthier & Gauthier, Reference Gauthier and Gauthier1990; Lezak, Reference Lezak2012). NPS (e.g., apathy, depression, aggression/agitation, anxiety, and irritability) (Devanand et al., Reference Devanand, Lee, Huey and Goldberg2022; Zhao et al., Reference Zhao, Tan, Wang, Jiang, Tan, Tan, Xu, Li, Wang, Lai and Yu2016) are common among ADRD syndromes, and presence and severity of NPS predict decreased quality of life (Kwon & Lee, Reference Kwon and Lee2021; Lyketsos et al., Reference Lyketsos, Steinberg, Tschanz, Norton, Steffens and Breitner2000; Martin & Velayudhan, Reference Martin and Velayudhan2020; Sadak et al., Reference Sadak, Katon, Beck, Cochrane and Borson2014). Executive dysfunction (Allain et al., Reference Allain, Etcharry-Bouyx and Verny2013; Carlson et al., Reference Carlson, Xue, Zhou and Fried2009; Duarte-Abritta et al., Reference Duarte-Abritta, Sánchez, Abulafia, Gustafson, Vázquez, Sevlever, Castro, Fiorentini, Villarreal and Guinjoan2021; Guarino et al., Reference Guarino, Favieri, Boncompagni, Agostini, Cantone and Casagrande2019; Harrington et al., Reference Harrington, Chiang, Pogoda, Gomez, Thomas, Marion, Miller, Siddarth, Yi, Zhou, Lee, Arakaki, Cowan, Tran, Charleswell, Ross, Fonteh and Ginsberg2013) and NPS (Apostolova & Cummings, Reference Apostolova and Cummings2008; Diniz et al., Reference Diniz, Butters, Albert, Dew and Reynolds2013; Gallagher et al., Reference Gallagher, Fischer and Iaboni2017; Kwon & Lee, Reference Kwon and Lee2021; Leoutsakos et al., Reference Leoutsakos, Forrester, Lyketsos Constantine and Smith2015) are evident in pre-clinical ADRD and throughout disease progression, and can be considered relevant cross-diagnostic features across ADRD syndromes (Johns et al., Reference Johns, Phillips, Belleville, Goupil, Babins, Kelner, Ska, Gilbert, Inglis, Panisset, De Boysson and Chertkow2009; Kwon & Lee, Reference Kwon and Lee2021; Ramirez-Gomez et al., Reference Ramirez-Gomez, Zheng, Reed, Kramer, Mungas, Zarow, Vinters, Ringman and Chui2017).

Shared neurobiological and clinical mechanisms may provide opportunities for development of generalizable, transdiagnostic interventions in ADRD (Zeng et al., Reference Zeng, Wang, Cai, Zhang, Zhang, Peng, Xiao, Ouyang and Yan2023). EF and NPS are both considered frontally mediated processes, implying overlapping pathology in ADRD (Bruen et al., Reference Bruen, McGeown, Shanks and Venneri2008; J. Cummings, Reference Cummings2020; J. L. Cummings, Reference Cummings1993; J. Cummings & Mega, Reference Cummings and Mega2003; Godefroy, Reference Godefroy2003). Cross-sectional research examining the association between NPS and EF in ADRD further highlights overlapping clinical mechanisms (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Sultzer, Hinkin, Mahler and Cummings1992; García-Alberca et al., Reference García-Alberca, Lara, Berthier, Cruz, Barbancho, Green and González-Barón2011). For example, agitation and disinhibition were associated with deficits in planning, organization, inhibitory control, and selective and divided attention (De Lucia et al., Reference De Lucia, Carbone, Muzii, Ferrara, Rengo, Maldonato and Femminella2023). However, there is limited research on the predictive role of NPS on the longitudinal trajectory of EF, which hinders our understanding of how these factors interact over time.

Recent longitudinal studies have shown mixed findings on the predictive role of NPS on EF over time. Two studies have shown that NPS predict more rapid cognitive decline over time in cognitively normal older adults (Burhanullah et al., Reference Burhanullah, Tschanz, Peters, Leoutsakos, Matyi, Lyketsos, Nowrangi and Rosenberg2020; Krell-Roesch et al., Reference Krell-Roesch, Syrjanen, Machulda, Christianson, Kremers, Mielke, Knopman, Petersen, Vassilaki and Geda2021). One study found that higher baseline NPS severity predicted steeper decline on EF tasks over time in patients with MCI due to probable Lewy body disease and Parkinson’s disease (Wright et al., Reference Wright, Donaghy, Burn, Taylor, O’Brien, Yarnall, Matthews, Firbank, Thomas and Lawson2023). In contrast, baseline NPS had no effect on cognitive functioning over time in patients with subjective cognitive decline, MCI, or probable AD (Eikelboom et al., Reference Eikelboom, van den Berg, Singleton, Baart, Coesmans, Leeuwis, Teunissen, van Berckel, Pijnenburg, Scheltens, van der Flier, Ossenkoppele and Papma2021).

Mixed findings may be the result of the measurement and modeling approaches used in these studies. First, EF was operationalized using only one test (Burhanullah et al., Reference Burhanullah, Tschanz, Peters, Leoutsakos, Matyi, Lyketsos, Nowrangi and Rosenberg2020) or with a composite score derived from a global cognitive screening measure (e.g., the Mini-Mental State Examination) (Eikelboom et al., Reference Eikelboom, van den Berg, Singleton, Baart, Coesmans, Leeuwis, Teunissen, van Berckel, Pijnenburg, Scheltens, van der Flier, Ossenkoppele and Papma2021; Wright et al., Reference Wright, Donaghy, Burn, Taylor, O’Brien, Yarnall, Matthews, Firbank, Thomas and Lawson2023); neither approach captures the full range and extent of EF abilities. Further, manifest variables—those that are directly observed—do not account for measurement error (e.g., variability in task administration) or the interaction among measures (e.g., association between two EF tasks). Second, linear mixed effects modeling does not offer as much flexibility or nuance compared to other approaches, as it assumes both linear change over time and a linear relationship between domains of interest. In other words, this approach assumes that 1) EF is a static construct that declines at a steady rate, and 2) the relationship between NPS and EF is consistent over time. Thus, important information about dynamic, nonlinear associations between NPS and EF decline might be missed (McNeish & Matta, Reference McNeish and Matta2018).

Latent growth curve modeling (LGCM) is considered the gold standard approach for modeling growth and decline over time in developmental psychology (Nesselroade, Reference Nesselroade1991) and cognitive aging research (McArdle et al., Reference McArdle, Hamgami, Jones, Jolesz, Kikinis, Spiro and Albert2004, Reference McArdle, Grimm, Hamagami, Bowles and Meredith2009; Ng et al., Reference Ng, Ng and Yang2023) because it provides precise estimates of intra- and inter-individual change. This model suggests that a single trajectory underlies growth or decline over time, allowing for strong and reliable estimates of 1) how things change over time and 2) what factors are associated with those changes (Ferrer et al., Reference Ferrer, Balluerka and Widaman2008; Liu & Chang, Reference Liu and Chang2010; McArdle, Reference McArdle2009). LGCM also allows for flexible estimation of nonlinear change (e.g., differences in rate of change in EF over time) (Grimm et al., Reference Grimm, Ram and Hamagami2011; McNeish & Matta, Reference McNeish and Matta2018). For example, one study used LGCM to examine the relationship among cognitive domains over time among cognitively normal older adults. Findings showed subtle declines in EF/attention and processing speed, while language and memory scores improved over time. Additionally, lower EF at baseline predicted decline in memory over time, suggesting that intact baseline EF is important for preserving memory functioning throughout aging (MacAulay et al., Reference MacAulay, Calamia, Cohen, Daigle, Foil, Brouillette, Bruce-Keller and Keller2018). A separate study used LGCM to model longitudinal change in neuropsychological performance in MCI and found that EF declined more rapidly than all other neurocognitive domains, suggesting intra-individual variability in cognitive decline (Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Gross, Pa, McLaren, Park and Manly2012). Therefore, LGCM may offer a more robust approach to estimating within- and between-person differences in the association between baseline NPS and longitudinal EF.

The present study

Precise research is needed to clarify the trajectory of EF decline in ADRD as well as the predictive role of early NPS on EF decline. Evaluation of NPS effects on EF decline over time among patients with ADRD may provide novel insights into the prognostic value of NPS and elucidate shared frontally mediated symptom presentations. Such insights are essential for advancing prognostic accuracy and identifying critical time points for targeted intervention. This study used the National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center (NACC) Uniform Data Set (UDS) to examine whether baseline NPS severity predicts rate of decline in EF in older adults with ADRD over four years. We modeled inter- and intra-individual patterns of EF change over time using second-order latent growth curve modeling. We expected that those with higher baseline NPS severity would exhibit more accelerated decline in EF.

Method

The NACC UDS provides a comprehensive data repository for research on neurodegenerative disorders. While the primary disorder of interest is Alzheimer’s disease, several other neurodegenerative conditions are included in the database and patients with ADRD are followed over time. The UDS contains longitudinal data that have been collected since 2005 at NIA-funded Alzheimer’s Disease Research Centers (ADRCs) across the United States. Data elements and collection methods have been described previously (Beekly et al., Reference Beekly, Ramos, van Belle, Deitrich, Clark, Jacka and Kukull2004, Reference Beekly, Ramos, Lee, Deitrich, Jacka, Wu, Hubbard, Koepsell, Morris and Kukull2007; Besser et al., Reference Besser, Kukull, Knopman, Chui, Galasko, Weintraub, Jicha, Carlsson, Burns, Quinn, Sweet, Rascovsky, Teylan, Beekly, Thomas, Bollenbeck, Monsell, Mock, Zhou and Morris2018; Morris et al., Reference Morris, Weintraub, Chui, Cummings, DeCarli, Ferris, Foster, Galasko, Graff-Radford, Peskind, Beekly, Ramos and Kukull2006). The NACC UDS includes neuropsychological, behavioral, medical, and health history data in order to accurately diagnose neurodegenerative disease and track its course (Morris et al., Reference Morris, Weintraub, Chui, Cummings, DeCarli, Ferris, Foster, Galasko, Graff-Radford, Peskind, Beekly, Ramos and Kukull2006). Participants and study partners enrolled at each ADRC provide written consent as part of the IRB-approved protocol at that site. This consent covers both the data collection procedures required by the respective center as well as the inclusion of the participant’s data in the larger NACC UDS database.

Participants

Participants were selected from the NACC UDS (v3-v3.2) data set (https://naccdata.org/). Patient evaluations were completed at funded ADRCs during the period between March 2015 and the freeze date of June 2023. Patient demographic variables and diagnostic status were used to identify the sample for analysis (Supplementary Figure 1). Data were included from four study visits: baseline (wave 0), 1 year (wave 1), 2 years (wave 2), and 3 years (wave 3). The following inclusionary criteria were applied for sample identification: participants aged 50 years or older; participants completed a baseline evaluation during UDS v3 or v3.2; and participants completed evaluations across the four consecutive waves (n = 3,770 after initial selection criteria). Participants were excluded if their cognitive status was “normal cognition” at all waves (excluded n = 1,844), if they were diagnosed as cognitively impaired due to a non-neurodegenerative etiology (i.e., “cognitively impaired, not MCI”; excluded n = 293), and if their cognitive data were considered invalid according to clinician report (excluded n = 8).

Measures

Race and ethnicity

In order to examine participant race and ethnicity, a new variable was calculated that combined data from the NACC-derived race variable for the six main census race groups and the UDS ethnicity variable for Hispanic/Latino ethnicity. Five new racial/ethnic groups were created from these data: non-Hispanic white, Hispanic white, non-Hispanic Black, Hispanic Black, and all other categories.

Cognitive status and dementia etiology

Cognitive status and etiologic diagnosis for each patient was determined through a formal process at each ADRC using the 2011 National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association (NIA-AA) guidelines (Albert et al., Reference Albert, DeKosky, Dickson, Dubois, Feldman, Fox, Gamst, Holtzman, Jagust, Petersen, Snyder, Carrillo, Thies and Phelps2011; Besser et al., Reference Besser, Kukull, Knopman, Chui, Galasko, Weintraub, Jicha, Carlsson, Burns, Quinn, Sweet, Rascovsky, Teylan, Beekly, Thomas, Bollenbeck, Monsell, Mock, Zhou and Morris2018). Diagnoses are assigned by either a consensus panel of experts or by the single physician conducting the examination, and this varies by center. Cognitive impairment includes the following categories: 1) normal cognition, 2) impaired-not-MCI (subjects who are cognitively impaired due to non-neurodegenerative etiology), 3) MCI (subjects with either amnestic or non-amnestic MCI), and 4) dementia (subjects who have a diagnosis of dementia) (Petersen & Morris, Reference Petersen and Morris2005). We excluded from our analytic sample all those with an “impaired-not-MCI” diagnosis. Participants were also excluded if their diagnostic severity was rated “normal cognition” across all four waves. Etiology of impairment includes the following categories, which are available for each of the neurodegenerative disorders (AD, DLB, Vascular, frontotemporal lobar degeneration [FTLD]): 1) primary (e.g., AD is the primary cause of observed cognitive impairment) and 2) contributing (e.g., AD is a contributing cause of observed cognitive impairment (Morris et al., Reference Morris, Weintraub, Chui, Cummings, DeCarli, Ferris, Foster, Galasko, Graff-Radford, Peskind, Beekly, Ramos and Kukull2006). See Supplementary Figure 1 for a detailed description of sample selection.

Characterization variables

The Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS) is a self-report measure of depression symptoms (Yesavage & Sheikh, Reference Yesavage and Sheikh1986). Patients rate whether they experienced 15 depression symptoms over the last week (0 = No, 1 = Yes). Scores are summed with scores of 9–11 indicating moderate depression and scores of 12–15 indicating severe depression. The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR®) Dementia Staging Instrument is a 5-point scale that characterizes six domains of cognitive and functional abilities (Morris, Reference Morris1993). Information is obtained through semi-structured interview of the patient and informant, and clinicians rate the patient’s level of overall impairment (0.0 = No impairment–3.0 = Severe Impairment).

Model variables

Neuropsychiatric symptoms

The NPI-Q is a widely used measure to assess neuropsychiatric symptoms among clinical populations (Kaufer et al., Reference Kaufer, Cummings, Ketchel, Smith, MacMillan, Shelley, Lopez and DeKosky2000). The NPI-Q relies on a caregiver/informant report of the presence and severity of 12 neuropsychiatric symptoms evident within the past month. Assessed symptoms include delusions, hallucinations, agitation/aggression, depression/dysphoria, anxiety, elation/euphoria, apathy/indifference, disinhibition, irritability/lability, motor disturbance, nighttime behaviors, and appetite/eating problems (Kaufer et al., Reference Kaufer, Cummings, Ketchel, Smith, MacMillan, Shelley, Lopez and DeKosky2000). Informants endorsed the presence (0 = No, 1 = Yes) and severity (1 = Mild, 2 = Moderate, 3 = Severe) of each symptom. The total NPI-Q symptom severity score is calculated by summing the severity score for each symptom (range 0–36). The NPI-Q has adequate psychometric properties, including acceptable test–retest reliability and convergent validity (Kaufer et al., Reference Kaufer, Cummings, Ketchel, Smith, MacMillan, Shelley, Lopez and DeKosky2000).

Executive functioning

Three measures in the NACC UDS v.3 neuropsychological battery provide distinct values of EF (e.g., phonemic fluency, working memory, set-shifting/cognitive flexibility) and were used to estimate a single EF factor. Phonemic Fluency (2-letter version) is a measure of speeded word generation in response to phonemic cues. Participants are asked to name items that begin with a certain letter, and the number of unique correct responses are scored. Possible scores range from 0 to 80 points (Besser et al., Reference Besser, Kukull, Knopman, Chui, Galasko, Weintraub, Jicha, Carlsson, Burns, Quinn, Sweet, Rascovsky, Teylan, Beekly, Thomas, Bollenbeck, Monsell, Mock, Zhou and Morris2018). Number Span Backward is a measure of working memory. Numbers are presented orally and participants are asked to recall the numbers in reverse order; item difficulty increases in ascending order. We use the number of correct trials as a total score in the model, which ranges from 0 to 14 (Besser et al., Reference Besser, Kukull, Knopman, Chui, Galasko, Weintraub, Jicha, Carlsson, Burns, Quinn, Sweet, Rascovsky, Teylan, Beekly, Thomas, Bollenbeck, Monsell, Mock, Zhou and Morris2018). Trail Making Test Part B (TMT-B) is a measure of set-shifting and cognitive flexibility. Participants are asked to draw lines in number and letter order while shifting between numbers and letters. Total time to complete the task is scored with possible scores ranging from 0 to 300 s (Reitan & Wolfson, Reference Reitan and Wolfson1993).

Analyses

Statistical analyses were conducted in R version 4.0.3 using the lavaan package (Roseel, 2012). NPI-Q severity scores and TMT-B scores were reverse scored so that higher scores indicated better functioning across all measures. Analyses were conducted in three stages. First, confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was used to model EF from the three EF measures, and measurement invariance was examined across waves. Second, a curve-of-factors (CUFF) latent growth curve model was then used to examine rate of change in the EF factor across waves. Third, structural regression was used to examine whether NPS severity at W0 predicted rate of change in the EF factor over four waves (Ferrer et al., Reference Ferrer, Balluerka and Widaman2008).

Measurement invariance of executive functioning

CFA was used to estimate the executive function factor from Number Span Backward, Letter Fluency, and TMT-B (Staffaroni et al., Reference Staffaroni, Asken, Casaletto, Fonseca, You, Rosen, Boxer, Elahi, Kornak, Mungas and Kramer2021). Full information maximum-likelihood (FIML) estimation and maximum likelihood with robust standard errors (MLR) estimation were used as they are robust to missingness and non-normality, respectively (Enders & Bandalos, Reference Enders and Bandalos2001). Four nested models were compared to test measurement invariance across the four waves: configural invariance model (establishes model specification equivalence), metric invariance model (establishes equivalence of factor loadings), scalar invariance model (establishes equivalence of intercepts), and strict invariance model (establishes equivalence of residuals). Models are considered invariant if there is no significant reduction in model fit at any step. The configural model had the following identification constraints: all factor loadings and intercepts were freely estimated; the mean and variance of all factors were set to 0 and 1, respectively, (creating a standardized latent factor). Correlations were permitted between factors at each time point, and temporal correlations were permitted between like indicators at each wave. In the nested metric model, factor loadings were set to be equivalent across time, and constraints on the factor variances were removed for wave 1 through wave 3. In the nested scalar model, indicator intercepts were set to be equivalent across time, and constraints on the factor means were removed for wave 1 through wave 3. In the nested strict model, indicator residual variances were set to be equivalent across time.

Overall model fit was determined using χ2 goodness-of-fit test, comparative fit index (CFI), and root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA). A nonsignificant p value suggests adequate model fit for the χ2 test; however, this test is affected by large sample sizes (Cheung & Rensvold, Reference Cheung and Rensvold2002; Hu & Bentler, Reference Hu and Bentler1999). Fit indices were considered satisfactory when CFI > .95 and RMSEA < .06 (Hu & Bentler, Reference Hu and Bentler1999). Model comparisons were analyzed using change in chi-square (Δχ2) and change in CFI (ΔCFI) (Cheung & Rensvold, Reference Cheung and Rensvold2002).

Longitudinal change in EF predicted by NPS

Longitudinal change in EF was assessed using a second-order growth model, or curve-of-factors (CUFF) LGCM (Ferrer et al., Reference Ferrer, Balluerka and Widaman2008; Tisak & Meredith, Reference Tisak and Meredith1990), in which change is modeled at the latent level over time. The CUFF model is an extension of the LGCM; while the LGCM estimates change of observed variables (e.g., raw scores or composite scores) over time, the CUFF model measures change at the latent level over time. Differently stated, the CUFF model fits a growth curve to factor scores (hence “curve-of-factors”) and estimates longitudinal change of the latent factors (Ferrer et al., Reference Ferrer, Balluerka and Widaman2008). First, the strict temporal invariance CFA model was estimated, which consisted of four factors: Wave 0 EF, Wave 1 EF, Wave 2 EF, and Wave 3 EF, with added constraints for identification purposes. Letter Fluency and TMT-B were rescaled (Letter Fluency/10; TMT-B/1000) to help with residual variance estimation. We then fit a growth curve to these four factors. Growth parameters were estimated to examine the intercept and slope (i.e., rate of change in EF), and the association between intercept and slope at the latent level. The intercept factor loadings were constrained to be constant across waves with loadings of 1 for all time points. The slope factor loadings had fixed coefficients of 0, 1, 2, and 3 to reflect each visit. Latent EF factor means were constrained to be 0 to model change in EF with the intercept and slope factors. The intercept factor mean and variance were constrained to be 0 and 1, respectively, for identification purposes. This specification is consistent with fixing the latent EF mean and variance at baseline to 0 and 1 to establish a standardized latent factor at baseline. The slope factor mean and variance were freely estimated. The intercept and slope latent factors were regressed on baseline NPI-Q total symptom severity scores to estimate the predictive value of baseline NPS total severity on EF decline.

Results

The final sample (n = 1,625) consisted of older adults (M W0age = 71.3, SD W0age = 8.18; 46.7% female, M education = 16 years, SD education = 2.91 years) who predominantly identified as non-Hispanic white (79.4% non-Hispanic white, 9.5% non-Hispanic Black, 5.6% Hispanic white, 5.1% other, 4% Hispanic Black). The majority of the sample met criteria for MCI at W0 (48%), and a large percentage of the sample met criteria for dementia at waves 1–3 (W1: 44.9%; W2: 50.7%; W3: 55.8%). AD was the presumed primary etiology for cognitive impairment for the majority of the sample across all waves followed by FTLD, Lewy body disease, and cerebrovascular disease (Table 1). On average, 1 or more symptoms were endorsed on the NPI-Q at W0 (median number of symptoms = 2, min = 0, max = 11). Among those who endorsed symptoms at W0, total NPS severity was low overall (median total symptom severity = 3, min = 1, max = 26). The most frequently endorsed symptom at W0 was anxiety (31.44%), followed by irritability/lability (31.35%), and depression/dysphoria (30.32%). Hallucinations were the least frequently endorsed symptom at W0 (3.94%). See Table 2 for additional characterization of NPS severity at W0.

Table 1. Participant demographics stratified by time point

Note: Etiology variables may not add up to 1,625 as 1) there may be overlapping etiologies, 2) etiology diagnosis might change over time, and 3) there are other classifications that are not included here. All totals are relative to a given etiology category. M = mean, SD = standard deviation, MCI = mild cognitive impairment, FTLD = frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Disease etiology derived from clinician diagnosis of cause of observed cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease, Lewy Body Disease, FTLD, or vascular diseases. Other* ethnoracial group includes those identifying as Asian, American Indian/Alaska Native, Native Hawaiian or Other Pacific Islander, Multiracial, and Unknown.

Table 2. Frequency of neuropsychiatric symptom severity ratings at wave 0 by symptom and overall total

Note: Neuropsychiatric symptom ratings derived from the NPI-Q. Item-level ratings: symptom not present = 0, mild = 1, moderate = 2, severe = 3. Total Symptom Severity score takes the sum of each of the 12 symptom severity scores. Total Symptom Severity Score range = 0-26. Based on quantiles, the following item-level ratings were applied to the Total Symptom Severity score: no symptoms reported on any items (“absent symptoms”) = 0, “low severity” overall = 1, “medium severity” overall = 2-4, and “high severity” overall > 4.

Cognitive variables met assumptions for normal distribution (skewness < |1.0|; 2 > kurtosis < 4 ) for all variables across all time points, and NPI-Q at W0 did not meet assumptions for normality (skewness = 2.02; kurtosis = 7.93) (Field, Reference Field2013) (Table 3). Thus, FIML with MLR was used in all models, and robust fit statistics are reported. Results from the three structural equation models are discussed separately, below.

Table 3. Cognitive & neuropsychiatric characteristics stratified by time point for overall sample

Note: M = mean, SD = standard deviation, Min = minimum, Max = maximum, CDR = clinical dementia rating, NPS = neuropsychiatric symptoms. CDR impairment ratings derived from the Clinical Dementia Rating Global Impairment score. Depression derived from Geriatric Depression Scale total score. NPS derived from Neuropsychiatric Symptom Inventory Questionnaire (NPIQ) total severity score. Descriptive statistics are based on raw variables.

Measurement invariance over time

Table 4 provides the model fit for the measurement invariance models. Strict measurement invariance was established at all four stages, suggesting that the EF factor is time-invariant (see Supplementary Figure 2). The largest ΔCFI was .008, which suggests imposing measurement invariance constraints do not substantially worsen model fit.

Table 4. Measurement invariance analysis results

Note: Robust fit statistics are reported. Δχ2 = change in χ2. CFI = comparative fit index. ΔCFI = change in CFI. RMSEA = root mean-square error of approximation. *p < .05. **p < .001. aThis step was determined to be invariant.

Decline in EF over time

We initially attempted to freely estimate the latent EF factor variances, but we encountered a Heywood case (i.e., negative variance estimate). We fit alternative specifications that fixed variances to 0, equated variance estimates across waves (i.e., equivalent residual EF variance at each wave), or constrained the variances for the Heywood case (i.e., EF W0) to be positive (Supplementary Table 1). All models are included in the supplemental materials, and all models imply the same substantive conclusions. We focus here on the best fitting model without a Heywood case, a model in which we fixed EF factor variance estimates at W0 to be greater than 0.

Using the strict invariance model, a CUFF model was estimated to examine rate of change in EF over time (see Supplementary Figure 3). The CUFF model exhibited good fit (χ 2[57] = 195.44, p < .001; CFI = .98, RMSEA = .04). EF declined at a rate of .16 SDs per wave (p < .001). Differently stated, participants with average EF at baseline (standardized M EF at W0 = 0) would be expected to decline .48 SDs by W3 (Table 5). EF intercept and slope were positively correlated (r = .25), such that the higher initial EF scores were, the less decline in EF over time. The slope variance estimate (slope variance = .05, p < .001) suggests that people slightly differ in their rate of EF decline (i.e., some people decline at a faster rate than others).

Table 5. Model implied decline in EF based on baseline NPS severity

Note: Values represent decline in EF from wave to wave based on baseline NPS severity. Values are in standard deviation (SD) units. Model-implied intercept (slope when NPS = 0) = −.16. Model-implied slope (when NPS >0) = .013.

NPS Predicting EF Decline over time

Baseline NPI-Q total symptom severity score was entered as a predictor in the CUFF model to determine the predictive value of baseline NPS on EF rate of decline. The model exhibited good fit (χ 2[67] = 198.93, p < .001; CFI = .98, RMSEA = .04). NPS total symptom severity at W0 predicted decline in EF (p < .001), such that those with greater total NPS severity at W0 decreased more rapidly in EF over time (Figure 1). For every point increase in total NPS severity, participants declined in EF by an additional .013 SD (small effect) per year (Supplementary Figure 4). For example, for participants with W0 NPS severity at the median (total NPS severity = 2), they would decline an additional −.56 SDs in EF by W3. For participants with the highest level of symptom severity at W0 (total NPS severity = 26), they would decline an additional −1.49 SDs in EF by W3 (Table 5). Like the EF CUFF model, the slope variance estimate (slope variance = .05, p < .001) suggests that people slightly differ in their rate of EF decline (i.e., some people decline at a faster rate than others). Model-implied indices were used to develop an open access, interactive, web-based application [available at: https://gracejgoodwin.shinyapps.io/GrowthCurveEFapp/] allowing users to observe EF trajectories as a function of baseline NPS severity (Supplementary Figure 5).

Figure 1. Model-implied trajectory of executive function decline by NPS severity. Note: Main Graph: depicts model-implied differences in EF decline by NPS severity group. EF = executive functioning. NPS = neuropsychiatric symptoms. X-axis represents time points that correspond to annual visits (0 = wave 0, 1 = wave 1, 2 = wave 2, 3 = wave 3). Y-axis represents model-implied EF score. Mild NPS group refers to people with NPS at −1.5 standard deviations below the raw NPS mean. Moderate NPS refers to people with NPS at the NPS mean. Severe NPS refers to people with NPS scores at 1.5 standard deviations above the NPS mean. These labels do not correspond to item ratings with similar labels. Inset Graph: depicts the magnitude of the difference in EF decline between groups over time. X-axis represents time points; Y-axis represents expected gap between mild and severe NPS on EF. W0 gap between EF performance between mild NPS and severe NPS = .148 intercept-SDs; W1 gap = .188; W2 gap = .228; W3 gap = .268. The gap in EF performance nearly doubles between mild and severe NPS by W3.

Post hoc robustness check

We performed two checks to determine the robustness of our findings. First, we tested whether extreme NPI-Q outliers were driving effects in the CUFF model. We Winsorized NPI-Q total symptom severity scores that were greater than three standard deviations from the mean to the next highest value (Wilcox, Reference Wilcox1993). We used the Winsorized NPI-Q total symptom severity score in the predicted CUFF model to determine whether effects were consistent without extreme cases. There was a statistically significant effect of total NPS severity on rate of decline suggesting that outliers were not exclusively driving effects.

Second, we tested whether the association between total NPS severity and EF decline was linear. We created quantiles for the W0 NPI-Q severity variable (median = 2), which equally split the data into 4 categories: “absent symptoms” (NPI-Q total symptom severity = 0), “low severity” (NPI-Q total symptom severity = 1), “medium severity” (NPI-Q total symptom severity = 2–4), “high severity” (NPI-Q total symptom severity > 4). The absent symptom and low severity groups were roughly equivalent to one another, implying that NPS does not have a sizeable impact on EF levels or trajectories at lower levels. However, for participants scoring above the median, NPS was associated with lower EF levels (in an approximately linear manner) and quicker rate of EF decline. The rate of EF decline was similar for the medium and high groups, implying that above a medium baseline NPS severity threshold, EF deficits magnify over time. Given that the NPI-Q total score is used clinically, we chose to retain the continuous variable in our final CUFF model. However, future work with larger samples would be required to more precisely identify the baseline severity threshold for detrimental EF prognosis (Supplementary Table 2).

Discussion

The present study examined whether initial visit NPS severity predicted rate of EF decline over time in ADRD. We used second-order LGCM to identify inter- and intra-individual differences in the association between NPS at initial visit and rate of change in EF. Consistent with expectations, those with greater total NPS severity at baseline exhibited a more rapid rate of EF decline over four years.

Prevalence of neuropsychiatric symptoms at initial visit

The majority of our sample met criteria for MCI at W0 and there was a higher prevalence of dementia at each subsequent wave. Much of our sample remained in the same diagnostic category across sequential visits, though we observed disease conversion, as seen through the shifting sample proportions between MCI and dementia over 4 years. NPS burden at W0 was generally low, which is representative of early clinical presentations of MCI (Edwards et al., Reference Edwards, Spira, Barnes and Yaffe2009; Lyketsos et al., Reference Lyketsos, Lopez, Jones, Fitzpatrick, Breitner and DeKosky2002). A median of 2 NPS items were endorsed at W0, and of those who reported symptoms, the majority endorsed mild symptom severity. Consistent with previous research, the most frequently endorsed NPS were irritability/lability, anxiety, and depression/dysphoria, while psychotic symptoms were less common (Lyketsos et al., Reference Lyketsos, Lopez, Jones, Fitzpatrick, Breitner and DeKosky2002; Martin & Velayudhan, Reference Martin and Velayudhan2020). Though symptom burden was generally low at W0 for the overall sample, presence of at least one symptom and low NPS severity at early stages has been shown to increase risk for conversion to dementia (Peters et al., Reference Peters, Rosenberg, Steinberg, Norton, Welsh-Bohmer, Hayden, Breitner, Tschanz and Lyketsos2013). In particular, irritability and depression predict conversion from MCI to dementia (AD and non-AD dementias) independent of age, education, cognitive screener score, and apolipoprotein E status (Mourao et al., Reference Mourao, Mansur, Malloy-Diniz, Castro Costa and Diniz2016; Roberto et al., Reference Roberto, Portella, Marquié, Alegret, Hernández, Mauleón, Rosende-Roca, Abdelnour, de Antonio, Gil, Tartari, Vargas, Espinosa, Ortega, Pérez-Cordón, Sanabria, Orellana, de Rojas, Moreno-Grau and Valero2021).

Change in EF over time

Latent growth curve analysis showed that participants significantly declined in EF over time within a sample of participants with mixed ADRD etiologies. There were inter-individual differences in the rate of EF change, such that higher EF scores at initial visit were associated with less EF decline over time. There was also intra-individual variability in the rate of change, such that the level of EF decline varied from wave to wave. In contrast, raw score means of EF measures remained stable (Number Span Backward, Letter Fluency) or slightly declined (TMT-B) over the four waves, obscuring more nuanced trends in susceptibility and rate of decline and showcasing the added information gained from a latent growth curve approach. Findings are consistent with studies using latent growth curves to model change in EF over time in MCI: EF declines over time in AD and there is significant variability in rate of decline in EF over time (Gustavson et al., Reference Gustavson, Jak, Elman, Panizzon, Franz, Gifford, Reynolds, Toomey, Lyons, Kremen and Peter2021; Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Gross, Pa, McLaren, Park and Manly2012). While we did not compare EF with other cognitive domains, previous literature suggests that EF may decline earlier than memory, highlighting the importance of tracking non-memory cognitive domains for early detection and timely intervention. Some studies have shown inconsistent patterns of EF change over time in ADRD, likely due to measurement error and variable sensitivity across EF scales (Eikelboom et al., Reference Eikelboom, van den Berg, Singleton, Baart, Coesmans, Leeuwis, Teunissen, van Berckel, Pijnenburg, Scheltens, van der Flier, Ossenkoppele and Papma2021; Guarino et al., Reference Guarino, Favieri, Boncompagni, Agostini, Cantone and Casagrande2019). Our robust approach allows for examination of nuanced inter- and intra-individual variability in rate of decline in EF over time.

NPS as a prognostic marker for executive function decline

As expected, NPS severity at initial visit predicted rate of change in EF, such that those who had greater NPS severity at their initial visit had more rapid decline in EF over time. While the influence of NPS on the slope of EF decline is small, the longitudinal effect on EF is notable and clinically meaningful. While NPS burden was generally low at W0, those with only two symptoms at initial visit still showed more rapid decline in EF over four years relative to those without symptoms. Moreover, compared to absent symptoms and low symptom severity, those with medium levels of NPS severity at initial visit (NPS = 2–4) demonstrated a sharper decline in EF. This pattern was evident within a sample of mixed neurodegenerative etiologies, supporting the generalizability of findings. Similar to our results, other investigations of NPS severity have revealed those with moderate NPS burden at early stages may be at greater risk for executive dysfunction and associated functional decline (Gallo et al., Reference Gallo, Schmidt and Libon2008; Razani et al., Reference Razani, Casas, Wong, Lu, Alessi and Josephson2007). Our findings are also consistent with previous work that showed baseline NPS were associated with more rapid decline in non-executive cognitive tasks, including word list memory, animal fluency, and praxis recall (Burhanullah et al., Reference Burhanullah, Tschanz, Peters, Leoutsakos, Matyi, Lyketsos, Nowrangi and Rosenberg2020). Specific NPS have also been shown to predict accelerated decline across cognitive domains (Zainal & Newman, Reference Zainal and Newman2023). For example, among community dwelling cognitively normal older adults or adults with MCI, mild NPS have also been shown to predict more accelerated decline across several cognitive domains (e.g., memory, attention, language, and visuospatial skills). This work also identified specific NPS (i.e., apathy, depression, nighttime behaviors) that were responsible for greater decline across domains, pointing to their use as prognostic markers (Krell–Roesch et al., Reference Krell-Roesch, Syrjanen, Machulda, Christianson, Kremers, Mielke, Knopman, Petersen, Vassilaki and Geda2021). While examining the association between specific NPS and rate of EF decline was beyond the scope of the present work, it is an important future direction that will pinpoint specific symptoms that may be responsible for this pattern.

Limitations and future directions

There are several limitations to the present study. While we included a diverse sample representing several ADRD syndromes, the majority of participants were diagnosed with AD. While our sample is consistent with disease base-rates (i.e., AD is more common than other related dementias) (Goodman et al., Reference Goodman, Lochner, Thambisetty, Wingo, Posner and Ling2017), future research sampling a more balanced and representative group of ADRD syndromes will identify disease-specific associations between NPS and EF. Additionally, as is true of the overall trends with the NACC sample, our analysis consisted of predominantly highly educated, non-Hispanic white participants, which limits the generalizability of our findings to diverse groups. Future research with minoritized samples is essential to identify differences in the association between EF and NPS and to improve access to individualized care. Relatedly, the NPI-Q symptom descriptions may be susceptible to cultural bias or strongly associated with other comorbid conditions among different ethnoracial groups. These interpretative differences can influence symptom endorsement patterns (Babulal et al., Reference Babulal, Zhu and Trani2023). Additionally, the NPI-Q does not include information about frequency of experienced symptoms (e.g., once per week, several times per week), which limits interpretation of NPS burden. Examining the association between NPS symptom frequency and EF may provide additional prognostic clarity. Measurement of EF was limited to three neurocognitive subtests, which do not capture all aspects of EF. Modeling EF using a variety of measures tapping several subdomains of EF (e.g., planning, decision-making) is necessary for improving ecological validity of findings. Finally, while total NPS severity scores were used as a first step in our analyses, future research examining the role of individual NPS will allow for development of precise and targeted interventions.

Conclusions

In summary, older adults with ADRD exhibit significant decline in EF over four years, and this decline becomes more accelerated with higher NPS severity at early stages. Further, we identified thresholds of NPS severity that may be prognostic of anticipated cognitive change. Our results suggest that early assessment of NPS is important for predicting EF decline in MCI and dementia. Early evaluation of NPS may allow for identification of patients who are at higher risk for cognitive decline and offer the opportunity for early symptom management and intervention. Early treatment of NPS, regardless of the level of severity, should be prioritized to minimize EF decline and associated declines in independence.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617724000730.

Acknowledgements

The NACC database is funded by NIA/NIH Grant U24 AG072122. NACC data are contributed by the NIA-funded ADRCs: P30 AG062429 (PI James Brewer, MD, PhD), P30 AG066468 (PI Oscar Lopez, MD), P30 AG062421 (PI Bradley Hyman, MD, PhD), P30 AG066509 (PI Thomas Grabowski, MD), P30 AG066514 (PI Mary Sano, PhD), P30 AG066530 (PI Helena Chui, MD), P30 AG066507 (PI Marilyn Albert, PhD), P30 AG066444 (PI John Morris, MD), P30 AG066518 (PI Jeffrey Kaye, MD), P30 AG066512 (PI Thomas Wisniewski, MD), P30 AG066462 (PI Scott Small, MD), P30 AG072979 (PI David Wolk, MD), P30 AG072972 (PI Charles DeCarli, MD), P30 AG072976 (PI Andrew Saykin, PsyD), P30 AG072975 (PI David Bennett, MD), P30 AG072978 (PI Neil Kowall, MD), P30 AG072977 (PI Robert Vassar, PhD), P30 AG066519 (PI Frank LaFerla, PhD), P30 AG062677 (PI Ronald Petersen, MD, PhD), P30 AG079280 (PI Eric Reiman, MD), P30 AG062422 (PI Gil Rabinovici, MD), P30 AG066511 (PI Allan Levey, MD, PhD), P30 AG072946 (PI Linda Van Eldik, PhD), P30 AG062715 (PI Sanjay Asthana, MD, FRCP), P30 AG072973 (PI Russell Swerdlow, MD), P30 AG066506 (PI Todd Golde, MD, PhD), P30 AG066508 (PI Stephen Strittmatter, MD, PhD), P30 AG066515 (PI Victor Henderson, MD, MS), P30 AG072947 (PI Suzanne Craft, PhD), P30 AG072931 (PI Henry Paulson, MD, PhD), P30 AG066546 (PI Sudha Seshadri, MD), P20 AG068024 (PI Erik Roberson, MD, PhD), P20 AG068053 (PI Justin Miller, PhD), P20 AG068077 (PI Gary Rosenberg, MD), P20 AG068082 (PI Angela Jefferson, PhD), P30 AG072958 (PI Heather Whitson, MD), P30 AG072959 (PI James Leverenz, MD).

Funding statement

SEJ is supported by NIH/NIGMS P20GM109025 (Project R-2). The funding body had no role in the design of the study, analyses, interpretation of the data, and writing of the manuscript.

Competing interests

The authors report no conflicts of interest.

References

Acosta, I., Borges, G., Aguirre‐Hernandez, R., Sosa, A. L., Prince, M., 10/66 Dementia Research Group (2018). Neuropsychiatric symptoms as risk factors of dementia in a Mexican population: A 10/66 Dementia Research Group study. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 14, 271279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Albert, M. S., DeKosky, S. T., Dickson, D., Dubois, B., Feldman, H. H., Fox, N. C., Gamst, A., Holtzman, D. M., Jagust, W. J., Petersen, R. C., Snyder, P. J., Carrillo, M. C., Thies, B., & Phelps, C. H. (2011). The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease: Recommendations from the national institute on aging-Alzheimer’s association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 7, 270279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allain, P., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., & Verny, C. (2013). Executive functions in clinical and preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Revue Neurologique, 169, 695708.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Apostolova, L. G., & Cummings, J. L. (2008). Neuropsychiatric manifestations in mild cognitive impairment: A systematic review of the literature. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 25, 115126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Babulal, G. M., Zhu, Y., & Trani, J.-F. (2023). Racial and ethnic differences in neuropsychiatric symptoms and progression to incident cognitive impairment among community-dwelling participants. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 19, 36353643.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beekly, D. L., Ramos, E. M., Lee, W. W., Deitrich, W. D., Jacka, M. E., Wu, J., Hubbard, J. L., Koepsell, T. D., Morris, J. C., & Kukull, W. A. (2007). The national Alzheimer’s coordinating center (NACC) database: The uniform data set. Alzheimer Disease & Associated Disorders, 21, 249258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beekly, D. L., Ramos, E. M., van Belle, G., Deitrich, W., Clark, A. D., Jacka, M. E., & Kukull, W. A. (2004). The national Alzheimer’s coordinating center (NACC) database. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord, 18, 270–277.Google Scholar
Besser, L., Kukull, W., Knopman, D. S., Chui, H., Galasko, D., Weintraub, S., Jicha, G., Carlsson, C., Burns, J., Quinn, J., Sweet, R. A., Rascovsky, K., Teylan, M., Beekly, D., Thomas, G., Bollenbeck, M., Monsell, S., Mock, C., Zhou, X. H., …Morris, J. C. (2018). Version 3 of the national Alzheimer’s coordinating center’s uniform data set, Alzheimer Disease & Associated Disorders, 32, 351358.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruen, P. D., McGeown, W. J., Shanks, M. F., & Venneri, A. (2008). Neuroanatomical correlates of neuropsychiatric symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain, 131, 24552463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burhanullah, M. H., Tschanz, J. T., Peters, M. E., Leoutsakos, J.-M., Matyi, J., Lyketsos, C. G., Nowrangi, M. A., & Rosenberg, P. B. (2020). Neuropsychiatric symptoms as risk factors for cognitive decline in clinically normal older adults: The cache county study. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 28, 6471.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cahn-Weiner, D. A., Ready, R. E., & Malloy, P. F. (2003). Neuropsychological predictors of everyday memory and everyday functioning in patients with mild Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 16, 8489.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, M. C., Xue, Q.-L., Zhou, J., & Fried, L. P. (2009). Executive decline and dysfunction precedes declines in memory: The women’s health and aging study II. The Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 64A, 110117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, S., Sultzer, D., Hinkin, C., Mahler, M., & Cummings, J. (1992). Executive dysfunction in Alzheimer’s disease: Association with neuropsychiatric symptoms and functional impairment. JAMA: The Journal of the American Medical Association, 268, 1473.Google Scholar
Cheung, G. W., & Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 9, 233255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cummings, J. (2020). The neuropsychiatric inventory: Development and applications. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 33, 7384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cummings, J. L. (1993). Frontal-subcortical circuits and human behavior. Archives of Neurology, 50, 873880.Google ScholarPubMed
Cummings, J., & Mega, M. S. (2003). Neuropsychiatry and Behavioral Neuroscience. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
David, N. D., Lin, F., & Porsteinsson, A. P. (2016). Trajectories of neuropsychiatric symptoms and cognitive decline in mild cognitive impairment. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 24, 7080.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Lucia, N., Carbone, G., Muzii, B., Ferrara, N., Rengo, G., Maldonato, N. M., Femminella, G. D., & Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (2023). Neuropsychiatric symptoms and their neural correlates in individuals with mild cognitive impairment. International Psychogeriatrics, 35, 623632.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Devanand, D. P., Lee, S., Huey, E. D., & Goldberg, T. E. (2022). Associations between neuropsychiatric symptoms and neuropathological diagnoses of Alzheimer disease and related dementias. JAMA Psychiatry, 79, 359.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diniz, B. S., Butters, M. A., Albert, S. M., Dew, M. A., & Reynolds, C. F. (2013). Late-life depression and risk of vascular dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: systematic review and meta-analysis of community-based cohort studies. British Journal of Psychiatry, 202, 329335.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duarte-Abritta, B., Sánchez, S.-M., Abulafia, C., Gustafson, D. R., Vázquez, S., Sevlever, G., Castro, M. N., Fiorentini, L., Villarreal, M. F., & Guinjoan, S. M. (2021). Amyloid and anatomical correlates of executive functioning in middle-aged offspring of patients with late-onset Alzheimer’s disease. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 316, 111342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edwards, E. R., Spira, A. P., Barnes, D. E., & Yaffe, K. (2009). Neuropsychiatric symptoms in mild cognitive impairment: Differences by subtype and progression to dementia. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 24, 716722.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eikelboom, W. S., van den Berg, E., Singleton, E. H., Baart, S. J., Coesmans, M., Leeuwis, A. E., Teunissen, C. E., van Berckel, B. N. M., Pijnenburg, Y. A. L., Scheltens, P., van der Flier, W. M., Ossenkoppele, R., & Papma, J. M. (2021). Neuropsychiatric and cognitive symptoms across the alzheimer disease clinical spectrum: Cross-sectional and longitudinal associations. Neurology, 97, e1276e1287.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enders, C. K., & Bandalos, D. L. (2001). The relative performance of full information maximum likelihood estimation for missing data in structural equation models. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 8, 430457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farias, S. T., Harrell, E., Neumann, C., & Houtz, A. (2003). The relationship between neuropsychological performance and daily functioning in individuals with alzheimer’s disease: Ecological validity of neuropsychological tests. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 18, 655672.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferrer, E., Balluerka, N., & Widaman, K. F. (2008). Factorial invariance and the specification of second-order latent growth models. Methodology, 4, 2236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Field, A. (2013). Discovering Statistics Using IBM SPSS Statistics (4th ed.). Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Gallagher, D., Fischer, C. E., & Iaboni, A. (2017). Neuropsychiatric symptoms in mild cognitive impairment: An update on prevalence, mechanisms, and clinical significance. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 62, 161169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallo, J. L., Schmidt, K. S., & Libon, D. J. (2008). Behavioral and psychological symptoms, neurocognitive performance, and functional independence in mild dementia. Dementia, 7, 397413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
García-Alberca, J. M., Lara, J. P., Berthier, M. L., Cruz, B., Barbancho, M.Á., Green, C., & González-Barón, S. (2011). Can impairment in memory, language and executive functions predict neuropsychiatric symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease (AD)? Findings from a cross-sectional study. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 52, 264269.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gauthier, L., & Gauthier, S. (1990). Assessment of functional changes in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroepidemiology, 9, 183188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Godefroy, O. (2003). Frontal syndrome and disorders of executive functions. Journal of Neurology, 250, 16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, R. A., Lochner, K. A., Thambisetty, M., Wingo, T., Posner, S. F., & Ling, S. M. (2017). Prevalence of dementia subtypes in U.S. Medicare fee-for-service beneficiaries, 2011–2013. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 13, 2837.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grimm, K. J., Ram, N., & Hamagami, F. (2011). Nonlinear growth curves in developmental research. Child Development, 82, 13571371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guarino, A., Favieri, F., Boncompagni, I., Agostini, F., Cantone, M., & Casagrande, M. (2019). Executive functions in Alzheimer disease: A systematic review. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 10, 1–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gustavson, D. E., Jak, A. J., Elman, J. A., Panizzon, M. S., Franz, C. E., Gifford, K. A., Reynolds, C. A., Toomey, R., Lyons, M. J., Kremen, W. S., & Peter, J. (2021). How well does subjective cognitive decline correspond to objectively measured cognitive decline? Assessment of 10–12 year change. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 83, 291304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harrington, M. G., Chiang, J., Pogoda, J. M., Gomez, M., Thomas, K., Marion, S. D. B., Miller, K. J., Siddarth, P., Yi, X., Zhou, F., Lee, S., Arakaki, X., Cowan, R. P., Tran, T., Charleswell, C., Ross, B. D., Fonteh, A. N., & Ginsberg, S. D. (2013). Executive function changes before memory in preclinical Alzheimer’s pathology: A prospective, cross-sectional, case control study. PLoS ONE, 8, e79378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6, 155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johns, E. K., Phillips, N. A., Belleville, S., Goupil, D., Babins, L., Kelner, N., Ska, B., Gilbert, B., Inglis, G., Panisset, M., De Boysson, C., & Chertkow, H. (2009). Executive functions in frontotemporal dementia and Lewy body dementia. Neuropsychology, 23, 765777.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, J. K., Gross, A. L., Pa, J., McLaren, D. G., Park, L. Q., & Manly, J. J. (2012). Longitudinal change in neuropsychological performance using latent growth models: A study of mild cognitive impairment. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 6, 540550.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jung, Y. H., Park, S., Jang, H., Cho, S. H., Kim, S. J., Kim, J. P., Kim, S. T., Na, D. L., Seo, S. W., & Kim, H. J. (2020). Frontal-executive dysfunction affects dementia conversion in patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Scientific Reports, 10, 772.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufer, D. I., Cummings, J. L., Ketchel, P., Smith, V., MacMillan, A., Shelley, T., Lopez, O. L., & DeKosky, S. T. (2000). Validation of the NPI-Q, a brief clinical form of the neuropsychiatric inventory. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 12, 233239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krell-Roesch, J., Syrjanen, J. A., Machulda, M. M., Christianson, T. J., Kremers, W. K., Mielke, M. M., Knopman, D. S., Petersen, R. C., Vassilaki, M., & Geda, Y. E. (2021). Neuropsychiatric symptoms and the outcome of cognitive trajectories in older adults free of dementia: The mayo clinic study of aging. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 36, 13621369.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kwon, C.-Y., & Lee, B. (2021). Prevalence of behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia in community-dwelling dementia patients: A systematic review. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12, 741059.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leoutsakos, J.-M. S., Forrester, S. N., Lyketsos Constantine, G., & Smith, G. S. (2015). Latent classes of neuropsychiatric symptoms in NACC controls and conversion to mild cognitive impairment or dementia. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 48, 483493.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lezak, M. D. (2012). Neuropsychological assessment (5th ed.) Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Liu, H., & Chang, L. (2010). Multivariate Longitudinal Data Analysis. In International Encyclopedia of Education (pp. 120127). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-08-044894-7.00286-4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lyketsos, C. G., Lopez, O., Jones, B., Fitzpatrick, A. L., Breitner, J., & DeKosky, S. (2002). Prevalence of neuropsychiatric symptoms in dementia and mild cognitive impairment: Results from the cardiovascular health study. JAMA, 288, 1475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lyketsos, C. G., Steinberg, M., Tschanz, J. T., Norton, M. C., Steffens, D. C., & Breitner, J. C. S. (2000). Mental and behavioral disturbances in dementia: Findings from the cache county study on memory in aging. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 708714.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacAulay, R. K., Calamia, M. R., Cohen, A. S., Daigle, K., Foil, H., Brouillette, R., Bruce-Keller, A. J., & Keller, J. N. (2018). Understanding heterogeneity in older adults: Latent growth curve modeling of cognitive functioning. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 40, 292302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, E., & Velayudhan, L. (2020). Neuropsychiatric symptoms in mild cognitive impairment: A literature review. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 49, 146155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McArdle, J. J. (2009). Latent variable modeling of differences and changes with longitudinal data. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 577605.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McArdle, J. J., Grimm, K. J., Hamagami, F., Bowles, R. P., & Meredith, W. (2009). Modeling life-span growth curves of cognition using longitudinal data with multiple samples and changing scales of measurement. Psychological Methods, 14, 126149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McArdle, J. J., Hamgami, F., Jones, K., Jolesz, F., Kikinis, R., Spiro, A., & Albert, M. S. (2004). Structural modeling of dynamic changes in memory and brain structure using longitudinal data from the normative aging study. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 59, P294P304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNeish, D., & Matta, T. (2018). Differentiating between mixed-effects and latent-curve approaches to growth modeling. Behavior Research Methods, 50, 13981414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, J. (1993). The clinical dementia rating (CDR). Neurology, 43, 24122412a.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, J., Weintraub, S., Chui, H., Cummings, J., DeCarli, C., Ferris, S., Foster, N., Galasko, D., Graff-Radford, N., Peskind, E., Beekly, D., Ramos, E., & Kukull, W. (2006). The uniform data set (UDS): Clinical and cognitive variables and descriptive data from Alzheimer disease centers. Alzheimer Disease & Associated Disorders, 20, 210216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mourao, R. J., Mansur, G., Malloy-Diniz, L. F., Castro Costa, E., & Diniz, B. S. (2016). Depressive symptoms increase the risk of progression to dementia in subjects with mild cognitive impairment: Systematic review and meta-analysis: MCI, depression and risk of dementia. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 31, 905911.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nandi, A., Counts, N., Chen, S., Seligman, B., Tortorice, D., Vigo, D., & Bloom, D. E. (2022). Global and regional projections of the economic burden of Alzheimer’s disease and related dementias from 2019 to 2050: A value of statistical life approach. eClinicalMedicine, 51, 101580.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nesselroade, J. R. (1991). The warp and the woof of the developmental fabric. In Visions of aesthetics, the environment & development: The legacy of Joachim F. Wohlwill (pp. 213240). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
Ng, G., Ng, W. Q., & Yang, H. (2023). Executive functions predict the trajectories of rumination in middle-aged and older adults: A latent growth curve analysis. Emotion, 23, 776786.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Office of the Assistant Secretary for Planning and Evaluation. (n.d.). What is Alzheimer’s disease and related dementias. National Alzheimer’s Project Act. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, https://aspe.hhs.gov/collaborations-committees-advisory-groups/napa/what-ad-adrd Google Scholar
Pakzad, S., Ringuette, J., Borque, P., & Sepehry, A. A. (2018). Executive functions and screening for mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimers disease: A cross-sectional study. Acta Psychopathologica, 04, 1–9.Google Scholar
Peters, M. E., Rosenberg, P. B., Steinberg, M., Norton, M. C., Welsh-Bohmer, K. A., Hayden, K. M., Breitner, J., Tschanz, J. T., & Lyketsos, C. G. (2013). Neuropsychiatric symptoms as risk factors for progression from CIND to dementia: The cache county study. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 21, 11161124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, R. C., & Morris, J. C. (2005). Mild cognitive impairment as a clinical entity and treatment target. Archives of Neurology, 62, 1160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramirez-Gomez, L., Zheng, L., Reed, B., Kramer, J., Mungas, D., Zarow, C., Vinters, H., Ringman, J. M., & Chui, H. (2017). Neuropsychological profiles differentiate Alzheimer disease from subcortical ischemic vascular dementia in an autopsy-defined cohort. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 44, 111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Razani, J., Casas, R., Wong, J. T., Lu, P., Alessi, C., & Josephson, K. (2007). Relationship between executive functioning and activities of daily living in patients with relatively mild dementia. Applied Neuropsychology, 14, 208214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. 1993. The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery: Theory and Clinical Interpretation (2nd ed.). Neuropsychology Press.Google Scholar
Roberto, N., Portella, M. J., Marquié, M., Alegret, M., Hernández, I., Mauleón, A., Rosende-Roca, M., Abdelnour, C., de Antonio, E. E., Gil, S., Tartari, J. P., Vargas, L., Espinosa, A., Ortega, G., Pérez-Cordón, A., Sanabria, Ángela, Orellana, A., de Rojas, I., Moreno-Grau, S., …Valero, S. (2021). Neuropsychiatric profiles and conversion to dementia in mild cognitive impairment, a latent class analysis. Scientific Reports, 11, 6448.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roseel (2012). Lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling. Journal of Statistical Software, 48, 136 Google Scholar
Sadak, T. I., Katon, J., Beck, C., Cochrane, B. B., & Borson, S. (2014). Key neuropsychiatric symptoms in common dementias: Prevalence and implications for caregivers, clinicians, and health systems. Research in Gerontological Nursing, 7, 4452.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Staffaroni, A. M., Asken, B. M., Casaletto, K. B., Fonseca, C., You, M., Rosen, H. J., Boxer, A. L., Elahi, F. M., Kornak, J., Mungas, D., & Kramer, J. H. (2021). Development and validation of the uniform data set (v3.0) executive function composite score (UDS3-EF). Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 17, 574583.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suchy, Y. (2015). Executive Functioning: A Comprehensive Guide for Clinical Practice. Oxford University Press Incorporated.Google Scholar
Teng, E., Lu, P. H., & Cummings, J. L. (2007). Neuropsychiatric symptoms are associated with progression from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer’s disease. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 24, 253259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tisak, J., & Meredith, W. (1990). Longitudinal Factor Analysis. In Statistical Methods in Longitudinal Research (pp. 125149). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-724960-5.50009-3,CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilcox, R. R. (1993). Some results on a Winsorized correlation coefficient. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 46, 339349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wright, L. M., Donaghy, P. C., Burn, D. J., Taylor, J.-P., O’Brien, J. T., Yarnall, A. J., Matthews, F. E., Firbank, M. J., Thomas, A. J., & Lawson, R. A. (2023). Predicting cognitive decline using neuropsychiatric symptoms in prodromal Lewy body dementia: A longitudinal study. Parkinsonism & Related Disorders, 113, 105762.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yesavage, J., & Sheikh, J. (1986). 9/Geriatric depression scale (GDS). Clinical Gerontologist, 165173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zainal, N. H., & Newman, M. G. (2023). Elevated anxious and depressed mood relates to future executive dysfunction in older adults: A longitudinal network analysis of psychopathology and cognitive functioning. Clinical Psychological Science, 11, 218238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeng, Y., Wang, J., Cai, X., Zhang, X., Zhang, J., Peng, M., Xiao, D., Ouyang, H., & Yan, F. (2023). Effects of physical activity interventions on executive function in older adults with dementia: A meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Geriatric Nursing, 51, 369377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhao, Q.-F., Tan, L., Wang, H.-F., Jiang, T., Tan, M.-S., Tan, L., Xu, W., Li, J.-Q., Wang, J., Lai, T.-J., & Yu, J.-T. (2016). The prevalence of neuropsychiatric symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 190, 264271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Participant demographics stratified by time point

Figure 1

Table 2. Frequency of neuropsychiatric symptom severity ratings at wave 0 by symptom and overall total

Figure 2

Table 3. Cognitive & neuropsychiatric characteristics stratified by time point for overall sample

Figure 3

Table 4. Measurement invariance analysis results

Figure 4

Table 5. Model implied decline in EF based on baseline NPS severity

Figure 5

Figure 1. Model-implied trajectory of executive function decline by NPS severity. Note: Main Graph: depicts model-implied differences in EF decline by NPS severity group. EF = executive functioning. NPS = neuropsychiatric symptoms. X-axis represents time points that correspond to annual visits (0 = wave 0, 1 = wave 1, 2 = wave 2, 3 = wave 3). Y-axis represents model-implied EF score. Mild NPS group refers to people with NPS at −1.5 standard deviations below the raw NPS mean. Moderate NPS refers to people with NPS at the NPS mean. Severe NPS refers to people with NPS scores at 1.5 standard deviations above the NPS mean. These labels do not correspond to item ratings with similar labels. Inset Graph: depicts the magnitude of the difference in EF decline between groups over time. X-axis represents time points; Y-axis represents expected gap between mild and severe NPS on EF. W0 gap between EF performance between mild NPS and severe NPS = .148 intercept-SDs; W1 gap = .188; W2 gap = .228; W3 gap = .268. The gap in EF performance nearly doubles between mild and severe NPS by W3.

Supplementary material: File

Goodwin et al. supplementary material 1

Goodwin et al. supplementary material
Download Goodwin et al. supplementary material 1(File)
File 5.9 KB
Supplementary material: File

Goodwin et al. supplementary material 2

Goodwin et al. supplementary material
Download Goodwin et al. supplementary material 2(File)
File 623.9 KB